Skip to main content
eScholarship
Open Access Publications from the University of California

About

The annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society is aimed at basic and applied cognitive science research. The conference hosts the latest theories and data from the world's best cognitive science researchers. Each year, in addition to submitted papers, researchers are invited to highlight some aspect of cognitive science.

Workshop

Cognition, Collectives, and Human Culture

Cognitive capacities such as learning, reasoning, anddecision-making are often studied in tasks where a single par-ticipant acts in isolation. Yet humans don’t learn, reason, andmake decisions in a vacuum. Human cognition is distinc-tively social: Much of what we do influences—and is influ-enced by—other people.The goal of this workshop is to bring together diverse per-spectives on the interplay between human cognition and thedynamic, social environments we inhabit. The workshop isorganized around three key themes. Theme 1 lays out thecognitive tools that equip individuals to thrive in social en-vironments, including specialized mechanisms for teachingand learning from others. Theme 2 examines how the socialenvironment is itself shaped by the dynamic interactions be-tween multiple individuals, producing emergent behaviors atthe level of the collective. Finally, Theme 3 explores howhuman cognition responds to the demands of particular so-cial environments, including how cultural variability in socialcognition might emerge across development.Collectively, the research showcased in this workshopenriches this year’s conversation on “How to Develop aMind: Learning in Humans, Animals, and Machines”by highlighting the social and cultural context of learn-ing and development. In addition, our speakers representa broad cross-section of the conference, spanning multi-ple disciplines (computer science, anthropology, psychol-ogy), perspectives (computational, ecological, developmen-tal), and career stages (from research assistants to full pro-fessors). Below, we describe each theme and presentercontributions in detail. To take part in the workshop,visit cognitioncollectivesandculture.github.io forthe current schedule.

Workshop on Scaling Cognitive Science

The proliferation of web-connected devices has presentedsignificant opportunities and challenges to cognitive sci-ence — opportunities in that cognitive scientists can collectdata relevant to human cognition orders of magnitude fasterthan before, addressing questions that were otherwise impos-sible to address; and challenges in that cognitive scientistsrequire new infrastructure to collect these data and new meth-ods to analyze them once collected.This workshop brings together cognitive scientists who areat the forefront of these opportunities and challenges in scal-ing cognitive science, along with cognitive scientists whowould like to be, to engage in a day of interactive exchangeand development of ideas related to scaling cognitive science.The workshop is a full day. Each presentation addresses adifferent opportunity or challenge. One set of presentationshighlights opportunities. A second set highlights challengesof statistical analysis and data collection. In a break-out ses-sion, attendees address these opportunities and challengeshead on.

Choices and Decisions

Limits on Predictability of Risky Choice Behavior

Research in decision-making has recently begun to empha-size predictive accuracy as the dominant principle for design-ing and evaluating choice models. This emphasis has led tothe development of increasingly more precise models of hu-mans’ risk preferences, as measured in certain experimentalparadigms built upon certainty equivalence testing. In thispaper, we argue that the level of precision attained by recentchoice models is unexpected, because human preferences areirreducibly noisy. We support this argument by conducting ex-periments to measure intra-observer consistency in choice be-havior in two common risk preference paradigms: decisionsfrom description and experience. We find that while currentchoice models of decisions from experience align fairly wellwith the upper limits of choice consistency seen in our experi-mental data, choice models for decisions from description aresignificantly more consistent with humans’ choices than thehumans themselves are consistent with their own choices. Wediscuss some theoretical and practical implications of our re-sults.

What determines the learned predictiveness effect?Separating cue-outcome correlation from choice relevance

Evidence from a variety of learning tasks suggests that cuesthat are more predictive of an outcome attract greater attentionand are learned about more effectively in subsequent tasks. Wetested whether this learned predictiveness effect is due to theobjective strength of the cue-outcome association (cue-outcome correlation), or the degree to which the cue isinformative for making the correct choice on each trial (choicerelevance), by manipulating the possible outcome choicesavailable on each trial. Experiment 1 compared two sets of cuesthat were equally (and imperfectly) correlated with outcomesand showed learning biases in favor of the set of cues that hadinitially been more relevant for choices made on each trial.Experiment 2 used a more conventional learned predictivenessdesign in which the cue-outcome correlation was stronger forone set of cues (perfect predictors) than the other set (imperfectpredictors). However, here we manipulated whether or not theimperfect predictors could be used to make a correct choice,and thus whether the imperfect predictors possessed choicerelevance that was equal to or less than the perfect predictors.In this case, we found no evidence that the relevancemanipulation made any difference; learning biases towards theperfect predictor were evident regardless. The results suggestthat both cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance canlead to changes in associability and learning biases; both wereindividually sufficient but neither were necessary.

Exploration Decisions Precede and Improve Explicit Uncertainty Judgments inPreschoolers

We investigate the relationship between exploratory learningand confidence scale judgments in understanding andimproving children’s early recognition of uncertainty. Four-and five-year-olds were presented with stimuli that varied intheir amount of occlusion. We assessed children’s ability todistinguish between these levels of uncertainty using twotypes of measures. Experiment 1 used a traditional 3-pointconfidence scale to examine explicit uncertainty judgments.Experiment 2 examined exploration preference as an implicitmeasure of uncertainty using the same stimuli. We comparedchildren’s performance on these two tasks before and aftertheir experience of disconfirming evidence, to assess theimpact of surprising events on the recognition of uncertainty.Results indicate that children intuitively recognize gaps intheir knowledge and express this in their exploratory behaviorbefore they are able to spontaneously produce accurateconfidence judgments. We also find that this implicitrecognition of uncertainty may be leveraged to support andimprove explicit judgments, even without extensive training.

Prospect Theory and Optimal Risky Choices with Goals

Decision making under risk is often studied as a preferential choice governed by stable individual personality characteristics, but risky choice can also be viewed as a dynamic problem of resource accumulation to survive. When decision makers aim to reach a particular goal in limited time, such as “earn at least $100 in five choices,” risky choice becomes a non-trivial planning problem. This problem has an optimal solution that can differ from immediate expected-value maximization. We studied the optimality of risky choices under such minimum goal requirements experimentally and find that the observed choices under goals approximate the optimal solution. However, because the optimal model is very complex, we examine if simpler models can predict people’s choices better. We test an extended version of prospect theory, assuming a dynamic reference point that depends on the distance to the goal. This “dynamic prospect theory” was better than the alternative model in describing people’s decisions (i.e., for 63% of the participants, it was the best model). Our findings show that humans can excel in a highly complex, dynamic, risky choice problem and that a dynamic version of prospect theory provides one possible explanation for how people decide under risk when long-term goals matter.

Toward Unifying Cognitive Architecture and Neural Task Set Theories

PRIMs theory describes a computational foundation for un-derstanding task-general human learning and transfer usingrule-based cognitive architectures. Integration with ACT-Rhas yielded Actransfer, a model that replicates human learn-ing and transfer across many tasks. However, this model re-quires task-specific latency scaling parameters from ACT-Rto model different tasks, implying that there is missing com-putation in the theory. Neuroscience literature has separatelydefined the “task set” as the neural encoding that configuresstimulus-response rule behavior in working memory. The pro-cess of switching between different task sets is often used toexplain human latency costs. This paper introduces an alter-nate instantiation of PRIMs theory that enacts task set process-ing to account for the missing computation via a novel memorystructure called a procedure context. Human tasks of varyingcomplexity are modeled across two experiments. Procedurecontexts model human latencies and interference effects in alltasks by integrating latency, decision making, task representa-tion, and learning as aspects of a single unified process. Thisapproach offers promise for future modeling within cognitivescience by uniting theories from neuroscience and cognitivearchitectures.

Neuroscience and Psychophysics

Adaptations of Executive Function and Prefrontal Cortex Connectivity FollowingExergame Play in 4- to 5-year old Children

This study examined the separate and combined effects ofexercise and cognitive training on children’s executivefunction (EF) and associated neural substrates. Sixty-twochildren were recruited and randomly assigned to an Exergame(exercise + cognitive activity), Exercise (physical activity),Sedentary (cognitive activity), or Control (no-play) Condition.The training consisted of 20 min sessions 2x/week and wascompleted by 49 children 4- to 5-years-old. Resting-stateprefrontal cortex (PFC) connectivity utilizing functional near-infrared spectroscopy, behavioral assessments of EF, andteacher ratings of EF were assessed pre- and posttest.Exergame training significantly improved performance ontransfer EF assessments compared to the other conditions andincreased PFC connectivity. The changes in PFC connectivitywere positively associated with EF improvement. Thesefindings suggest that the combination of cognitive and exercisetraining modulates the effects on EF and elucidates the neuralmechanisms underlying the changes in EF induced fromexergame play.

Dissociable systems for recognizing places and navigating through them:neuropsychological and developmental evidence

Recent neuroimaging evidence suggests that scene processing depends on dissociable systems for visually-guided navi-gation (including the occipital place area, OPA) and scene categorization (including the parahippocampal place area). Ifthese systems are truly dissociable, then it should be possible to find cases in which one system is impaired, while theother is spared. Further, if dissociable, then these systems may develop independently. Here we tested visually-guidednavigation and scene categorization abilities in 36 adults with Williams syndrome (WS) a developmental disorder in-volving cortical thinning in and around the OPA as well as 82 typically developing 4-8 year old children. We foundthat i) WS adults are impaired in visually-guided navigation, but not scene categorization, relative to mental-age matchedchildren; and ii) visually-guided navigation matures later in typical development than scene categorization. These findingsprovide neuropsychological and developmental evidence for dissociable scene processing systems for recognizing placesand navigating through them.

Motor interference changes meaning

What role does the motor system play in language understanding? Here we show that effector-specific motor interferencecan change how people interpret language about actions. An action like voting can be understood in terms of its concretedetails (writing marks on a ballot) or its abstract significance (influencing an election). If neural circuits for performingmotor actions enable people to mentally represent an actions concrete details, then occupying these circuits with a sec-ondary motor task should make the actions details harder to represent. Consistent with this hypothesis, in two experiments(N=180), tapping a complex rhythm with either the hands or the feet increased the proportion of abstract interpretations ofphrases describing actions with the same effector. Thus, meaningless motor activity causes qualitative changes in languagecomprehension: Performing different actions can lead to different understandings of the same words and phrases.

The “cognitive speed-bump”:How world champion Tetris players trade milliseconds for seconds

Tetris is a fast-paced puzzle solving game that requires play-ers to rapidly maneuver falling blocks to clear rows and scorepoints. Skilled Tetris players learn to execute moves in thegame very quickly to keep up with the increasing time pres-sure. But world champion Tetris players employ more complexstrategies that save precious milliseconds that enable them toreach even higher levels of play. Such strategies show mas-tery of the game’s event structure, but also come with a startupcost— a “cognitive speed bump”— wherein they must mo-mentarily decide whether to rotate a block left or right, evenfor scenarios where the distinction is not meaningful for per-formance. We present data showing both the world champions’superior overall action times, but also a preliminary “speedbump” that is consistent both within and between world cham-pion players. Potential underlying memory structures are ex-plored, and implications are discussed for both the Soft Con-straints Hypothesis and the relationship between Hick’s Lawand expertise.

Human Learning

Explanation Supports Hypothesis Generation in Learning

A large body of research has shown that engaging in explanation improves learning across a range of tasks. The act of explaining has been proposed to draw attention and cognitive resources toward evidence that will support a good explanation—information that is broad, abstract, and consistent with prior knowledge—which in turn aids discovery and generalization. However, it remains unclear whether explanation acts on the learning process via improved hypothesis generation, increasing the probability that the correct hypothesis is considered in the first place, or hypothesis evaluation, the appraisal of the correct hypothesis in light of evidence. In the present study, we address this question by separating the hypothesis generation and evaluation processes in a novel category learning task and quantifying the effect of explanation on each process independently. We find that explanation supports the generation of broad and abstract hypotheses but has less effect on the evaluation of hypotheses.

Analogical Transfer and Recognition Memory in Relational Classification Learning

People spontaneously make connections between superficially distinct domains through relational similarity, but this spontaneous transfer has yet to be demonstrated across distinct classification tasks. A related issue is that the acquisition of a category may affect recognition memory for category- consistent items. Participants in the Category Learning condition completed an initial classification task. The Category Learning and Baseline conditions each received category- consistent items to study followed by a recognition test. Both groups completed a final classification task in a novel domain abiding by the same underlying category structures as the initial classification task. The Category Learning group showed 1) increased false alarms during the recognition test and 2) higher accuracy in the final classification task (when told the classification phases were unrelated). This suggests that classification learning led to a schematization of the category-defining concept (evidenced by increased false alarms), which supported spontaneous transfer of relational concepts across distinct classification tasks.

Learning to build physical structures better over time

Our ability to plan and build a wide array of physicalstructures, from sand castles to skyscrapers, is a definingfeature of modern human intelligence. What cognitive toolsenable us to create such complex and varied structures?Here we investigate how practice “reverse-engineering” a setof physical structures impacts the procedures that peoplesubsequently use to build those structures, as well as how wellthey build them over time. Participants (N=105) viewed 2Dsilhouettes of 8 unique block towers in a virtual environmentsimulating rigid-body physics, and aimed to reconstruct eachone in less than 60 seconds. We found that people learnto build each tower more accurately and quickly acrossrepeated attempts, and that these gains reflect both group-levelconvergence upon a smaller set of viable policies, as wellas error-dependent updating of each individual’s strategies.Taken together, our study provides novel insight into howhumans learn from prior experience to discover better solutionsto physical reasoning problems over time.

Effects of “chained” study on spontaneous relational discovery

Prior knowledge of relational structure allows people toquickly make sense of and respond to new experiences. Whenawareness of such structure is not necessary to support learn-ing, however, it is unclear when and why individuals “spon-taneously discover” an underlying relational schema. Thepresent study examines the determinants of such discovery indiscrimination-based transitive inference (TI), whereby peo-ple learn about a hierarchy of interrelated premises and aretested on their ability to draw inferences that bridge studiedassociations. Experiencing “chained” sequences of overlap-ping premises during training was predicted to facilitate thediscovery of relational structure. Among individuals withoutprior knowledge of the hierarchy, chaining improved relationallearning and was most likely to result in explicit awareness ofthe underlying relations between items. These findings addto growing evidence that the temporal dynamics of training,including successive presentation of overlapping associations,are key to understanding spontaneous relational discovery dur-ing learning.

How a-priori biases affect sequence learning in a serial reaction time task

The ability to chain together sequences of information and action is pivotal to everyday acquisition of skills. Despite extensive research of sequence learning, little focus has been given to individual performance in standard tasks measuring this capability. As a result, little is known regarding what knowledge participants gain during such tasks. In the current work, an individual- and item-based analysis is performed of eye movements that occur during a spatial sequence learning task and reflect anticipation of upcoming target locations. We show that the knowledge participants acquire during the task is tightly linked to a-priori response biases they bring into the experiment. Results suggest that a-priori biases may be a sizeable influence on performance in learning experiments, that tends to be overlooked. Implications for designing and reading studies of sequence learning are discussed.

Semantics

Systematicity in a Recurrent Neural Network by Factorizing Syntax andSemantics

Standard methods in deep learning fail to capture composi-tional or systematic structure in their training data, as shownby their inability to generalize outside of the training distribu-tion. However, human learners readily generalize in this way,e.g. by applying known grammatical rules to novel words. Theinductive biases that might underlie this powerful cognitive ca-pacity remain unclear. Inspired by work in cognitive sciencesuggesting a functional distinction between systems for syn-tactic and semantic processing, we implement a modificationto an existing deep learning architecture, imposing an analo-gous separation. The resulting architecture substantially out-performs standard recurrent networks on the SCAN dataset, acompositional generalization task, without any additional su-pervision. Our work suggests that separating syntactic fromsemantic learning may be a useful heuristic for capturing com-positional structure, and highlights the potential of using cog-nitive principles to inform inductive biases in deep learning.

Distinguishing Fact from Opinion: Effects of Linguistic Packaging

During language comprehension, what guides how wedistinguish between objective facts and subjective opinions?Our three experiments investigate whether people’s ability todetect subjective content – which we indicated by means ofopinion-conveying adjectives (e.g. amazing, frustrating) – ismodulated by the adjective’s structural position. Our resultsindicate that altering the linguistic structure of a sentenceinfluences our perception of how subjective it is: Even whenthe basic information being conveyed is held constant,packaging this information in different ways elicits differentlevels of perceived subjectivity. When a subjective adjectiveoccurs in a structural position associated with newinformation, the text is rated as more subjective compared to atext that conveys the same basic information but has the sameadjective in a position associated with already-knowninformation. This suggests that the difference between factand opinion, or at least our ability to recognize opinion-basedinformation, can be distorted by linguistic packaging.

Symmetric alternatives and semantic uncertainty modulate scalar inference

Scalar inferences are commonly assumed to involve both lit-eral semantic interpretation and social cognitive reasoning.However, the precise way to characterize listeners’ represen-tation of context - including the space of possible utterance al-ternatives as well as the space of possible conventional mean-ings associated with linguistic forms - is a matter of ongoingdebate. We report a partial replication of a scalar inferencepriming study by Rees and Bott (2018), introducing a novelbaseline condition against which to compare behavior acrossdifferent priming treatments. We also investigate the effectof raising participants’ awareness of communicatively strongeralternatives that explicitly encode an exhaustive meaning (e.g.some but not all with respect to some). Our results suggestthat exhaustive alternatives (which are ‘symmetric’ to canoni-cal alternatives) can modulate the availability and strength ofscalar inferences, and that semantic uncertainty is an indepen-dent channel through which scalar inferences are modulated.We discuss implications for theories of pragmatic competence.

Simple Mechanisms, Rich Structure: Statistical Co-Occurrence Regularities in Language Shape the Development of Semantic Knowledge

Many hallmarks of human intelligence including language, reasoning, and planning require us to draw upon knowledge about the world in which concepts, denoted by words, are organized by meaningful, semantic links between them (e.g., juicy-apple-pear). The goal of the present research was to investigate how these organized semantic networks may emerge in development from simple but powerful mechanisms sensitive to statistical co- occurrence regularities of word use in language. Specifically, we tested whether a mechanistic account of how co-occurrence regularities shape semantic development accurately predicts how semantic organization changes with development. Using a sensitive, gaze-based measure of the semantic links organizing knowledge in children and adults, we observed that developmental changes in semantic organization were consistent with a key role for statistical co-occurrence regularities.

Becoming Organized: How Simple Learning Mechanisms may Shape theDevelopment of Rich Semantic Knowledge

With development, we acquire rich body of knowledge aboutthe world in which concepts denoted by words (e.g., juicy,apple, and pear) are connected by meaningful, semantic links(e.g., apples and pears are similar, and can both be juicy). Onepotentially powerful driver of this development is sensitivity toregularities with which words co-occur in language.Specifically, language is rich regularities that can support: (1)Associative semantic links between words that directly co-occur together (e.g., juicy-apple), and (2) Taxonomic semanticlinks between words similar in meaning that share patterns ofdirect co-occurrence (e.g., apple and pear both co-occur withjuicy). Here, we investigated the development of abilities toform semantic links from these regularities. Results revealedthat both children and adults formed direct co-occurrence-based links, whereas only adults formed shared co-occurrencebased links. We discuss how these results may provide keyinsight into how semantic organization develops.

Modeling Language

A Model of Temporal Connective Acquisition

Temporal connectives are function words that relate events intime. Despite their ubiquity and utility, children acquire themeanings of temporal connective words late in development.Experimental work has uncovered patterns in the acquisitionof temporal connectives that clarify the learning challenge thatthese words pose to children. In particular, developmentalstudies have identified differing acquisition trajectories acrossconnective types, asymmetries in learning within pairs of re-lated connectives, and monotonic increases in comprehensionwith age. Expanding on prior theoretical accounts, we formal-ize temporal connective acquisition in a computational wordlearning framework. We demonstrate that each of the empir-ically determined acquisition patterns emerges in the learningbehavior of our computational model. Finally, we discuss ourfindings in relation to earlier theories and to general learnabil-ity concerns in language acquisition.

Towards a Complete Model of Reading:Simulating Lexical Decision, Word Naming, and Sentence Reading with Über-Reader

This paper presents simulations of eye movements during read-ing, lexical decision, and naming using Über-Reader, a newcomputational model that aims to provide a complete accountof the perceptual, cognitive, and motor processes involved inreading. The present simulations focused on Über-Reader’sword-identification module—an implementation of the Multi-ple-Trace Memory model (Ans et al., 1998) based on the theo-retical assumptions of the MINERVA 2 model of episodicmemory (Hintzman, 1984)—with a vocabulary comprising thefull corpus of the English Lexicon Project (Balota et al., 2007).The model’s lexicon was probed with words and one-letter-dif-ferent non-words from the Schilling et al. (1998) corpus, andoutputs of the model were scored to evaluate performanceagainst the empirical data. The outcomes of these simulationswill inform further development of Über-Reader by providingthe foundation for our ultimate goal of simulating reading, inits entirety.

Interactions of length and overlapin the TRACE model of spoken word recognition

What determines degree of competition among phonologicallysimilar words? One proposal is that proportion of overlappredicts competition independently of word length. We arguethat proportion of overlap may provide descriptive adequacy,but does not provide an explanation. We show that TRACEcorrectly predicts patterns previously attributed to proportionof overlap. In additional simulations, with independentmanipulations of word length and proportion of overlap,proportion of overlap fails to predict the full pattern of results.We discuss how competition dynamics in TRACE modulatecompetition as word length and proportion of overlap change.These results have implications for theories of human spokenword recognition, and will motivate experiments to test thesenew TRACE predictions.

A Computational Analysis of the Constraints on Parallel Word Identification

The debate about how attention is allocated during readinghas been framed in as: Either attention is allocated in a strictlyserial manner, to support the identification of one word at atime, or it is allocated as a gradient, to support the concurrentprocessing of multiple words. The first part of this article re-views reading models to examine the feasibility of both posi-tions. Although word-identification and sentence-processingmodels assume that words are identified serially to incremen-tally build larger units of representation, discourse-processingmodel allow several propositions to be co-active in workingmemory. The remainder of this article then describes an in-stance-based model of word identification, Über-Reader, andsimulations comparing the identification of single words andword pairs. These simulations indicate that, although wordpairs can be identified, accurate identification is restricted toshort high-frequency words due to the computational de-mands of both memory retrieval and limited visual acuity.

Corrective Processes in Modeling Reference Resolution

Reference resolution is one of the core components of language understanding. In spite of its centrality, psychological evidence has shown that the reference resolution process is prone to errors and egocentric bias. In this work, we propose an extension to Analogical Reference Resolution, a computational model based on analogical retrieval, which accounts for such errors. We test the extended model on a study by Epley et al. (2004) and replicate human patterns of bias and correction.

Memory

Forming Concepts of Mozart and Homer Using Short-Term and Long-TermMemory:A Computational Model Based on Chunking

A fundamental issue in cognitive science concerns the mentalprocesses that underlie the formation and retrieval of conceptsin the short-term and long-term memory (STM and LTMrespectively). This study advances Chunking Theory and itscomputational embodiment CHREST to propose a singlemodel that accounts for significant aspects of conceptformation in the domains of literature and music. The proposedmodel inherits CHREST’s architecture with its integratedSTM/LTM stores, while also adding a moving attentionwindow and an “LTM chunk activation” mechanism. Theseadditions address the overly destructive nature of primacyeffect in discrimination network based architectures andexpand Chunking Theory to account for learning, retrieval andcategorisation of complex sequential symbolic patterns – likereal-life text and written music scores. The model was trainedthrough exposure to labelled stimuli and learned to categoriseclassical poets/writers and composers. The model categorisedpreviously unseen literature pieces by Homer, Chaucer,Shakespeare, Walter Scott, Dickens and Joyce, as well asunseen sheet music scores by Bach, Mozart, Beethoven andChopin. These findings offer further support to mechanismsproposed by Chunking Theory and expand it into thepsychology of music.

Using Emails to Quantify the Impact of Prior Exposure on Word Recognition Memory

Recognition memory studies have reliably demonstrated the word frequency effect (WFE), where low-frequency words are more accurately recognized than high-frequency words. The context noise account of WFE argues that pre-experimental exposure to stimuli generates interference that compromises high-frequency words more than low-frequency words. Because the representations of the contexts associated with more recent exposures are assumed to overlap more with the representation of the study context, stimuli that have been seen more recently are thought to generate the most interference. We asked participants to log their daily email for two months. Based on the participant’s email corpus, we constructed an individualized study-test recognition memory task to investigate the effect of recency. Results show that recency has a graded effect on recognition memory that extends for at least two months providing support for the context noise account.

Proceduralization and Working Memory in Association Learning

Humans are highly variable in their ability to learn and executecomplex tasks; however, there are conflicting theories on skillacquisition. This study compared two different explanationsfor how association learning interacts with other cognitiveprocesses: a) reinforcement learning and working memory areseparate, competing processes operating simultaneously onassociation learning; and, b) associations are proceduralizedinto production rules and reinforcement learning acts on thoserules. Participants completed a simple association learning taskfollowed by a delayed test under two conditions designed tocontrast these theories. The results are consistent with aproceduralization account in which reinforcement learning andworking memory are not competitive interfering systems, butthere remain important questions about how these two accountsmay be best integrated.

Understanding Memory for WHERE using Smartphone Data

A primary challenge for alibi generation research is establishing the ground truth of the real world events of interest.We used a smartphone app to record data on participants for a month prior to a memory test. The app captured theiraccelerometry continuously and their GPS location and sound environment every ten minutes. After a week retentioninterval, we asked participants to identify where they were at a given time from among four alternatives. Participants wereincorrect 36% of the time. Furthermore, our forced choice procedure allowed us to conduct a conditional logit analysisto assess the relative importance of different aspects of the events to the decision process. We found strong evidence thatparticipants confuse days across weeks. In addition, people often confused weeks in general and also hours across days.Similarity of location induced more errors than similarity of sound environments or movement types.

Prefrontal-striatal circuitry supportsadaptive memory prioritization across development

Previous work has revealed that the ability to strategically en-code high-value information may improve gradually over de-velopment as cognitive control mechanisms mature. However,studies of value-directed memory have relied on explicit cuesof information value, which are rarely present in real-worldcontexts. Here, using a novel fMRI paradigm, we examinedwhether individuals across a wide age range (N = 90; ages 8– 25 years) could learn the value of information from expe-rience and use learned value signals to strategically modulatememory. We found that memory prioritization for high-valueinformation improved across development, and was supportedby increased engagement of the caudate and prefrontal cortexduring both encoding and retrieval of high-value information.Our results suggest that across development, the dynamic ad-justment of memory based on the statistics of the environmentis supported by a wide network of brain regions involved inboth the recognition and use of information value.

Gender and Individuals

Paradoxical Gender Gaps in Mathematics Achievement: Pressure as a key

Two studies explore gender gaps that favor girls in low-stakeslearning contexts yet are not evident in high-stakesachievement measures. Study 1 (n = 386) combined controldata across multiple experiments testing student’s learningfrom a challenging proportional reasoning lesson to exploreconsistent gender gaps in favor of girls. This learning gap couldnot be explained by the baseline mathematics, affective,motivational, or Executive Function individual differences wemeasured. In Study 2 (n =178), we experimentally manipulatedpressure, raising the stakes by telling some students that theirperformance would determine whether or not their entire classreceived an incentive. Gender gaps in favor of girls remainedin the absence of pressure, but when external pressure wasimposed before or after learning, the female advantagedisappeared. These data suggest managing feelings of pressurein learning or testing contexts may be an important step inultimately increasing female representation in math-intensivefields.

Gender convergence in the expressions of love: A computational analysis of lyrics

Love is a central theme in modern music, but do women andmen differ in their expressions of love? Results from empiri-cal studies on gender differences in love attitudes have evolvedfrom showing consistent differences to more similarities overtime and witnessed gender convergence in relationship expec-tations, housework responsibilities, and sexual attitudes. Inde-pendently, pop culture studies have shown how music can beused as a contextual artifact whose lyrics can reflect a culture’schanging psychological processes and ideologies. We combinethese two research areas to explore whether the gender con-vergence reported in psychological studies is mirrored in lovesongs. Using a corpus of lyrics and song metadata from 1960to 2009, we present a computational analysis of the lexical dis-tribution of lyrics across genre, gender and time. We show thatlove songs between vocalists who are men vs. women havebecome significantly more similar in their lyrical expressionsof love.

Embodiment and gender interact in alignment to TTS voices

The current study tests subjects’ vocal alignment toward femaleand male text-to-speech (TTS) voices presented via threesystems: Amazon Echo, Nao, and Furhat. These systems vary intheir physical form, ranging from a cylindrical speaker (Echo), toa small robot (Nao), to a human-like robot bust (Furhat). We testwhether this cline of personification (cylinder < mini robot

Preschoolers use minimal statistical information about social groups to infer thepreferences and group membership of individuals

We don’t learn about each person we meet from scratch: Ourknowledge of social groups (e.g., cognitive scientists) shapesour expectations about new individuals (e.g., the reader). Herewe explore how 4- and 5-year-old children and adults use min-imal statistical evidence about social groups to support induc-tive inferences about individuals. Overall, we find that bothchildren and adults readily infer the preferences and groupmembership of new individuals when they have appropriateevidence to support these inferences. However, our resultsalso suggest that children and adults interpret this informa-tion in different ways. Adults’ responses align closely witha Bayesian model that assumes that each group’s preferencesare independent of one another. By contrast, we find prelimi-nary evidence that children’s inferences about the preferencesof new group members are sensitive to the composition (Exper-iment 1) and size (Experiment 2) of the opposing group. Ourwork provides insights into how people form structured, gen-eralizable representations of social groups from sparse data.

Symposium

Toward a Unified Theory of Proportion

Proportional reasoning is a ubiquitous part of the humanexperience. We engage in proportional reasoning to meetboth informal and specialized goals across a range ofdomains, such as medicine (e.g., disease rates, drug dosages),finance and commerce (e.g., interest rates, discounts),cooking and baking (e.g., scaling ingredient amounts), andmany others. Given this variation in usage, it may not besurprising that proportional reasoning does not have asingular definition or interpretation, but instead is a complextopic with many interconnected concepts. The central goal ofthis symposium is to shed light on this complexity bydiscussing diverse perspectives of proportional reasoning.

Language and Groups

Influence of partner behaviour on overspecification

Speakers often overspecify by using colour adjectives redun-dantly in referential communication. We investigated whetherthis tendency to overspecify is influenced by a partner’s lin-guistic behaviour, and whether the effect is enhanced by lex-ical repetition and semantic relatedness. We used a director-matcher task in which speakers interacted with either a consis-tently overspecific or a consistently optimal partner. Our re-sults show that partner behaviour influences overspecification.An analysis over time indicates that speakers tended to over-specify at the outset, but reduced this behaviour over interac-tion with an optimal partner much more than with an overspe-cific partner. This may suggest that overspecification (at leastwith colour modifiers) is the “default” behaviour, with speak-ers adapting to optimality in a partner’s linguistic behaviour.

Generalizing meanings from partners to populations:Hierarchical inference supports convention formation on networks

A key property of linguistic conventions is that they hold overan entire community of speakers, allowing us to communicateefficiently even with people we have never met before. Atthe same time, much of our language use is partner-specific:we know that words may be understood differently by differ-ent people based on our shared history. This poses a chal-lenge for accounts of convention formation. Exactly how doagents make the inferential leap to community-wide expecta-tions while maintaining partner-specific knowledge? We pro-pose a hierarchical Bayesian model to explain how speakersand listeners solve this inductive problem. To evaluate ourmodel’s predictions, we conducted an experiment where par-ticipants played an extended natural-language communicationgame with different partners in a small community. We ex-amine several measures of generalization and find key signa-tures of both partner-specificity and community convergencethat distinguish our model from alternatives. These resultssuggest that partner-specificity is not only compatible with theformation of community-wide conventions, but may facilitateit when coupled with a powerful inductive mechanism.

Probabilistic weighting of perspectives in dyadic communication

In successful communication, speakers tailor their language tothe context and listeners make inferences about the speaker’sknowledge. Several current accounts propose that both speak-ers and listeners accomplish this by rational analysis of thestatistics in the environment, including their partner. Here weexamine perspective-taking behaviour in a dyadic conversationtask, where the same individuals act in the role of both speakerand listener. We model perspective-taking in both productionand comprehension, taking into account the dyadic situation.Our findings suggest that conversational partners weight theirown perspective more than the partner’s when speaking, andthe partner’s perspective more than their own when listening.We also find that in both production and comprehension, con-versational partners change the weighting of perspectives overtime, moving towards relying more on the partner’s perspec-tive at the expense of their own perspective. Surprisingly, wefind little evidence that listeners or speakers adapt to the id-iosyncratic statistics of their partner’s linguistic behaviour.

Production Expectations Modulate Contrastive Inference

Contrastive inferences, whereby a listener pragmatically infersa speaker’s referential intention of a partial referring expres-sion like the yellow by reasoning about other objects in thecontext, are notoriously unstable. We report a production-centric model of interpretation couched within the RationalSpeech Act framework. Adjective production probabilities alistener expects for objects in a context drive the size of con-trastive inferences: the greater the asymmetry in expectationfor a speaker to use a pre-nominal adjective for the target ratherthan for competitors, the greater the listener’s resulting targetpreference. Modifier production probabilities were collected(Exp. 1) and used to make predictions about comprehensionin an incremental decision task (Exp. 2). The model’s inter-pretation predictions are supported by the data. This accounthas the potential to explain the fluctuating appearance of con-trastive inferences and shifts the explanatory focus away fromcontrastive inference towards online interpretation of referringexpressions more broadly.

Does informational independence always matter? Children believe small group discussion is more accurate than ten times as many independent informants

Learners faced with competing statements that each have support from multiple sources must decide whom to trust. Lacking firsthand knowledge, they frequently trust the majority. Yet, majorities can be misleading if most members are relying on hearsay from just a few members with firsthand knowledge. Thus, past work has emphasized the importance of informational independence when deciding whom to trust, showing that children and adults do consider informational independence important in certain contexts. However, because informational independence precludes group deliberation, we ask whether children make the reverse inference and devalue informational independence when facing a problem that could benefit from deliberation. In two studies, children and adults ignore informational independence when attempting to answer abstract reasoning questions. However, for a question type for which deliberative reasoning would be of doubtful benefit, children and adults seek advice from multiple independent sources rather than a deliberative group.

Judgement and Decision Making

Do you want to know a secret? The role of valence and delay in early informationpreference

People tend to place value on information even when it doesnot affect the outcome of a decision. Two competingaccounts offer explanations for such non-instrumentalinformation seeking. One account foregrounds the role ofanticipation and the other focusses on uncertainty aversion.Both accounts make similar predictions for short cue-outcome delays and when outcomes are positively valenced,but they differ in their explanation of information preferenceat long delays with negative outcomes. We present a seriesof experiments involving both primary and secondaryreinforcers that pit these accounts against each other. Theresults indicate a consistent preference for non-instrumentalinformation even at long cue-outcome delays and noevidence for information avoidance with negative outcomes.This pattern appears to provide more support for theuncertainty-aversion account than one based on anticipation.

Knowledge Representations in Health Judgments

In the present paper, we introduce a novel computationalapproach for uncovering mental representations underlyinghealthiness judgments for food items. Using semantic vec-tor representations derived from large-scale natural languagedata, we quantify the complex representations that people holdabout foods, and use these representations to predict how bothlay decision makers and experts (trained dietitians) judge thehealthiness of food items. We also successfully predict theimpact of behavioral interventions (e.g. the provision of nutri-ent content information or “traffic-light labels”) on healthinessjudgments for food items. Our models are highly general, andare capable of making predictions for nearly any food item.Finally, these models outperform competing models based onfactual nutritional content, suggesting that health judgmentsdepend more on complex (semantic) knowledge representa-tions than on quantified nutritional information. The results inthis paper illustrate how methods from cognitive science andcomputational linguistics can be combined with existing theo-ries in psychology, to better predict, understand, and influencehealth behavior.

Learning what is relevant for rewards via value-based serial hypothesis testing

Learning what is relevant for reward is a ubiquitous and crucialtask in daily life, where stochastic reward outcomes can de-pend on an unknown number of task dimensions. We designeda paradigm tailored to study such complex scenarios. In the ex-periment, participants configured three-dimensional stimuli byselecting features for each dimension and received probabilis-tic feedback. Participants selected more rewarding featuresover time, demonstrating learning. To investigate the learningprocess, we tested two learning strategies, feature-based rein-forcement learning and serial hypothesis testing, and found ev-idence for both. The extent to which each strategy was engageddepended on the instructed task complexity: when instructedthat there were fewer relevant dimensions (and therefore fewerreward-generating rules were possible) people tended to seri-ally test hypotheses, whereas they relied more on learning fea-ture values when more dimensions were relevant. To explainthe behavioral dependency on task complexity and instruc-tions, we tested variants of the value-based serial hypothesistesting model. We found evidence that participants constructedtheir hypothesis space based on the instructed task condition,but they failed to use all the information provided (e.g. rewardprobabilities). Our current best model can qualitatively capturethe difference in choice behavior and performance across taskconditions.

Facets of Cognition

The Differential Relationship of Extracurricular Activities and Screen Time with Adolescents’ Fluid and Crystallized Cognition

Adolescents are going through a period of rapid growth in cognitive resources, both in crystallized, or knowledge-based, cognition and fluid cognition, or the ability to think and reason flexibly. Past literature reveals an ongoing debate as to whether, or in what way, different activities during childhood relate to these abilities. The current study leveraged the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development baseline dataset to explore the interplay between nine- and ten-year-olds’ extracurricular activities, screen time, and the different components of cognition. Results indicate that adolescents’ activities explain more variance in crystallized than fluid cognition. Further, participation in artistic activities is associated with increased fluid and crystallized cognition, though sports is positively associated with fluid but negatively associated with crystallized cognition. Different types of screen time, though notably not video game playing, may be negatively associated with cognition. Screen time explains more variance in fluid cognition than extracurricular activities do, whereas the opposite is true of crystallized cognition. This correlational study suggests potential avenues for further work to disentangle the causal links underlying the relationships between experiences and cognition. Do such activities change adolescents’ cognitive skill, or do children self-select to participate in certain types of activities that complement their existing skills?

Quantifying Curiosity: A Formal Approach to Dissociating Causes of Curiosity

Curiosity motivates exploration and is beneficial for learning,but curiosity is not always experienced when facing theunknown. In the present research, we address this selectivity:what causes curiosity to be experienced under somecircumstances but not others? Using a Bayesian reinforcementlearning model, we disentangle four possible influences oncuriosity that have typically been confounded in previousresearch: surprise, local uncertainty/expected informationgain, global uncertainty, and global expected informationgain. In two experiments, we find that backward-lookinginfluences (concerning beliefs based on prior experience) andforward-looking influences (concerning expectations aboutfuture learning) independently predict reported curiosity, andthat forward-looking influences explain the most variance.These findings begin to disentangle the complexenvironmental features that drive curiosity.

Explaining the Existential: Functional Roles of Scientific and Religious Explanation

Questions about the origins of life and the universe seem to call out for explanation, with science and religion offering candidate answers. These answers clearly differ in content, but do they also differ in psychological function? In Study 1 (N=501) participants on Amazon Mechanical Turk rated scientific and religious answers to existential questions on dimensions related to epistemic functions (e.g., “This explanation is based on evidence”) as well as moral/social/emotional functions (e.g., “If everyone believed this, the world would be a more moral place”; “This explanation is comforting”). For non-religious participants, only scientific explanations were assigned high values along epistemic dimensions; For religious participants, only religious explanations were assigned high values along non-epistemic dimensions. In Study 2 (N=130), priming a non-epistemic need boosted religious participants’ evaluation of the quality of religious (vs. scientific) explanations. These findings shed light on the functions of scientific and religious cognition and raise new questions about explanatory co-existence and the origins of religious belief.

The latent factor structure of developmental change in early childhood

Piaget proposed that development proceeded in stages; morerecently researchers have proposed modular theories in whichdifferent abilities develop on their own timetable. Despite theabundance of theory, there is little empirical work on the struc-ture of developmental changes in early childhood. We inves-tigate this question using a large dataset of parent-reporteddevelopmental milestones. We compare a variety of factor-analytic item response theory models and find that variationin development from birth to 55 months of age is best de-scribed by a model with three distinct dimensions. We alsofind evidence that dimensionality increases across age, withthe youngest children described by a two-factor model. Theseresults provide a model-based method for linking holistic de-scriptions of early development to basic theoretical questionsabout the nature of change in childhood.

Examining Sustained Attention in Child-Parent Interaction: A Comparative Studyof Typically Developing Children and Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Sustained attention (SA) is a critical skill in which a child is able tomaintain visual attention to an object or stimulus. The current studyemploys head-mounted eye trackers to study the cognitive processesunderlying SA by analyzing micro-level behaviors during parent-child social interactions in both typically and atypically developingchildren. Specifically, we examined the role of parent look, parenttouch, and child touch on SA duration. Results show that parent lookequally influences SA in both groups, while parent touch is morecritical for SA for TD children and the child’s own touching is morecritical for SA in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD).Implications of different pathways to maintain SA between typicallydeveloping children and children with ASD are discussed.

Language Development

Structured ecologies for social and linguistic development

This is a joint work of two labs that offers a perspective ondevelopment and learning, which complements theconference’s focus on “changes in representation andprocessing abilities in development”. Strong background inecological psychology allowed us to recognize the richnessand multilayered structuring of infants’ environment, whichactively engages them and to which infants tune their action-perception. We conceptualize this environment as reliable“social physics”, constituted of predictable, enacted socialevents, in which infants learn to participate. Using bothtraditional (qualitative and quantitative) and dynamicalsystems methods, we show the structuring of such events onmultiple timescales and levels and how participating in themsculpts the child’s agency in the social world. We show howthis background allows a fresh look on language acquisitionand how it informs computational modelling of languageemergence and models of human-robot interaction.

Not what you expect: The relationship between violation of expectation andnegation

Language acquisition research has shown that children aredelayed in their production and comprehension of truth-functional negation (e.g., “A raven is not a writing desk.”) ascompared to other kinds of negation (e.g., rejection and nonex-istence). The source of this delay is unclear, it may reflectdifficulty in mapping the concept of negation to the way itmanifests in their language, or it may be due to a lack of aconceptual or cognitive ability. This work aims to investigatethe circumstances under which a learner might infer the pres-ence of negation in a message, inspired by the approach ofPapafragou, Cassidy, and Gleitman (2007). Namely, we in-vestigate the degree to which videos in which agents fail incompleting an action encourages adult participants to infer theuse of negation in an utterance describing it. In addition toEvent Type (i.e., Failures vs. Successes), we provided par-ticipants with additional linguistic information (i.e., syntacticinformation via Jabberwocky sentences), lexical information(i.e., an alphabetical list of the content words), and Full Lin-guistic Context (the English sentence with a single item miss-ing). With adults, we ask whether learners with the ability toattend to goals and perceive deviations from their completioncould make use of this information, and if so, to what extentdo varying degrees of converging linguistic evidence furtherassist in inferring the use of a negator.

Can Automated Gesture Recognition Support the Study of Child LanguageDevelopment?

Children’s prelinguistic gestures play a central role in theircommunicative development. Early gesture use has beenshown to be predictive of both concurrent and later languageability, making the identification of gestures in video data atscale a potentially valuable tool for both theoretical and clini-cal purposes. We describe a new dataset consisting of videos of72 infants interacting with their caregivers at 11&12 months,annotated for the appearance of 12 different gesture types. Wepropose a model based on deep convolutional neural networksto classify these. The model achieves 48.32% classification ac-curacy overall, but with significant variation between gesturetypes. Critically, we found strong (0.7 or above) rank ordercorrelations between by-child gesture counts from human andmachine coding for 7 of the 12 gestures (including the criticalgestures of declarative pointing, hold outs and gives). Giventhe challenging nature of the data - recordings of many differ-ent dyads in different environments engaged in diverse activi-ties - we consider these results a very encouraging first attemptat the task, and evidence that automatic or machine-assistedgesture identification could make a valuable contribution to thestudy of cognitive development.

The (Un)Surprising Kindergarten Path

During sentence comprehension, listeners form expectationsabout likely structures before they have reached the end of asentence. Children are more likely than adults to ignore late-arriving evidence when it contradicts their initial parse. Whilethis difference is often ascribed to developmental changes inexecutive function, this paper investigates whether statisticalproperties of child-directed speech could be responsible forchildren’s failure to revise temporarily ambiguous sentences.We examined well-studied garden-path sentences andcalculated surprisal values derived from adult and child-directed corpora at each word. For adult corpora, surprisal washighest where the sentence structure was disambiguated. Forchild corpora, however, values at the disambiguating regionwere low relative to other words in the sentence. This suggeststhat for children, the disambiguating words may be statisticallyweak cues to ruling out their original parse, and that inprinciple, the statistics of child-directed speech couldcontribute to children’s difficulty with garden-path sentences.

The complementary roles of knowledge and strategy in insight problem-solving

Two main classes of theory have been proposed to account forinsight problem-solving performance; those that invoke theovercoming of constraints arising from prior knowledge as thesource of insight, and those that propose strategic search formoves that make progress towards a hypothesized goal state.An experiment using matchstick algebra problems assessed thecontributions of each source. Results indicate that, while priorknowledge creates the conditions under which matchstickalgebra problems are more or less difficult to solve, search formoves that make the most apparent progress towards ahypothesized goal provides the key to eventual solution.

Neural Networks

Context variability promotes generalization in reading aloud:Insight from a neural network simulation

How do neural network models of quasiregular domains learnto represent knowledge that varies in its consistency withthe domain, and generalize this knowledge appropriately?Recent work focusing on spelling-to-sound correspondencesin English proposes that a graded “warping” mechanismdetermines the extent to which the pronunciation of a newlylearned word should generalize to its orthographic neighbors.We explored the micro-structure of this proposal by training anetwork to pronounce new made-up words that were consistentwith the dominant pronunciation (regulars), were comprisedof a completely unfamiliar pronunciation (exceptions), orwere consistent with a subordinate pronunciation in English(ambiguous). Crucially, by training the same spelling-to-soundmapping with either one or multiple items, we tested whethervariation in adjacent, within-item context made a givenpronunciation more able to generalize. This is exactly whatwe found. Context variability, therefore, appears to act as amodulator of the warping in quasiregular domains.

Neural Language Models Capture Some, But Not All, Agreement AttractionEffects

The number of the subject in English must match the num-ber of the corresponding verb (dog runs but dogs run). Yetin real-time language production and comprehension, speak-ers often mistakenly compute agreement between the verb anda grammatically irrelevant non-subject noun phrase instead.This phenomenon, referred to as agreement attraction, is mod-ulated by a wide range of factors; any complete computationalmodel of grammatical planning and comprehension would beexpected to derive this rich empirical picture. Recent develop-ments in Natural Language Processing have shown that neuralnetworks trained only on word-prediction over large corporaare capable of capturing subject-verb agreement dependen-cies to a significant extent, but with occasional errors. In thispaper, we evaluate the potential of such neural word predic-tion models as a foundation for a cognitive model of real-timegrammatical processing. We use LSTMs, a common sequenceprediction model used to model language, to simulate six ex-periments taken from the agreement attraction literature. TheLSTMs captured the critical human behavior in three out of thesix experiments, indicating that (1) some agreement attractionphenomena can be captured by a generic sequence process-ing model, but (2) capturing the other phenomena may requiremodels with more language-specific mechanisms.

Probing Neural Language Models for Human Tacit Assumptions

Humans carry stereotypic tacit assumptions (STAs) (Prince,1978), or propositional beliefs about generic concepts. Suchassociations are crucial for understanding natural language.We construct a diagnostic set of word prediction prompts toevaluate whether recent neural contextualized language mod-els trained on large text corpora capture STAs. Our promptsare based on human responses in a psychological study of con-ceptual associations. We find models to be profoundly effec-tive at retrieving concepts given associated properties. Our re-sults demonstrate empirical evidence that stereotypic concep-tual representations are captured in neural models derived fromsemi-supervised linguistic exposure.

Events, Actions, and Sequencing

How to Help Best: Infants’ Changing Understanding of Multistep Actions Informstheir Evaluations of Helping

Research beginning with Piaget reveals a change in infants’understanding of multistep, means-end action sequences:Whereas 12-month-old infants reason that (e.g.) one opens abox to access its contents, younger infants are more likely toreason that one’s goal is simply to open the box. Here weexplore the implications of this developmental change ininfants’ action understanding for infants’ social evaluations.Using a puppet show paradigm, we examined infants’evaluations of two agents who helped another agent to achieveeither the end or the means of a means-end sequence, bothbefore and after 12 months of age. In a subsequent preferencetest, 15-month-old infants reached for an End-Helper over aMeans-Helper, whereas 8-month-old infants did the reverse.These findings link infants’ evaluation of helpers to theirrepresentations of action plans, consistent with recentcomputational models of naïve psychology.

Ten-month-olds infer relative costs of different goal-directed actions

While it is straightforward to compare the costs of differentvariants of the same action (e.g., walking to a coffeeshop at theend of the block will always be less costly than walking to acoffeeshop three blocks away), the relative costs of differentactions are not directly comparable (e.g., would it be easier tojump over or walk around a fence?). Across two experimentswe demonstrate that 10-month-old infants spontaneouslyencode the manner of different goal-directed actions (jumpingover an obstacle vs. detouring around it, Experiment 1) and usethe principle of cost-efficiency to infer their relative costs(jumping is less costly to bypass low walls, but detouring isless costly to bypass high walls, Experiment 2). By relatingaction choices to the physical parameters of the environment,infants identify the least costly actions given thecircumstances, which allows them to make behavioralpredictions in new environments and may also enable them toinfer others’ motor competence.

A rational model of sequential self-assessment

People’s assessment of their ability varies in whether it is mea-sured once following a task or sequentially via confidencejudgments recorded throughout. Multiple models have beendeveloped to predict one-off judgments of performance, whichhave often distinguished between peoples’ biases about theirgeneral ability in a domain and their sensitivity to correctness.We propose a rational model of sequential self-assessmentwhich allows us to make predictions about each individualseparately—unlike in the one-off case which looks exclusivelyat the population level—and to identify, in addition to bias andsensitivity, the extent to which individuals’ beliefs are respon-sive to their most recent evidence over the course of a task. Wefit our model to data where participants solve algebraic equa-tions and show that bias, sensitivity, and responsiveness varymeaningfully across participants.

Illusory causal connections and their effect on subjective probability

Our world is filled with statistical information: from dice rolls to lotteries, we often act based on our impressions of probability. Yet the human mind is not wired to reason about truly probabilistic events, often imposing structure on data or events where no such structure exists (as in ‘illusory correlations’). Here, we consider a case study in intuitive statistics: disjunctive events. For example, participants are asked to imagine a button that, when pressed, has a 1 in 100 chance of yielding a prize. They are told to imagine pressing that button 100 times. Across several paradigms, we show that people overestimate the probability of this disjunctive event — in stark contrast to classic demonstrations where people underestimate such probabilities (e.g., when iteratively selecting marbles from jars with replacement). These results reflect a tendency to view events as causally connected in illusory ways; implications for other domains of reasoning are discussed.

Intentionality Effects on Event Boundaries

Theories of event cognition have hypothesized that the boundaries of events are characterized by change, including a change in the agent’s goal, but the role of higher-order goal information on the placement of event boundaries has not been addressed experimentally. We tested whether goals can affect how viewers determine event boundaries. Participants read a context sentence stating an agent’s goal (e.g., “Jesse wants to eat the orange with her breakfast” vs. “Jesse wants to use the orange as a garnish”). Participants then saw an image of an event outcome (e.g., a partly peeled orange) and were asked to identify whether the event had occurred (“Did she peel the orange?”). Participants were more likely to respond Yes to a partly complete outcome if the outcome satisfied the agent’s goal. Our results offer the first direct evidence in support of the conclusion that higher-order intentionality information affects the way events are conceptualized.

Emotions and Beliefs

The Emotion-Induced Belief Amplification Effect

Exposure to images constitutes a ubiquitous day-to-dayexperience for most individuals. From mass-media exposure,to engagement with social-networking sites, to educationalcontexts, we are bombarded with images. Here, we explore theeffect that emotional images have on belief endorsement. Toinvestigate this effect, we test whether statements accompaniedby emotionally arousing images become more or lessbelievable than the same statements when they areaccompanied by neutral images or by no images. We find thatemotional images increase statement believability (Experiment1, replicated in preregistered Experiment 2). We discuss theimplications of this finding in the context of interventionsaimed at reducing misinformation.

How do Emotions Change during Learning with an Intelligent Tutoring System?Metacognitive Monitoring and Performance with MetaTutor

Emotional experiences have a significant impact on learningabout complex topics. Yet, challenges exist becauseemotions are typically operationalized as end products,excluding if, how, and when emotions change duringlearning and their relation to metacognition and performancewith advanced learning technologies such as intelligenttutoring systems (ITSs). In this paper, we addressed thesechallenges by capturing and analyzing 117 college students’concurrent and self-reported emotions at 3 time points duringlearning with MetaTutor, an ITS. Analyses revealed negativerelationships between increases in boredom, metacognitivemonitoring accuracy, and performance. We also foundthat if confusion persisted over time during learning, itwas detrimental to performance. These findings provideimplications for designing affect-sensitive ITSs which fosteremotion-regulation and metacognitive monitoring based onchanges in emotions during learning to optimize performance.

Emotional Words – The Relationship of Self- and Other-Annotation of Affect in Written Text

For human and automatic text annotation of emotions, it is as- sumed that affect can be traced in language on (combinations of) individual words, text fragments, or other linguistic pat- terns, which can be identified and labelled correctly. For exam- ple, many sentiment analysis systems consider isolated words affectively meaningful units, whose proportions in a given text reveal its overall affective meaning. However, whether these words and their combinations as identified either by humans or algorithms also match the actual feelings of the authors remains unclear. Potential discrepancies between affect expression and perception in text have received surprisingly little scholarly at- tention, although a number of studies has already identified dis- parities between self- and other-annotation in affect detection for speech and audio-visual data. Therefore, we ask whether a similar difference shows in annotations of emotions in text.

Inconsistencies Among Beliefs as a Basis for Learning via Thought Experiments

Although many studies have shown that being exposed toempirical data that contradict one’s beliefs can lead to learning,it is not clear whether calling attention to inconsistenciesamong beliefs without the provision of new data, leads tolearning. The present study asked whether calling attention toinconsistent beliefs via thought experiments leads to beliefrevision. Five-hundred-seventy-five participants were assignedto three different conditions in a pre-training, training, post-training design. The results showed that participants generatedinconsistent beliefs between pre-training and training, but theydid not spontaneously revise them at post-training (BaselineCondition). They did revise them, however, when they wereasked to reason about the implications of the training thoughtexperiments (Warning Condition) and when they saw anexplicit inference drawn from the training thought experiments(Explicit Inference Condition). These results show that, withprompting, scientifically naïve adults can learn from thoughtexperiments.

Crowdsourcing to Analyze Belief Systems Underlying Social Issues

People’s beliefs and attitudes about social and scientificissues, such as capital punishment and climate change, appearto form complex but generally coherent networks.Understanding the nature of these networks is a prerequisitefor designing interventions for changing beliefs on the basisof rational arguments and evidence. It is therefore importantto develop methods to represent and analyze the form andnature of belief networks, which may not be explicitlyverbalizable. Adopting an emerging approach that utilizescrowdsourcing to develop educational interventions, wemined discussions from the Reddit forum Change My View todetermine which beliefs and types of information underliepeople’s attitudes about capital punishment. By combiningcomputational analyses based on a topic model with morequalitative assessments of the extracted topics, we found thatmoral arguments are more prevalent than statistical ordata-based arguments. The present study serves as a test casefor the open sourced software crowdpy, a Python toolkit forrunning naturalistic studies on the web, which will enableother researchers to use crowdsourcing in their research. Thisapproach sets the stage for research exploring potentialinterventions to change people’s beliefs.

Complex Dynamics

Concealable Stigmatized Identity Disclosure as a Possible Perturbation to ComplexSocial Systems

Interpersonal coordination is essential for successfulcooperative action. Beyond synchronized joint action toachieve a goal such as moving furniture, humans tend tospontaneously coordinate movement in everyday action (i.e.,coordinated limb movement during walking). Furthermore,these actions are said to arise from the interaction dominantdynamics between agents and foment cooperative behavior. Assuch, existing research demonstrates that closer affiliation isassociated with entrainment of physiological signals includingheart beat and rhythmic limb movement. Considering the rolesocial stigmatization plays in disrupting social interaction, thepresent research investigated the impact of concealable stigmadisclosure (depression diagnosis or bisexual identity)—as aperturbation to a nonlinear dynamical system—oninterpersonal coordination and affiliation. Study 1 resultsdemonstrate that depression disclosure may lead to more socialdistancing in a collision avoidance walking task compared tobisexual and neutral disclosures. In study 2, interactionimproved affiliation regardless of disclosure type.

Quantifying Emergent, Dynamic Tonal Coordination in Collaborative MusicalImprovisation

Groups of interacting individuals often coordinate in service ofabstract goals, such as the alignment of mental representationsin conversation, or the generation of new ideas in group brain-storming sessions. What are the mechanisms and dynamicsof abstract coordination? This study examines coordination ina sophisticated paragon domain: collaboratively improvisingjazz musicians. Remarkably, freely improvising jazz ensem-bles collectively produce coherent tonal structure (i.e. melodyand harmony) in real time performance without previously es-tablished harmonic forms. We investigate how tonal structureemerges out of interacting musicians, and how this structureis constrained by underlying patterns of coordination. Dyadsof professional jazz pianists were recorded improvising in twoconditions of interaction: a ‘coupled’ condition in which theycould mutually adapt to one another, and an ‘overdubbed’ con-dition which precluded mutual adaptation. Using a computa-tional model of musical tonality, we show that this manipu-lation effected the directed flow of tonal information amongstpianists, who could mutually adapt to one another’s notes incoupled trials, but not in overdubbed trials. Consequently,musicians were better able to harmonize with one another incoupled trials, and this ability increased throughout the courseof improvised performance. We present these results and dis-cuss their implications for music technology and joint actionresearch more generally.

Mental state inference from indirect evidence through Bayesian eventreconstruction

From childhood, people routinely explain each other’s behav-ior in terms of inferred mental states, like beliefs and desires.In many cases, however, people can also infer the mental statesof agents whose behavior we cannot see, such as when we in-fer that someone was anxious upon encountering a chewed-uppencil, or that someone left in a hurry if they left the door open.Here we present a computational model of mental-state attri-bution that works by reconstructing the actions an agent took,based on the indirect evidence that revealed their presence. Ourmodel quantitatively fits participant judgments, outperforminga simple alternative cue-based account. Our results shed lighton how people infer mental states from minimal indirect evi-dence, and provides further support to the idea that human The-ory of Mind is instantiated as a probabilistic generative modelof how unobservable mental states produce observable action.

Formalizing Interdisciplinary Collaboration in the CogSci Community

Is cognitive science interdisciplinary or multidisciplinary? Wecontribute to this debate by examining the authorship struc-ture and topic similarity of contributions to the Cognitive Sci-ence Society from 2000 to 2019. We compare findings fromCogSci to abstracts from the Vision Science Society over thesame time frame. Our analysis focuses on graph theoretic fea-tures of the co-authorship network—edge density, transitivity,and maximum subgraph size—as well as clustering within thetopic space of CogSci contributions. We also combine struc-tural and semantic information with an analysis of homophily.We validate this approach by predicting new collaborations inthis year’s CogSci proceedings. Our results suggest that cog-nitive science has become increasingly interdisciplinary in thelast 19 years. More broadly, we argue that a formal quantita-tive approach which combines structural co-authorship infor-mation and semantic topic analysis provides inroads to ques-tions about the level of interdisciplinary collaboration in thecognitive science community.

Loss Functions Modulate the Optimal Bias-Variance Trade-off

Prediction problems vary in the extent to which accuracy isrewarded and inaccuracy is penalized—i.e., in their loss func-tions. Here, we focus on a particular feature of loss functionsthat controls how much large errors are penalized relative tohow much precise correctness is rewarded: convexity. Weshow that prediction problems with convex loss functions (i.e.,those in which large errors are particularly harmful) favor sim-pler models that tend to be biased, but exhibit low variability.Conversely, problems with concave loss functions (in whichprecise correctness is particularly rewarded) favor more com-plex models that are less biased, but exhibit higher variabil-ity. We discuss how this relationship between the bias-variancetrade-off and the shape of the loss function may help explainfeatures of human psychology, such as dual-process psychol-ogy and fast versus slow learning strategies, and inform statis-tical inference.

Learning and Development

Awe Yields Learning: A Virtual Reality Study

There is a considerable amount of literature on the role ofimmersion and presence in virtual reality learningenvironments. Far less is known about the interaction ofimmersion and presence with the important individualcharacteristics that influence learning behavior, particularly,dispositional awe. Dispositional awe is manifested by anemotional response to information that defies existing mentalschemas in a given domain and by a need to accommodate thisexperience. In a virtual reality study with eight elementaryschool classes, we investigated the interaction of immersivetendencies with dispositional awe and compassion on learninggains in the domain of nature conservation. We tested thisinteraction using a novel virtual reality concept in whichchildren are sent to virtually simulated space to experience theoverview effect, a cognitive shift in awareness reported byastronauts. The findings of the study showed that participantsexperienced strong feelings of awe and scored highly onoverview effect constructs. Importantly, their learning gainswere influenced by the overview effect which was, in turn,supported by presence, dispositional awe, and compassion.This study shows the potential of using immersive virtualreality experiences in educational programs, combiningwonder and learning.

Hearing water temperature:Characterizing the development of nuanced perception of auditory events

Without conscious thought, listeners link events in the worldto sounds they hear. We study one surprising example: Adultscan judge the temperature of water simply from hearing itbeing poured. How do these nuanced perceptual skillsdevelop? Is extensive auditory experience required, or arethese skills present in early childhood? In Exp.1, adults wereexceptionally good at judging whether water was hot vs. coldfrom pouring sounds (M=93% accuracy; N=104). In Exp.2, wetested this ability in N=113 children aged 3-12 years, and foundevidence of developmental change: Age significantly predictedaccuracy (p<0.001, logistic regression), such that 3-5 year oldchildren performed at chance while 85% of children age 6+answered correctly. Overall our data suggest that perception ofnuanced differences between auditory events is not part ofearly-developing cross-modal cognition, and instead developsover the first six years of life.

Specificity of Infant Statistical Learning

Sensitivity to transitional probabilities (TP) in continuous speech has been extensively documented, yet little is knownabout how infants represent sequences that are the output of statistical learning. Across 3 experiments we test 8-month-old English-learning infants indexical, segmental, and suprasegmental representations of newly-encountered statistically-defined words. Following familiarization with a naturally-produced Italian corpus that contained two trochaic (strong-weak) high TP (HTP) words produced by a female speaker, infants were tested on their ability to discriminate modifiedHTP words (Experiment 1=male voice; Experiment 2=onset consonant change); Experiment 3=iambic stress pattern),from foils. Infants demonstrated a significant familiarity preference for modified HTP words in Experiments 1 and 3,but failed to recognize consonant modified HTP words in Experiment 2. Findings demonstrate infants can generalizerepresentations of statistically-defined words across a range of acoustic forms less relevant to word meaning in English,but not across phonemic characteristics that are core to word meaning.

Children hear more about what is atypical than what is typical

How do children learn the typical features of objects in theworld? For many objects, this information must come from thelanguage they hear. However, language does not veridicallyreflect the world: People are more likely to talk about atypicalfeatures (e.g., “purple carrot”) than typical features (e.g., “or-ange carrot”). Does the speech children hear from their parentsalso overrepresent atypical features? We examined the typical-ity of adjectives produced by parents in a large, longitudinalcorpus of parent-child interaction. Across nearly 2000 uniqueadjective–noun pairs, we found parents’ adjectives predomi-nantly mark atypical features of objects, although parents ofvery young children are relatively more likely to comment ontypical features as well. We then used vector space models toshow that learning the typical features of common categoriesfrom linguistic input alone is challenging even with sophisti-cated statistical inference techniques.

Numerosity

Dynamics vs. Development in Numerosity Estimation: A Computational ModelAccurately Predicts a Developmental Reversal

Perceptual judgments result from a dynamic process, but little is known about the dynamics of numerosity estimation. Arecent study proposed a computational model (D-MLLM) that combined a model of trial-to-trial changes with a modelfor the internal scaling of discrete number. Here, we tested a surprising prediction of the model – a situation in whichchildren’s estimates of numerosity would be better than those of adults. Consistent with the model simulations, taskcontexts led to a clear developmental reversal: children made more adult-like, linear estimates when to-be-estimatednumbers were descending over trials (backward), whereas adults became more like children with log estimates whennumbers were ascending (forward). In addition, adults’ estimates were subject to inter-trial differences regardless ofstimulus order. In contrast, children were not able to use the trial-to-trial dynamics unless task contexts were salient(forward or backward order), indicating the limited memory capacity for dynamic updates. Together, the model adequatelypredicts both developmental and trial-to-trial changes in number-line tasks.

Children use one-to-one correspondence to establish equality after learning tocount

Humans make frequent and powerful use of external symbolsto express number exactly, leading some to question whetherexact number concepts are only available through the acqui-sition of symbolic number systems. Although prior work hasaddressed this longstanding debate on the relationship betweenlanguage and thought in innumerate populations and semi-numerate children, it has frequently produced conflicting re-sults, leaving the origin of exact number concepts unclear.Here, we return to this question by replicating methods pre-viously used to assess exact number knowledge in innumer-ate groups, such as the Pirah ̃a, with a large sample of semi-numerate US toddlers. We replicate previous findings fromboth innumerate cultures and developmental studies showingthat numeracy is linked to the concept of exact number. How-ever, we also find evidence that this knowledge is surprisinglyfragile even amongst numerate children, suggesting that nu-meracy alone does not guarantee a full understanding of exact-ness.

How Reliable is the Give-a-Number task?

The Give-a-Number task has become a gold standard ofchildren’s number word comprehension and has beenincreasingly used to organize debate in developmentalpsychology. In this task, the experimenter asks children togive specific numbers of objects (e.g., 1 to 6), and based ontheir pattern of responses, children are classified into stagesthat can be readily related to other developmental milestones.The increasing popularity of Give-a-Number raises thequestion of how reliable it is, since the size of a correlationbetween two different tasks cannot reliably exceed the test-retest reliability of either measure taken individually. InExperiment 1, 2- to 4-year-old children were tested twice in asingle session with Wynn’s (1992) version of the Give-a-Number task, which features a titrated design. In Experiment2, we tested a second group of children with an alternativeversion that uses a larger number of trials in a non-titrateddesign. We found that in both cases the task was highlyreliable in differentiating children who could accurately countfrom those who could not, but that reliability differed forspecific numbers, and was more reliable for very smallnumbers (i.e., “one” and “two”) than for slightly larger ones(i.e., “three” and “four”). We discuss practical implications ofthese results for researchers studying numeracy and discussfurther directions to assess the validity of the task.

Starting small: Exploring the origins of successor function knowledge

Although most U.S. children can count sets by 3.5 years of age, many fail to understand that adding 1 to a set correspondsto counting up 1 word in the count list (i.e., the successor function). Initially, children have piecemeal knowledge of thisrelation, and do not understand that it holds for any number. Although generalized successor knowledge emerges around6 years of age, it is unknown when children’s item-based learning begins, and therefore when they begin learning relationsbetween number words – a critical precursor to mathematical reasoning. Here, we explore the timescale and mechanismsunderlying this knowledge in 2- to 4-year-old children. We find that these children have established item-based mappings,but that they are unrelated to count list knowledge. Instead, we show evidence that the origins of successor knowledgemay lie in mappings made between non-symbolic set representations and known number words.

Does the number sense represent number?

On a now orthodox view, humans and many other animals areendowed with a “number sense”, or approximate number system(ANS), that represents number. Recently, this orthodox view hasbeen subject to numerous critiques, with critics maintaining eitherthat numerical content is absent altogether, or else that someprimitive analog of number (‘numerosity’) is represented as opposedto number itself. We distinguish three arguments for these claims –the arguments from congruency, confounds, and imprecision – andshow that none succeed. We then highlight positive reasons forthinking that the ANS genuinely represents numbers. The upshot isthat proponents of the orthodox view should not feel troubled byrecent critiques of their position.

Language and Meaning

Informational goals, sentence structure, and comparison class inference

Understanding a gradable adjective (e.g., big) requires mak-ing reference to a comparison class, a set of objects or entitiesagainst which the referent is implicitly compared (e.g., big fora Great Dane), but how do listeners decide upon a compari-son class? Simple models of semantic composition stipulatethat the adjective combines with a noun, which necessarily be-comes the comparison class (e.g., “That Great Dane is big”means big for a Great Dane). We investigate an alternativehypothesis built on the idea that the utility of a noun in anadjectival utterance can be either for reference (getting the lis-tener to attend to the right object) or predication (describing aproperty of the referent). Therefore, we hypothesize that whenthe presence of a noun N can be explained away by its util-ity in reference (e.g., being in the subject position: “That N isbig”), it is less likely to set the comparison class. Across threepre-registered experiments, we find evidence that listeners usethe noun as a cue to infer comparison classes consistent with atrade-off between reference and predication. This work high-lights the complexity of the relation between the form of anutterance and its meaning.

Do Taxonomic and Associative Relations Affect Word Production in the Same Way?

Naming a picture is more difficult in the context of a taxonomically-related picture. Disagreement exists on whether non-taxonomic relations, e.g., associations, have similar or different effects on picture naming. Past work has reported facilitation, interference and null results but with inconsistent methodologies. We paired the same target word (e.g., cow) with unrelated (pen), taxonomically-related (bear), and associatively-related (milk) items in different blocks, as participants repeatedly named one of the two pictures in randomized order. Significant interference was uncovered for the same target item in the taxonomic vs. unrelated and associative blocks. There was no robust evidence of interference in the associative blocks. If anything, evidence suggested that associatively-related items marginally facilitated production. This finding suggests that taxonomic and associative relations have different effects on picture naming and has implications for theoretical models of lexical selection and, more generally, for the computations involved in mapping semantic features to lexical items.

Reconstructing Maps from Text

Previous research has demonstrated that Distributional Semantic Models (DSMs) are capable of reconstructing maps from news corpora (Louwerse & Zwaan, 2009) and novels (Louwerse & Benesh, 2012). The capacity for reproducing maps is surprising since DSMs notoriously lack perceptual grounding (De Vega et al., 2012). In this paper we investigate the statistical sources required in language to infer maps, and resulting constraints placed on mechanisms of semantic representation. Study 1 brings word co-occurrence under experimental control to demonstrate that direct co-occurrence in language is necessary for traditional DSMs to successfully reproduce maps. Study 2 presents an instance-based DSM that is capable of reconstructing maps independent of the frequency of co-occurrence of city names.

Does Surprisal Predict Code Comprehension Difficulty?

Recognition of the similarities between programming and nat-ural languages has led to a boom in the adoption of languagemodeling techniques in tools that assist developers. However,language model surprisal, which guides the training and eval-uation in many of these methods, has not been validated asa measure of cognitive difficulty for programming languagecomprehension as it has for natural language. We perform acontrolled experiment to evaluate human comprehension onfragments of source code that are meaning-equivalent but withdifferent surprisal. We find that more surprising versions ofcode take humans longer to finish answering correctly. Wealso provide practical guidelines to design future studies forcode comprehension and surprisal.

Speech and Phonetics

Evaluating computational models of infant phonetic learning across languages

In the first year of life, infants’ speech perception becomesattuned to the sounds of their native language. Many accountsof this early phonetic learning exist, but computational modelspredicting the attunement patterns observed in infants fromthe speech input they hear have been lacking. A recent studypresented the first such model, drawing on algorithms proposedfor unsupervised learning from naturalistic speech, and tested iton a single phone contrast. Here we study five such algorithms,selected for their potential cognitive relevance. We simulatephonetic learning with each algorithm and perform tests onthree phone contrasts from different languages, comparing theresults to infants’ discrimination patterns. The five models dis-play varying degrees of agreement with empirical observations,showing that our approach can help decide between candidatemechanisms for early phonetic learning, and providing insightinto which aspects of the models are critical for capturing in-fants’ perceptual development.

Input matters in the modeling of early phonetic learning

In acquiring language, differences in input can greatly affectlearning outcomes, but which aspects of language learning aremost sensitive to input variations, and which are robust, remainsdebated. A recent modeling study successfully reproduced aphenomenon empirically observed in early phonetic learning—learning about the sounds of the native language in the firstyear of life—despite using input that differed in quantity andspeaker composition from what a typical infant would hear. Inthis paper, we carry out a direct test of that model’s robustnessto input variations. We find that, despite what the original resultsuggested, the learning outcomes are sensitive to properties ofthe input and that more plausible input leads to a better fit withempirical observations. This has implications for understandingearly phonetic learning in infants and underscores the impor-tance of using realistic input in models of language acquisition.

The Perceptimatic English Benchmark for Speech Perception Models

We present the Perceptimatic English Benchmark, an open ex-perimental benchmark for evaluating quantitative models ofspeech perception in English. The benchmark consists of ABXstimuli along with the responses of 91 American English-speaking listeners. The stimuli test discrimination of a largenumber of English and French phonemic contrasts. They areextracted directly from corpora of read speech, making themappropriate for evaluating statistical acoustic models (such asthose used in automatic speech recognition) trained on typicalspeech data sets. We show that phone discrimination is corre-lated with several types of models, and give recommendationsfor researchers seeking easily calculated norms of acoustic dis-tance on experimental stimuli. We show that DeepSpeech,a standard English speech recognizer, is more specialized onEnglish phoneme discrimination than English listeners, and ispoorly correlated with their behaviour, even though it yields alow error on the decision task given to humans.

A Model of Prenatal Acquisition of Vowels

Humans learn much about their language while still in thewomb. Prenatal exposure has been repeatedly shown to affectnewborn infants’ processing of the prosodic characteristics ofnative language speech. Little is known about whether and howprenatal exposure affects infants’ perception of speech soundsegments. Here we simulated prenatal learning of vowels intwo virtual fetuses whose mothers spoke (slightly) differentlanguages. The learners were two-layer neural networks andwere each exposed to vowel tokens sampled from an existentfive-vowel language (Spanish and Czech, respectively). Theinput acoustic properties approximated the speech signal thatcould possibly be heard in the intrauterine environment, andthe learners’ auditory system was relatively immature. Withoutsupervision, the virtual fetuses came to warp the continuousacoustic signal into “proto-categories” that were specific totheir linguistic environment. Both learners came to create twocategorization patterns and did so in language-specific ways,primarily on the basis of the vowels’ first-formantcharacteristics. Such prenatally formed proto-categories werenot adult-like in that they entirely collapsed some of the native-language contrasts. At the same time, the categories reflectedfeatures of the adult language in that they were language-specific. These results can inspire future work on speech andlanguage acquisition in real young humans.

Spatial Cognition

The semantics of spatial demonstratives

Spatial demonstratives (words like this and that) are thought toprimarily be used for carving up space into a peripersonal andextrapersonal domain. However, when given a noun out ofcontext and asked to couple it with a demonstrative, speakerstend to use this for manipulable objects (small, harmless,inanimate), while non-manipulable objects (large, harmful,animate) are more likely to be coupled with that. Here, weextend these findings and map demonstrative use along a widespectrum of semantic features. We conducted a large-scale (N= 2197) experiment eliciting demonstratives for 506 words,rated across 65+11 perceptually and cognitively relevantsemantic dimensions. We replicated the findings thatdemonstrative choice is influenced by object manipulability.Demonstrative choice was additionally found to be related to aset of semantic factors, including valence, arousal, loudness,motion, time and more generally, the self. Importantly,demonstrative choices were highly structured acrossparticipants, as shown by a strong correlation detected in asplit-sample comparison of by-word demonstrativedistribution.

A Cross-Cultural Principle Of Temporal Spatialization

The Temporal Focus Hypothesis proposes that a person’s tendency to conceptualize either the past or the future as beinglocated in front of them depends on their temporal focus: the balance of attention paid to the past (tradition) and thefuture (progress). How general is the TFH and to what extent can cultures and subcultures be placed on a single linerelating time spatialization and temporal focus in spite of stark differences in language, religion, history, and economicdevelopment? Data from 10 Western and Middle Eastern (sub)cultural groups (N=1198) were used to derive a linearmodel relating aggregated temporal focus and proportion of future-in-front responses. This model then successfully fittednine independently collected (sub)cultural groups in China and Vietnam (N=841). A logistic mixed model computedover the whole dataset (N=2039) showed that the group-level relation arose at the individual level and allowed precisequantification of its influence. Temporal focus shapes how people around the world think of time in spatial terms.

Data Foraging: Spatiotemporal Data Collection Decisions in Disciplinary FieldScience

Field scientists collect data in a noisy heterogeneous environment, where the value of additional data for characterizingthe natural system is weighed against the time and money involved in data collection. This is analogous to foraging forfood data is the resource and its collection can be optimized based on energy costs. Here we conduct a novel simulateddata foraging study to elucidate how spatiotemporal data collection decisions are made in field sciences, and how search isadapted in response to in-situ data. Expert geoscientists were asked to evaluate a hypothesis by collecting environmentaldata using a mobile robot. At any point, participants were able to stop the robot and change their search strategy ormake a conclusion about the hypothesis. We identified previously unrecognized spatiotemporal reasoning heuristics, towhich scientists strongly anchored, displaying limited adaptation in response to new data. We analyzed two key decisionfactors: variable-space coverage, and fitting error to a given hypothesis. We found that, despite varied search strategies, themajority of scientists made a conclusion as the fitting error converged. Scientists who made premature conclusions, eitherdue to insufficient variable-space coverage or before the fitting error stabilized, were more prone to incorrect conclusions.We believe the findings from this study could be used to improve field science training in data foraging, and aid in thedevelopment of technologies to support data collection decisions.

Multi-directional mappings in the minds of the Tsimane’:Size, time, and number on three spatial axes

From early in life, people implicitly associate time, number,and other abstract conceptual domains with space. Accord-ing to the Generalized Magnitude System proposal, these men-tal mappings reflect a common neural system for represent-ing various magnitudes, and share a common spatial organiza-tion. In a test of this proposal, here we measured mappings ofsize, time, and number in the Tsimane’, an indigenous Ama-zonian group with few of the cultural practices (like readingand math) that spatialize size, time, and number in the expe-rience of industrialized adults. On three spatial axes, the Tsi-mane’ systematically arranged imagistic stimuli according totheir magnitudes, but they showed no directional preferencesoverall and individuals often mapped different domains in op-posite directions. The results are inconsistent with predictionsof the Generalized Magnitude System proposal but can be ex-plained by Hierarchical Mental Metaphor Theory, accordingto which mental mappings initially reflect a set of correlationsobservable in the natural world.

Grounding Spatial Language in Perception by Combining Conceptsin a Neural Dynamic Architecture

We present a neural dynamic architecture that grounds sen-tences in perception which combine multiple concepts throughnested spatial relations. Grounding entails that the model getsfeatures and relations as categorical inputs and matches themto objects in space-continuous neural maps which represent vi-sual input. The architecture is based on the neural principlesof dynamic field theory. It autonomously generates sequencesof processing steps in continuous time, based solely on highlyrecurrent connectivity. Simulations of the architecture showthat it can ground sentences of varying complexity. We thusaddress two major challenges in dealing with nested relations:how concepts may appear in multiple different relational roleswithin the same sentence, and how in such a scenario variousgrounding outcomes may be “tried out” in a form of hypothesistesting. We close by discussing empirical evidence for crucialassumptions and choices made when developing the architec-ture.

Categorization

Identifying the neural dynamics of category decisions with computational model-based fMRI

Successful categorization requires a careful coordination ofattention, representation, and decision making. Comprehensivetheories that span levels of analysis are key to understandingthe computational and neural dynamics of categorization. Here,we build on recent work linking neural representations ofcategory learning to computational models to investigate howcategory decision making is driven by neural signals across thebrain. We combine functional magnetic resonance imagingwith hierarchical drift diffusion modelling to show that trial-by-trial fluctuations in neural activation from regions ofoccipital, cingulate, and lateral prefrontal cortices are linked tocategory decisions. Notably, lateral prefrontal cortex activationwas also associated with exemplar-based model predictions oftrial-by-trial category evidence. We propose that these brainregions underlie distinct functions that contribute to successfulcategory learning.

End-to-end Deep Prototype and Exemplar Models for Predicting Human Behavior

Traditional models of category learning in psychology focuson representation at the category level as opposed to the stim-ulus level, even though the two are likely to interact. Thestimulus representations employed in such models are eitherhand-designed by the experimenter, inferred circuitously fromhuman judgments, or borrowed from pretrained deep neuralnetworks that are themselves competing models of categorylearning. In this work, we extend classic prototype and ex-emplar models to learn both stimulus and category represen-tations jointly from raw input. This new class of models canbe parameterized by deep neural networks (DNN) and trainedend-to-end. Following their namesakes, we refer to them asDeep Prototype Models, Deep Exemplar Models, and DeepGaussian Mixture Models. Compared to typical DNNs, wefind that their cognitively inspired counterparts both providebetter intrinsic fit to human behavior and improve ground-truthclassification.

Contrasting Exemplar and Prototype Models in a Natural-Science Category Domain

A classic issue in the cognitive-science of human categorylearning has involved the contrast between exemplar and prototypemodels. However, experimental tests to distinguish the models haverelied almost solely on use of artificial categories composed ofsimplified stimuli. Here we contrast the predictions from the modelsin a real-world natural-science category domain – geologic rocktypes. Previous work in this domain used a set of complementarymethods, including multidimensional scaling and direct dimensionratings, to derive a high-dimensional feature space in which the rockstimuli are embedded. The present work compares the category-learning predictions of exemplar and prototype models that makereference to this derived feature space. The experiments includeconditions that should be favorable to prototype abstraction,including use of large-size categories, delayed transfer testing, andreal-world natural category structures. Nevertheless, the results ofthe qualitative and quantitative model comparisons point toward theexemplar model as providing a better account of the observedresults. Limitations and directions of future work are discussed.

The evolution of category systems within and between learners

How do cumulative cultural evolution and individual learningdiffer? In an abstract computational sense, both are optimisa-tion processes that search a space of possible explanations andprevious work has identified deep parallels in the mathematicalmodels used to describe them (Suchow, Bourgin, & Griffiths,2017). However, there are obvious differences as well: forexample, individual learning involves a single agent charac-terised by one set of prior beliefs, representational capabilities,and so forth, while cultural evolution involves multiple agentswho may vary along these factors. We argue that this differ-ence implies that the process of cumulative cultural evolutionshould involve searching a more restricted set of hypothesesand converge on simpler ones. In two iterated category learn-ing experiments, we test this prediction and find that transmis-sion chains composed of single individuals, who learn basedon their previous performance, consider both a wider varietyand more complex categorisation schemas than do chains in-volving multiple people.

The Effects of Feature Verbalizablity on Category Learning

This study intended to investigate the effects of varying factorson the use of verbal and implicit classification systems whenlearning novel categories in an interactive video gameenvironment by measuring the effects of feature type (easy vsdifficult to describe verbally). Verbal and implicitclassification were operationalized by measuring rule-basedand family resemblance strategy use respectively. Thisexperiment found that participants presented with stimuli thatwere easy to describe verbally were more likely to use rule-based classification, while participants presented with stimulithat were difficult to describe verbally showed no preferencefor one form of classification. The results of this study open upa novel field of research within category learning, furtherexploring the effects of feature verbalizablity.

Reading and Processing

Online Article Comprehension in Monolingual Spanish-Speaking Preschoolers with Specific Language Impairment: A Language-Mediated Visual Attention Study

Article production difficulties in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI) are well documented. However, evidence on article comprehension is scarce. In an eye tracking experiment, we compared online comprehension of definite and indefinite articles in monolingual Spanish- speaking children with SLI and children with typical language development (TLD) matched for age. Children listened to simple phrases while inspecting a visual context with four images. The article in the phrase agreed in number and gender with the target image only. Visual target preference was monitored as the phrase unfolded. Eye movements revealed that children with SLI showed a weak preference for the target on indefinite article trials only after hearing the noun, although no significant effects of definiteness were observed. In contrast, children with TLD were able to use the article to anticipate the noun. These findings contribute to reducing the gap between article production and comprehension in children with SLI.

Event-related potentials reveal differences between foveal and parafovealintegration of visual and contextual information during sentence processing

Electrical brain potentials in response to violation of expectations in language processing have revealed that people usesentence context to facilitate word recognition and integration. Less is known about the interaction between the qualityof visual information in reading and the use of contextual information. In the current study we manipulated the visualfield (foveal vs. parafoveal) in which a sentence-final expected word, orthographic neighbor of an expected word, orunexpected word is presented and recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the role of visual clarity. We findevidence that earlier stages of semantic retrieval indexed by the N400 are resilient to visual information presented at greatereccentricity, but that later, integration-related processes indexed by a posterior late positive complex (LPC) may depend onunambiguous, foveally presented visual information. These findings have implications for parafoveal processing duringnatural reading.

Synchrony and asynchrony of the two eyes in binocular fixationsin the reading of English and Chinese; the implications for ocular prevalence

We explore low-level, behavioural universals in reading,across English and Chinese. We investigated binocularcoordination in terms of the small non-alignments betweenthe two eyes’ fixations in time. We define a typology of ninesuch asynchronies and report the different spatial distributionsof these types across the screen of text. We interpret them interms of their implications for ocular prevalence—theprioritizing of the input from one eye over the input from theother eye in higher perception/cognition, after binocularfusion. The results show striking similarities of binocularreading behaviours across the two very differentorthographies. Asynchronies in which one eye begins thefixation earlier and/or ends it later occur most frequently inthe hemifield corresponding to that eye. We propose that suchsmall asynchronies in binocular fixations prioritize the higherprocessing of the input from that eye, after binocular fusion.

Task effects on the lexical boost effect in structural priming

Four structural priming experiments investigated the lexical boost effect in structural priming. In two experiments, wetested whether repeating the subject in prepositional object or double object ditransitive structures boosted structuralpriming. In two other experiments, we manipulated the repetition of the verb. Repetition of the subject noun affectedstructural priming, but only when the prime remained visible while participants produced the target sentence. In contrast,repetition of the verb boosted priming regardless of whether participants could see the prime and target simultaneously.We conclude that the subject noun repetition effect is more strategic in nature than the verb boost effect. Structures areautomatically associated with the verb, their syntactic head, whereas repetition of the subject noun only affects priming ifthe presentation method makes the repetition highly explicit.

Is Segmental Interference Position-dependent?

The paper investigates the existence of position-independent segments in written and typed word production. In two experiments, we employed the segmental interference effect to first replicate past findings that naming a picture is more difficult in the context of another picture with which it shares segments in the same position (e.g., glow-flow) compared to an unrelated word (e.g., glow-cave). We then tested a new condition, in which the same target word is paired with an anagram of the original competitor (glow-wolf). Critically, the anagram shared the same number of segments with the target word, but never in the same position. Both experiments found robust interference for targets produced in the context of anagrams, with a magnitude comparable to the interference induced by the position-overlapping word. The results suggest that not only are position-independent segments represented in the production system, but they also play a critical role in activating segmentally related words and creating competition during word production.

Language and Uncertainty

When Generic Language does not Promote Psychological Essentialism

Generic language (e.g., “Women are nurturing”; “Women donot like math”) is prominent in child-directed speech, and hasbeen shown to promote essentialist beliefs about the relevantkind, supporting stereotyping and prejudice. Here weinvestigate a theoretically-motivated intervention to break thelink between generics and essentialist assumptions. In a studywith 223 3-8-year-old children who learned about novel socialgroups from generic language, we demonstrate that a structuralconstrual of generics (attributing the category-propertyassociation to stable external constraints) mitigates essentialistassumptions about social categories. We discuss practicalapplications for reducing stereotype endorsement, andtheoretical implications regarding the meaning of genericlanguage and the development of social kind representations.

Measuring prosodic predictability in children’s home language environments

Children learn language from the speech in their home envi-ronment. Recent work shows that more infant-directed speech(IDS) leads to stronger lexical development. But what makesIDS a particularly useful learning signal? Here, we expandon an attention-based account first proposed by R ̈as ̈anen etal. (2018): that prosodic modifications make IDS less pre-dictable, and thus more interesting. First, we reproduce thecritical finding from R ̈as ̈anen et al.: that lab-recorded IDS pitchis less predictable compared to adult-directed speech (ADS).Next, we show that this result generalizes to the home lan-guage environment, finding that IDS in daylong recordings isalso less predictable than ADS but that this pattern is muchless robust than for IDS recorded in the lab. These results linkexperimental work on attention and prosodic modifications ofIDS to real-world language-learning environments, highlight-ing some challenges of scaling up analyses of IDS to largerdatasets that better capture children’s actual input.

Forward-looking Effects in Subject Pronoun Interpretation:What Comes Next Matters

We report two experiments investigating how the interpretationof subject-position pronouns is guided by the referentialstructure of the pronoun-containing clause, and how thisinformation interacts with information available in the clausethat precedes the pronoun. Thus, we consider information thatis available to the language processing system before thepronoun is encountered (pre-pronominal information), as wellas information that comes after the pronoun (post-pronominalinformation). In particular, we test how implicit causalitybiases of verbs that precede the pronoun-containing clauseinteract with the referential structure of the pronoun-containingclause, i.e., whether or not the clause with the pronoun containsanother ambiguous pronoun. We report two offline studieswhose results reveal significant effects of both pre- and post-pronominal referential information on pronoun resolution: Inaddition to replicating effects of implicit causality biasesobserved in prior work, we also show that people’s referentialbiases depend on whether the clause contains only a subject-position pronoun or also a second pronoun in object position.

Learner dynamics in a model of wug inflection:Integrating frequency and phonology

A recent large-scale wug-task study found that non-nativespeakers of English tend to produce fewer regular past-tense-ed inflections than native speakers (Cuskley et al., 2015). Inthis paper we present a model that can account for this dif-ference in behaviour as resulting from a difference in inputamounts and distributions. This model attends to both fre-quency, using Bayesian non-parametric methods, and phono-logical similarity between words, using a neural model of wordforms, and unifies these factors within a single probabilisticframework. We show that the general pattern of over-use ofirregular inflections in non-native speakers can result simplyfrom exposure to a smaller amount of input and does not re-quire any model-internal distinction of native and non-nativespeakers. Our model also captures the interaction betweenclass frequency and phonological similarity that was evidentacross all participant productions.

Learning under uncertainty changes during adolescence

As we transition from child to adult, we navigate the worlddifferently. In this world, many of the relationships betweenevents are unclear or uncertain because they are probabilisticin nature. We wanted to know how learning about probabilis-tic relationships changes with development and to interrogatethe underlying processes. We investigated these questions in aprobabilistic reinforcement learning task (The Butterfly Task)with 302 participants aged 8-30. We found performance in thistask increased with age through early-twenties, then stabilized.Using hierarchical Bayesian methods to fit computational rein-forcement learning models, we showed that this performanceincrease was driven by 1) an increase in learning rate (i.e. de-crease in integration time horizon); 2) a decrease in exploratorychoices. By contrast, forgetting rates did not change with age.We discuss our findings in the context of other studies and hy-potheses about adolescent brain development.

Linguistics

Linguistic Overhypotheses in Category Learning:Explaining the Label Advantage Effect

When learning to partition the world into categories, peoplerely on a set of assumptions (overhypotheses) about possi-ble category structures. Here we propose that the nature ofthese overhypotheses depends on the presence of a verbal la-bel associated with a given category. We describe a computa-tional model that demonstrates how labels can either acceler-ate or hinder category learning, depending on whether or notthe prior beliefs imposed by their presence align with the truecategory structure. This account provides an explanation forthe phenomena described in prior experimental work (Lupyan,Rakison, & McClelland, 2007; Brojde, Porter, & Colunga,2011) that have remained unexplained by other models. Basedon these results, we argue that the overhypothesis theory of la-bel effects provides a way to formalize and quantify the effectof language on category learning and to develop a more precisedelineation between linguistic and non-linguistic thought.

Prototype theory and emotion semantic change

An elaborate repertoire of emotions is one feature that dis-tinguishes humans from animals. Language offers a criticalform of emotion expression. However, it is unclear whetherthe meaning of an emotion word remains stable, and what fac-tors may underlie changes in emotion meaning. We hypothe-size that emotion word meanings have changed over time andthat the prototypicality of an emotion term drives this changebeyond general factors such as word frequency. We developa vector-space representation of emotion and show that thismodel replicates empirical findings on prototypicality judg-ments and basic categories of emotion. We provide evidencethat more prototypical emotion words have undergone lesschange in meaning than peripheral emotion words over the pastcentury, and that this trend holds within each family of emo-tion. Our work extends synchronic theories of emotion to itsdiachronic development and offers a computational character-ization of emotion semantics in natural language use.

Universal linguistic inductive biases via meta-learning

How do learners acquire languages from the limited data avail-able to them? This process must involve some inductivebiases—factors that affect how a learner generalizes—but it isunclear which inductive biases can explain observed patternsin language acquisition. To facilitate computational model-ing aimed at addressing this question, we introduce a frame-work for giving particular linguistic inductive biases to a neu-ral network model; such a model can then be used to em-pirically explore the effects of those inductive biases. Thisframework disentangles universal inductive biases, which areencoded in the initial values of a neural network’s param-eters, from non-universal factors, which the neural networkmust learn from data in a given language. The initial statethat encodes the inductive biases is found with meta-learning,a technique through which a model discovers how to acquirenew languages more easily via exposure to many possible lan-guages. By controlling the properties of the languages that areused during meta-learning, we can control the inductive biasesthat meta-learning imparts. We demonstrate this frameworkwith a case study based on syllable structure. First, we specifythe inductive biases that we intend to give our model, and thenwe translate those inductive biases into a space of languagesfrom which a model can meta-learn. Finally, using existinganalysis techniques, we verify that our approach has impartedthe linguistic inductive biases that it was intended to impart.

Birds and Words: Exploring environmental influences on folk categorization

Anthropologists and psychologists have long studied how liv-ing kinds are organized into categories, and a recurring themeconcerns the relationship between folk categories and thestructure of the environment. We ask whether the frequencyand physical size of a species affect how it is classified, andaddress this question by linking frequency data from eBird (anonline database of bird observations) with an existing taxon-omy of Zapotec bird names. A first set of analyses exploreswhether frequency and size predict whether a bird is namedand how many other birds it is grouped with. A second setexplores whether frequency and size predict the word formsused as category labels. We find some evidence that frequencyaffects both category extensions and naming, but the resultshint that frequency may be dominated by other factors such asperceptual similarity.

Comparative and Cultural Cognition

Rational After All: Changes in Probability Matching Behaviour Across Time inHumans and Monkeys

Probability matching—where subjects given probabilistic in-put respond in a way that is proportional to those inputprobabilities—has long been thought to be characteristic ofprimate performance in probability learning tasks in a vari-ety of contexts, from decision making to the learning of lin-guistic variation in humans. However, such behaviour is puz-zling because it is not optimal in a decision theoretic sense;the optimal strategy is to always select the alternative with thehighest positive-outcome probability, known as maximising(in decision making) or regularising (in linguistic tasks). Whilethe tendency to probability match seems to depend somewhaton the participants and the task (i.e., infants are less likelyto probability match than adults, monkeys probability matchless than humans, and probability matching is less likely inlinguistic tasks), existing studies suffer from a range of defi-ciencies which make it difficult to robustly assess these dif-ferences. In this paper we present three experiments whichsystematically test the development of probability matchingbehaviour over time in simple decision making tasks, acrossspecies (humans and Guinea baboons), task complexity, andtask domain (linguistic vs non-linguistic). In Experiments 1and 2 we show that adult humans and Guinea baboons exhibitsimilar behaviour in a non-linguistic decision-making task and,contrary to the prevailing view, a tendency to maximise (ba-boons) or significantly over-match (humans) rather than prob-ability match, which strengthens over time and more so withgreater task complexity; our non-human sample size (N = 20baboons) is unprecedented in the probability-matching litera-ture. Experiment 3 provides evidence against domain-specificprobability learning mechanisms, showing that human subjectsover-match high positive-outcome probabilities to a similar de-gree across linguistic and non-linguistic tasks. Our results sug-gest that previous studies may simply have insufficient trials toshow maximising, or be too short to show maximising strate-gies which unfold over time. We thus provide evidence ofshared probability learning mechanisms not only across lin-guistic and non-linguistic tasks but also across primate species.

Repetition Suppression in Low- and High-Order Regions of the Primate Visual Cortex

Stimulus recency has a strong effect on both behavior and neural responses. Its effects on neural responses have been most closely studied in the visual system in inferotemporal cortex (IT) in which recency gives rise to suppressed responses by a phenomenon known as repetition suppression. This observation has led to many possible explanations of how repetition suppression arises in the visual system. Here, we explore three of them: (1) top-down, (2) bottom-up and (3) independently in each brain region. Each of these accounts makes different predictions about the pattern of effects at different stages in visual processing for cases in which the stimulus either is or is not a match for the location or the identity of the preceding stimulus. We tested these predictions by recording from neurons in IT and V2, two separate stages of processing, while monkeys viewed displays of repeated and non-repeated image sequences.

“Think” and “believe” across cultures: A shared folk distinction between twocognitive attitudes in the US, Ghana, Thailand, China, and Vanuatu

Do people hold different kinds of beliefs about gods and spiritsthan they do about the everyday world? Many say no: that tothe faithful, gods and spirits are real in the same way that tablesand chairs are real. Yet experimental studies have found thatspeakers of American English tacitly distinguish between twocognitive attitudes—one for factual beliefs and one forreligious credences—through their differential use of the words“think” and “believe” (Heiphetz, Landers, and Van Leeuwen,2018). In three large-scale studies—conducted in fivestrikingly different linguistic and cultural-religious contexts(from west to east: the US, Ghana, Thailand, China, andVanuatu)—we demonstrate that such linguistic differentiationof factual belief and religious credence is cross-culturallyrobust. This lends support to the hypothesis that human theoryof mind includes nuanced distinctions among differentvarieties of “belief.”

Comparing Adaptive and Random Spacing Schedules during Learningto Mastery Criteria

Adaptive generation of spacing intervals in learning usingresponse times improves learning relative to both adaptivesystems that do not use response times and fixed spacingschemes (Mettler, Massey & Kellman, 2016). Studies haveoften used limited presentations (e.g., 4) of each learningitem. Does adaptive practice benefit learning if items arepresented until attainment of objective mastery criteria? Doesit matter if mastered items drop out of the active learning set? We compared adaptive and non-adaptive spacing underconditions of mastery and dropout. Experiment 1 comparedrandom presentation order with no dropout to adaptivespacing and mastery using the ARTS (AdaptiveResponse-time-based Sequencing) system. Adaptive spacingproduced better retention than random presentation.Experiment 2 showed clear learning advantages for adaptivespacing compared to random schedules that also includeddropout. Adaptive spacing performs better than randomschedules of practice, including when learning proceeds tomastery and items drop out when mastered.

Concepts and Systems

Prevalence-Induced Concept Change in Older Adults

Prevalence-induced concept change describes a cognitivemechanism by which someone’s definition of a concept shiftsas the prevalence of exemplars of that concept changes. Forinstance, in a task where people have to judge whether thecolour of an ambiguously-coloured dot is blue or purple, if thefrequency of objectively blue dots in the environmentdecreases, people expand their concept of blueness and judgemore dots to be blue than they did initially. In a series ofexperiments, Levari et al. (2018) demonstrated that thisphenomenon extends to higher-order decision-making, suchas ethical judgments as well. What these findings suggest isthat conceptual spaces (whether it’s about colours or ethicalstatements) in humans are not fixed, but are sensitive tochange. While Levari et al. (2018) established thisphenomenon in young adults, it is unclear how it affects olderadults: do they outsource control and become moresusceptible to concept change or are they rigid enough in theirbeliefs to be resistant to it? In the current study, we explorehow prevalence-induced concept change affects older adults’lower-level, perceptual, and higher- order, ethical,decision-making. We find that older adults are less sensitiveto prevalence-induced concept change than younger adultsacross both domains. A computational model reveals thatthese differences might in part be explained by older adults’tendency to perseverate (repeat responses). Our resultssuggest that older adults’ concept space may be less flexiblethan younger adults’ when faced with a changing world.

Nameability predicts subjective and objective measures of visual similarity

Do people perceive shapes to be similar based purely on theirphysical features? Or is visual similarity influenced by top-down knowledge? In the present studies, we demonstrate thattop-down information – in the form of verbal labels that peopleassociate with visual stimuli – predicts visual similarity asmeasured using subjective (Experiment 1) and objective(Experiment 2) tasks. In Experiment 1, shapes that werepreviously calibrated to be (putatively) perceptuallyequidistant were more likely to be grouped together if theyshared a name. In Experiment 2, more nameable shapes wereeasier for participants to discriminate from other images, againcontrolling for their perceptual distance. We discuss what theseresults mean for constructing visual stimuli spaces that areperceptually uniform and discuss theoretical implications ofthe fact that perceptual similarity is sensitive to top-downinformation such as the ease with which an object can benamed.

Separability and the Effect of ValenceAn Empirical Study of Thick Concepts

Thick terms and concepts, such as honesty and cruelty, are atthe heart of a variety of debates in linguistics, philosophy oflanguage, and metaethics. Central to these debates is thequestion of how the descriptive and evaluative components ofthick concepts are related and whether they can be separatedfrom each other. So far, no empirical data on how thick termsare used in ordinary language has been collected to informthese debates. In this paper, we present the first empiricalstudy, designed to investigate whether the evaluativecomponent of thick concepts can be separated. Our study mightbe considered to falsify the view that evaluation isconversationally implicated. However, our study also revealsan effect of valence, indicating that people reason differentlyabout positive and negative thick terms. While evaluationscannot be cancelled for negative thick terms, they can be forpositive ones. Three follow-up studies were conducted toexplain this effect. We conclude that the effect of valence isbest accounted for by a difference in the social norms guidingevaluative language.

Simple kinship systems are more learnable

Natural languages partition meanings into labelled categoriesin different ways, but this variation is constrained: languagesappear to achieve a near-optimal trade-off between simplicityand informativeness. Across 3 artificial language learning ex-periments, we verify that objectively simpler kinship systemsare easier for human participants to learn, and also show thatthe errors which occur during learning tend to increase sim-plicity while reducing informativeness. This latter result sug-gests that pressures for simplicity and informativeness operatethrough different mechanisms: learning favours simplicity, butthe pressure for informativeness must be enforced elsewhere,e.g. during language use in communicative interaction.

The Paradox of Time in Dynamic Causal Systems

Recent work has shown that people use temporal informationincluding order, delay, and variability to infer causality be-tween events. In this study we build on this work by investi-gating the role of time in dynamic systems, where causes takecontinuous values and also continually influence their effects.Recent studies of learning in these systems explored short in-teractions in a setting with comparatively rapidly evolving dy-namics and modeled people as relying on simpler, resource-limited strategies to grapple with the stream of information(Davis et al., 2020). A natural question that arises from such anaccount is whether interacting with systems that unfold moreslowly might reduce the systematic errors that result from thesestrategies. Paradoxically, we find that slowing the task indeedreduced the frequency of one type of error, but increased the er-ror rate overall. To capture the differences between conditions,we introduce a novel Causal Event Segmentation model basedon the notion that people compress the continuous scenes intoevents and use these to drive structure inference.

Biases

Bootstrap Hell: Perceptual Racial Biases in a Predictive Processing Framework

Predictive processing, or predictive coding,1 is transforming our knowledge of perception (Knill & Richards, 1996; Rao & Ballard, 1999), the brain (Friston, 2018; Hohwy, 2013; Knill & Pouget, 2004), and embodied cognition (Allen & Friston, 2018; Clark, 2016; Gallagher & Allen, 2018; Seth, 2015). Predictive processing is a hierarchical implementation of empirical Bayes, wherein the cognitive system creates generative models of the world and tests its hypotheses against incoming data. It is hierarchical insofar as the predictions at one level are tested against incoming signals from the lower level. The resulting prediction error, the difference between the expectation and the incoming data, is used to recalibrate the model in a process of prediction error minimization. Predictions may be mediated by pyramidal cells across the neocortex (Bastos et al., 2012; Hawkins & Ahmad, 2016; Shipp et al., 2013). Andy Clark has characterized predictive processing as creating a “bootstrap heaven” (2016, p. 19), enabling the brain to develop complex models of the world from limited data. This enables us to extract patterns from ambiguous signals and establish hypotheses about how the world works. The training signals that we get from the world are, however, biased in all the same unsightly ways that our societies are biased: by race, gender, socioeconomic status, nationality, and sexual orientation. The problem is more than a mere sampling bias. Our societies are replete with prejudice biases that shape the ways we think, act, and perceive. Indeed, a similar problem arises in machine learning applications when they are inadvertently trained on socially biased data (Avery, 2019; N. T. Lee, 2018). The basic principle in operation here is “garbage in, garbage out”: a predictive system that is trained on socially biased data will be systematically biased in those same ways. Unfortunately, we are unwittingly trained on this prejudiced data from our earliest years. As predictive systems, we bootstrap upwards into more complex cognitive processes while being fed prejudiced data, spiraling us into a “bootstrap hell.” This has repercussions for everything from higher-order cognitive processes down to basic perceptual processes. Perceptual racial biases include perceiving greater diversity and nuance in the faces of racial ingroup faces (the cross-race effect; Malpass & Kravitz, 1969), misperceiving actions of racial outgroup members as hostile (Pietraszewski et al., 2014), and empathetically perceiving emotions in racial ingroup (but not outgroup) faces (Xu et al., 2009), among other phenomena. They are particularly worrying due to their recalcitrance to conscious control or implicit bias training. We may be able to veto a prejudiced thought (but see Kelly & Roedder, 2008), but we cannot simply modify our perceptual experience at will. Recalcitrant predictions such as this are “hyperpriors” and are unamenable to rapid, conscious adjustment. I begin with an overview of predictive processing. I explain that the same principles that allow us to bootstrap our way into full cognition also allow for biases to develop. These biases include perceptual racial biases, which are visual and affective rather than cognitive. I explain how sampling biases in infancy and emotion perception contribute to perceptual racial biases (although many other factors certainly play a role). Finally, I hypothesize that traditional implicit bias training may not be enough to disentangle the web of hypotheses that contribute to perceptual racial bias.

Overconfident in Hindsight: Memory, Hindsight Bias and Overconfidence

Overconfidence and Hindsight Bias are two well-knowncognitive biases. Herein, it is argued these biases may berelated to one another and human memory limitations;specifically, that memory limitations result in hindsight bias,causing people to recall being right more often than theyactually were, which leads to overconfidence as people applythis misremembered confidence to future events. Analysescomparing three types of overconfidence (overestimation,overplacement and overprecision) and hindsight bias confirmstrong, positive correlations between the different types ofoverconfidence – from 0.488 up to .807 and moderatecorrelations (.331 to .398) between all of these and hindsightbias. Comparisons between bias scores and five broadcognitive abilities (from the CHC model) suggests hindsightbias is more pronounced in people with worse memories andgenerally, lower cognitive ability. Overall, results are arguedto support the proposed links between memory, hindsight biasand overconfidence and future directions are suggested.

The contingency illusion bias as a potential driver of science denial

Science denial is a pressing social problem, contributing toinactivity in the face of climate change, or to a resurgencein outbreaks of preventable diseases. Cognitive factors are asignificant driver of science denial, in addition to social fac-tors such as political ideology. Biases pertaining to judgmentsof contingency (i.e., inferring causal relationships where noneexist) have been associated with misbeliefs, such as belief inthe paranormal and conspiracy theories. Here, we examinewhether contingency biases likewise predict science denial.We show that (a) various tasks used to study relevant biases doin fact load on a single latent ‘contingency illusion’ factor; (b)this contingency illusion bias is associated with increased sci-ence denial; (c) the contingency illusion bias mediates the re-lationship between intuitive (vs. analytic) cognitive style andscience denial; and (d) this holds even when accounting forpolitical ideology.

Population-level amplification of perceptual bias

A longstanding conjecture that has been difficult to test holds that social interactions amplify the effects of people’s biases.We tested this conjecture in a perceptual decision-making paradigm. First, we formalized the algorithmic structure of de-cision making in networked crowds when individuals’ perceptions are biased by their utilities. Our analysis predicts thateven weak cognitive biases can be amplified by social interaction. We tested this prediction in a large networked behav-ioral experiment. Using a monetary incentive structure to induce a bias known as motivated perception, we manipulatedthe presence of a weak cognitive bias in social and asocial populations. Social decision making increased participants’perceptual accuracy relative to an asocial baseline. However, social decision making also led to significantly amplifiedrates of motivated perception, confirming the prediction that shared cognitive biases can be amplified in social networks.

Object Bias Disrupts Rule-Based Generalization in Adults Across Domains

Humans are remarkably adept at abstract rule learning, but little is known about when learners apply this knowledge. We investigated a fundamental constraint in rule generalization: attention to featural similarity (object bias). Across two experiments in different domains, we asked whether adults’ abstract rule generalization is constrained by superficial matches to the concrete exemplars present during learning, as is known to be the case for analogical reasoning (Gentner & Toupin, 1986). In the present studies, participants were exposed to a series of sequences following a simple rule and were asked to generalize to novel instances of either the same rule or a new rule. In one condition, an individual element present during initial learning was inserted into the new, unfamiliar pattern. Results showed that adults often chose this object match over the rule match, suggesting that abstract rule generalization, like analogical reasoning, is impacted by concrete features of the input.

Social Inference

Detecting social information in a dense database of infants natural visual experience

The faces and hands of caregivers and other social partners offer a rich source of social and causal information thatmay be critical for infants cognitive and linguistic development. Previous work using manual annotation strategies andcross-sectional data has found systematic changes in the proportion of faces and hands in the egocentric perspective ofyoung infants. Here, we examine the prevalence of faces and hands in a longitudinal collection of nearly 1700 headcamvideos collected from three children along a span of 6 to 32 months of agethe SAYCam dataset (Sullivan, Mei, Perfors,Wojcik, & Frank, under review). To analyze these naturalistic infant egocentric videos, we first validated the use of amodern convolutional neural network of pose detection (OpenPose) for the detection of faces and hands. We then appliedthis model to the entire dataset, and found a higher proportion of hands in view than previous reported and a moderatedecrease the proportion of faces in childrens view across age. In addition, we found variability in the proportion offaces/hands viewed by different children in different locations (e.g., living room vs. kitchen), suggesting that individualactivity contexts may shape the social information that infants experience.

Children use inverse planning to detect social transmission in design of artifacts

Do children use objects to infer the people and actions that created them? We ask how children judge whether designswere socially transmitted (copied), asking if children use asimple perceptual heuristic (more similar = more likelycopied), or make a rational, flexible inference (Bayesianinverse planning). We found evidence that children use inverseplanning to reason about artifacts’ designs: When children sawtwo identical designs, they did not always infer copyingoccurred. Instead, similarity was weaker evidence of copyingwhen an alternative explanation ‘explained away’ thesimilarity. Thus, children inferred copying had occurred lessoften when designs were efficient (Exp1, age 7-9; N=52), andwhen there was a constraint that limited the number of possibledesigns (Exp2, age 4-5; N=160). When thinking about artifacts,young children go beyond perceptual features and use a processlike inverse planning to reason about the generative processesinvolved in design.

Children use agents’ response time to distinguish between memory and novel inference

Psychologists frequently use response time to study cognitive processes, but response time may also be a part of the commonsense psychology that allows us to make inferences about other agents’ mental processes. We present evidence that by age six, children expect that solutions to a complex problem can be produced quickly if already memorized, but not if they need to be solved for the first time. We suggest that children could use response times to evaluate agents’ competence and expertise, as well as to assess the value and relevance of information.

Social Offloading:Just Working Together is Enough to Remove Semantic Interference

Cognitive interference is a classic cognitive phenomenon:processing one stimulus while ignoring another is morechallenging when the two are related. Recently, andsurprisingly, it has been shown that an individual’s cognitiveinterference can be removed by the people around them. In thepicture-word interference paradigm, participants respond to atarget picture and ignore distractor words. If the words aresemantically related to the target, interference slows responses.We found that this cognitive interference was removed, orsocially offloaded, when participants believed that they wereworking together with another person. In contrast to previousstudies we found it did not matter if the other person workedon the distractor words or on task irrelevant, coloured squares.Furthermore, the time course of this effect suggests that thesocial offloading of semantic interference is underpinned bylate inhibitory mechanisms rather than early distractor filtering.

Stubborn extremism as a potential pathway to group polarization

Group polarization is the widely-observed phenomenonin which the opinions held by members of a small groupbecome more extreme after the group discusses a topic.For example, conservative individuals become even moreconservative, while liberal individuals become even moreliberal. Social psychologists have offered competing ex-planations for this phenomenon. These typically re-quire questionable assumptions about human psychol-ogy. Here, we posit a more parsimonious explanation:the stubbornness of extreme opinions. Using agent-based modeling, we demonstrate that such “stubbornextremism” gives rise to group polarization, as well asother trends observed across the literature on polariza-tion. Our study revealed a further methodological prob-lem for the study of group polarization: reporting opin-ions as categories (e.g. on a Likert scale) inflates theobserved increase in opinion extremity. We concludewith a call for deeper integration of opinion dynamicsmodeling with the cognitive science of communicationand influence.

Agend-based Models

Simulating Early Word Learning in Situated Connectionist Agents

Recent advances in Deep Learning (DL) and ReinforcementLearning (RL) make it possible to train neural network agentswith raw, first-person visual perception to execute language-like instructions in 3D simulated worlds. Here, we inves-tigate the application of such deep RL agents as cognitivemodels, specifically as models of infant word learning. Wefirst develop a simple neural network-based language learningagent, trained via policy-gradient methods, which can inter-pret single-word instructions in a simulated 3D world. Tak-ing inspiration from experimental paradigms in developmentalpsychology, we run various controlled simulations with the ar-tificial agent, exploring the conditions in which established hu-man biases and learning effects emerge, and propose a novelmethod for visualising and interpreting semantic representa-tions in the agent. The results highlight the potential util-ity, and some limitations, of applying state-of-the-art learningagents and simulated environments to model human cognition.

Social Foraging in Groups of Search Agents with Human Intervention

Intelligent agents coordinate and cooperate flexibly when rules and dynamics of interaction can change over time and across different tasks and environmental conditions. Loose coupling emerges among agents when the rules of interaction are weak enough for agents to act independently or interdependently, and patterns of interaction vary as a function of conditions. Here, we examine collective foraging among simulated agents with and without human intervention. We find that loose coupling among search agents improved group foraging success, and that human players improved performance partly by subtle, indirect effects on group interactions. Analyses of movement patterns showed that loose coupling enabled collections of agents to self-organize and reorganize into a greater diversity of ad hoc groupings.

Too many cooks: Coordinating multi-agent collaboration through inverse planning

Collaboration requires agents to coordinate their behavior onthe fly, sometimes cooperating to solve a single task togetherand other times dividing it up into sub-tasks to work on in par-allel. Underlying the human ability to collaborate is theory-of-mind, the ability to infer the hidden mental states that driveothers to act. Here, we develop Bayesian Delegation, a decen-tralized multi-agent learning mechanism with these abilities.Bayesian Delegation enables agents to rapidly infer the hid-den intentions of others by inverse planning. These inferencesenable agents to flexibly decide in the absence of communi-cation when to cooperate on the same sub-task and when towork on different sub-tasks in parallel. We test this model ina suite of multi-agent Markov decision processes inspired bycooking problems. To succeed, agents must coordinate boththeir high-level plans (e.g., what sub-task they should work on)and their low-level actions (e.g., avoiding collisions). BayesianDelegation bridges these two levels and rapidly aligns agents’beliefs about who should work on what. Finally, we testedBayesian Delegation in a behavioral experiment where partici-pants made sub-task inferences from sparse observations of co-operative behavior. Bayesian Delegation outperformed heuris-tic models and was closely aligned with human judgments.

Learning in Social Environments with Curious Neural Agents

From an early age, humans are capable of learning about theirsocial environment, making predictions of how other agentswill operate and decisions about how they themselves will in-teract. In this work, we address the problem of formalizing thelearning principles underlying these abilities. We construct a cu-rious neural agent that can efficiently learn predictive models ofsocial environments that are rich with external agents inspiredby real-world animate behaviors such as peekaboo, chasing,and mimicry. Our curious neural agent consists of a controllerdriven by γ-Progress, a scalable and effective curiosity signal,and a disentangled world model that allocates separate networksfor interdependent components of the world. We show that ourdisentangled curiosity-driven agent achieves higher learning ef-ficiency and prediction performance than strong baselines. Cru-cially, we find that a preference for animate attention emergesnaturally in our model, and is a key driver of performance. Fi-nally we discuss future directions including applications of ourframework to modeling human behavior and designing earlyindicators for developmental variability.

Social Learning

A Bayesian Model of Social Influence under Risk and Uncertainty.

Humans live in an uncertain world and often rely on socialinformation in order to reduce uncertainty. However, therelationship between uncertainty and social information use is notyet fully understood. In this work we argue that previous studieshave often neglected different degrees of uncertainty that need tobe accounted for when studying social information use. Weintroduce a novel experimental paradigm to measure risky decisionmaking, wherein social information and uncertainty aremanipulated. We also developed a Bayesian model of socialinformation use. We show that across different levels ofuncertainty; social influence follows similar principles. Socialinformation is more impactful when individuals are moreuncertain. Notably, this relationship holds for experimentalmanipulations of uncertainty but also for subjective uncertaintywithin experimental conditions. We conclude with discussing thatsocial influence can be better understood when paying credit tosubjective uncertainties and preferences.

How much to copy from others?The role of partial copying in social learning

One of the major ways that people engage in adaptive problemsolving is by copying the solutions of others. Most of the workon this field has focused on three questions: when to copy, whoto copy from, and what to copy. However, how much to copyhas been relatively less explored. In the current research, weare interested in the consequences for a group when its mem-bers engage in social learning strategies with different tenden-cies to copy entire or partial solutions and different complex-ities of search problems. We also consider different networktopologies that affect the solutions visible to each member.Using a computational model of collective problem solving,we demonstrate that strategies where social learning involvespartial copying outperform strategies where individuals copyentire solutions. We analyze the exploration/exploitation dy-namics of these social learning strategies under the differentconditions.

Hatred is in the Eye of the Annotator: Hate Speech Classifiers Learn Human-LikeSocial Stereotypes

Social stereotypes impact individuals’ judgement about different social groups. One area where such stereotyping has acritical impact is in hate speech detection, in which human annotations of text are used to train machine learning models.Such models are likely to be biased in the same ways that humans are biased in their judgments of social groups. Inthis research, we investigate the effect of stereotypes of social groups on the performance of expert annotators in a largecorpus of annotated hate speech. We also examine the effect of these stereotypes on unintended bias of hate speechclassifiers. To this end, we show how language-encoded stereotypes, associated with social groups, lead to disagreementsin identifying hate speech. Lastly, we analyze how inconsistencies in annotations propagate to a supervised classifier whenhuman-generated labels are used to train a hate speech detection model.

Relative deprivation and social identity in laboratory based riots: A model

Though extreme events, riots are key signals of well-being in societies. Without understanding the psychological mecha-nisms behind them, however, it is difficult to discern the social changes that need to be made in order to reduce both theriots themselves and their underlying causes. In this work we use computational models to test both relative deprivationand social identity as explanations for data from a novel experimental framework, Parklife, which provides data on howand when individuals riot in the laboratory. Our models show that whilst norm formation and distinctiveness are importantfactors in explaining the behaviour of participants in Parklife, relative deprivation is a key and necessary mechanism inthe increase in anti-social behaviour observed in disadvantaged groups. This work offers the first direct test of relativedeprivation within a specialised system, and shows the power of computational simulations in connecting theories withdata, helping us to test hypotheses.

Givenness Hierarchy Theoretic Cognitive Status Filtering

For language-capable interactive robots to be effectively in-troduced into human society, they must be able to naturallyand efficiently communicate about the objects, locations, andpeople found in human environments. An important aspect ofnatural language communication is the use of pronouns. Ac-cording to the linguistic theory of the Givenness Hierarchy(GH), humans use pronouns due to implicit assumptions aboutthe cognitive statuses their referents have in the minds of theirconversational partners. In previous work, Williams et al. pre-sented the first computational implementation of the full GHfor the purpose of robot language understanding, leveraging aset of rules informed by the GH literature. However, that ap-proach was designed specifically for language understanding,oriented around GH-inspired memory structures used to assesswhat entities are candidate referents given a particular cogni-tive status. In contrast, language generation requires a modelin which cognitive status can be assessed for a given entity.We present and compare two such models of cognitive sta-tus: a rule-based Finite State Machine model directly informedby the GH literature and a Cognitive Status Filter designedto more flexibly handle uncertainty. The models are demon-strated and evaluated using a silver-standard English subset ofthe OFAI Multimodal Task Description Corpus.

Forms of Learning

Relation learning in a neurocomputational architecture supports cross-domaintransfer

Humans readily generalize, applying prior knowledge to novelsituations and stimuli. Advances in machine learning have be-gun to approximate and even surpass human performance, butthese systems struggle to generalize what they have learnedto untrained situations. We present a model based on well-established neurocomputational principles that demonstrateshuman-level generalisation. This model is trained to play onevideo game (Breakout) and performs one-shot generalisationto a new game (Pong) with different characteristics. The modelgeneralizes because it learns structured representations that arefunctionally symbolic (viz., a role-filler binding calculus) fromunstructured training data. It does so without feedback, andwithout requiring that structured representations are specifieda priori. Specifically, the model uses neural co-activation todiscover which characteristics of the input are invariant and tolearn relational predicates, and oscillatory regularities in net-work firing to bind predicates to arguments. To our knowledge,this is the first demonstration of human-like generalisation ina machine system that does not assume structured representa-tions to begin with.

Unconscious learning of automatic inhibition is reflected in frontal theta and sensorimotor oscillations

The cognitive control of action is thought to be mediated byconscious effort as reflected by changes in frontal theta activity. Wemeasured frontal theta during a response inhibition task in 16healthy adults who implicitly learned repeated patterns of go/switchcues, resulting in unaware differences in cognitive demand fordifferent cues. Learning was reflected by reduced reaction times(RT) to probable compared to unexpected switch cues. In the rareabsence of behavioural (RT) differences, concurrent measures ofpupil diameter revealed changes in effort with stimulus probability,while effort was accompanied by parametric increases intheta. Additionally, theta predicted pre-response sensorimotorgamma, suggesting interactions between frontal and sensorimotorcortex during cognitive control. These results provide furtherevidence for a functional role of theta in cognitive effort duringresponse preparation, inhibition and execution, even in the absenceof conscious awareness.

Metacognition and Motivation: The Role of Time-Awarenessin Preparation for Future Learning

In this work, we investigate how two factors, metacognitiveskills and motivation, would impact student learning acrossdomains. More specifically, our primary goal is to identify thecritical, yet robust, interaction patterns of these two factors thatwould contribute to students’ performance in learning logicfirst and then their performance on a subsequent new domain,probability. We are concerned with two types of metacognitiveskills: strategy-awareness and time-awareness, that is, whichproblem-solving strategy to use and when to use it. Our datawere collected from 495 participants across three consecutivesemesters, and our results show that the only students who con-sistently outperform their peers across both domains are thosewho are not only highly motivated but also strategy-aware andtime-aware.

Can I get your (robot) attention? Human sensitivity to subtle hints of human-likeness in a humanoid robot’s behavior

Designing artificial agents that can closely imitate human behavior, might influence humans in perceiving them as intentional agents. Nonetheless, the factors that are crucial for an artificial agent to be perceived as an animated and anthropomorphic being still need to be addressed. In the current study, we investigated some of the factors that might affect the perception of a robot's behavior as human-like or intentional. To meet this aim, seventy-nine participants were exposed to two different behaviors of a humanoid robot under two different instructions. Before the experiment, participants' biases towards robotics as well as their personality traits were assessed. Our results suggest that participants’ sensitivity to human-likeness relies more on their expectations rather than on perceptual cues.

Exploratory play, rational action, and efficient search

Play is a universal behavior widely held to be critical for learning and development. Recent studies suggest children’sexploratory play is consistent with formal accounts of learning. This ”play as rational exploration” view suggests thatchildren’s play is sensitive to costs, rewards, and expected information gain. By contrast, here we suggest that a definingfeature of human play is that children subvert normal utility functions in play, setting up problems where they incurneedless costs to achieve arbitrary rewards. Across three studies, we show that 4-5-year-old children not only infer playfulbehavior from observed violations of rational action (Experiment 1), but themselves take on unnecessary costs and performinefficient actions during play, despite acting efficiently in non-playful, instrumental contexts (Experiments 2-3). We endby discussing the value of apparently utility-violating behavior and why it might serve learning in the long run.

Reasoning

Morality justifies motivated reasoning

A great deal of work argues that people demand impartial,evidence-based reasoning from others. However, recentfindings show that moral values occupy a cardinal position inpeople’s evaluation of others, raising the possibility that peoplesometimes prescribe morally-good but evidentially-poorbeliefs. We report two studies investigating how peopleevaluate beliefs when these two ideals conflict and find thatpeople regularly endorse motivated reasoning when it can bemorally justified. Furthermore, we document two ways thatmoral considerations result in prescribed motivated reasoning.First, morality can provide an alternative justification forbelief, leading people to prescribe evidentially unsupportedbeliefs to others. And, second, morality can affect how peopleevaluate the way evidence is weighed by lowering or raisingthe threshold of required evidence for morally good and badbeliefs, respectively. These results illuminate longstandingquestions about the nature of motivated reasoning and thesocial regulation of belief.

Improving Cognitive Models for Syllogistic Reasoning

Multiple cognitive theories make conflicting explainations forhuman reasoning on syllogistic problems. The evaluation andcomparison of these theories can be performed by conceiv-ing them as predictive models. Model evaluation often em-ploys static sets of predictions rather than full implementationsof the theories. However, most theories predict different re-sponses depending on the state of their internal parameters.Disregarding the theories’ capabilities to adapt parameters todifferent reasoners leads to an incomplete picture of their pre-dictive power. This article provides parameterized algorithmicformalizations and implementations of some syllogistic theo-ries regarding the syllogistic single-response task. Evaluationsreveal a substantial improvement for most cognitive theoriesbeing made adaptive over their original static predictions. Thebest performing implementations are PHM, mReasoner andVerbal Models, which almost reach the MFA benchmark. Theresults show that there exist heuristic and model-based theo-ries which are able to capture a large portion of the patterns insyllogistic reasoning data.

Incremental Hypothesis Revision in Causal Reasoning Across Development

We explore whether children’s strategies on a causal learningtask show a bias observed in adults towards “exploitative” hy-pothesis revision. Adults and children (ages 4–6) were pre-sented with evidence which initially seemed to conform to asimple, salient rule (e.g. blue blocks activate a machine), butthen encountered evidence that violated this rule. The truerule in the “near” condition was more complex, but could bereached through iterative revision of the salient rule, while inthe “distant” condition, the true rule was comparatively sim-ple, but incremental revision could not yield the true rule. Par-ticipants then predicted the behaviour of a set of new blocks.Adults performed better in the near condition, while in the dis-tant condition adults did not appear to revise their initial hy-pothesis significantly. Unlike adults, children’s overall perfor-mance in both conditions was similar, while condition differ-ences may reflect a broader search for alternative solutions.

A New Approach to Testimonial Conditionals

Conditionals pervade every aspect of our thinking, from themundane and everyday such as ‘if you eat too much cheese,you will have nightmares’ to the most fundamental concernsas in ‘if global warming isn’t halted, sea levels will rise dra-matically’. Many decades of research have focussed on thesemantics of conditionals and how people reason from condi-tionals in everyday life. Here it has been rather overlookedhow we come to such conditionals in the first place. In manycases, they are learned through testimony: someone warns usabout the ill-effects of cheese. Any full account of the condi-tional must consequently incorporate such learning. Here, weprovide a new formal account of belief change in response to atestimonial conditional.

. . . that P is relevant for Q:Indicative conditionals and learning from testimony

Our beliefs change with learning, and much of what we learncomes from the testimony of other people. How much our be-liefs change may depend on how many people are the sourcesof a given piece of information, and how reliable their expertisemakes them. It is not clear, however, what exactly the effectsof reliability or number of speakers will be when the testimonyhas the form of an indicative conditional. Here, we test the hy-pothesis that learning a conditional amounts to increasing thedegree to which the antecedent of that conditional is relevantfor its consequent. Furthermore, we investigate whether this isaffected by number of speakers and by their expertise.

Pragmatics

Learning to refer informatively by amortizing pragmatic reasoning

A hallmark of human language is the ability to effectively andefficiently convey contextually relevant information. One the-ory for how humans reason about language is presented in theRational Speech Acts (RSA) framework, which captures prag-matic phenomena via a process of recursive social reasoning(Goodman & Frank, 2016). However, RSA represents idealreasoning in an unconstrained setting. We explore the idea thatspeakers might learn to amortize the cost of RSA computationover time by directly optimizing for successful communicationwith an internal listener model. In simulations with groundedneural speakers and listeners across two communication gamedatasets representing synthetic and human-generated data, wefind that our amortized model is able to quickly generate lan-guage that is effective and concise across a range of contexts,without the need for explicit pragmatic reasoning.

Are you thinking what I’m thinking?Perspective-taking in a language game

Many theories of communication claim that perspective-taking is afundamental component of the successful design of utterances for aspecific audience. We investigated perspective-taking in aconstrained communication situation: Participants played a wordguessing game where each trial required them to communicate atarget word without context. In each game, pairs of participants tookturns giving and receiving clues to guess target words, bothreceiving feedback after each trial. In Experiment 1, none of themeasures of participants’ performance improved over rounds,suggesting either that participants were unable to improve theirperspective-taking or that the task was simply too demanding forother reasons. In Experiment 2, we tested whether this lack ofimprovement was due to overall difficulty rather than inability totake perspective. While the success rate in Experiment 2 didimprove over the course of the game, our analyses indicated that theimprovement was due to participants discovering a frequencyheuristic (using rarer clue words) rather than improved perspective-taking per se. The results of these two experiments show thatimproving perspective-taking adaptively is very difficult when thereis no context to ground either signal choice or interpretation.

Visual grouping and pragmatic constraints in thegeneration of quantified descriptions

Studies suggest that people use the least possible effort to gen-erate natural language descriptions of sets of objects. Thismeans that they base descriptions on what is perceptually avail-able to them. For instance, people can subitize, i.e., rapidlyassess the exact quantity of small numbers of objects, so whenthe quantity of objects in the visual scene is beneath this thresh-old, they give numeric descriptions; when the quantity is abovethis threshold, they generate non-numeric descriptions. How-ever, no research examines how people describe visual scenesof items in groups. As such, it is unclear how people will formdescriptions of scenes that contain a large total number of itemsin groups. We report on a novel experiment designed to in-vestigate how people produce quantified descriptions of scenescomposed of salient visual groups. The results corroborate theleast effort hypothesis, and suggest that people’s incrementalperception of quantity drives their descriptions.

Recursive Adversarial Reasoning in the Rock, Paper, Scissors Game

In this study, we investigate people’s ability to predict andadapt to the behavior of others in order to make plans of theirown, a cornerstone of cooperative and competitive behavior.Participants played 300 rounds of rock, paper, scissors againstanother human player. We investigate the degree to which par-ticipants are able to identify patterns in their opponent’s be-havior in order to exploit them in subsequent rounds. We findstrong evidence that participants exploit their opponents overthe course of 300 rounds, suggesting that people identify de-pendencies in their opponent’s move choices during the game.Nonetheless, analysis of dependencies across participant movechoices reveals that people exhibit a number of regularities intheir own moves. Based on these dependencies, we argue thatparticipants are far from optimal in their exploiting, suggestingthat there are substantial constraints on people’s ability to iden-tify and adapt to patterned opponent behavior across repeatedinteractions.

Parents scaffold the formation of conversational pacts with their children

Adults readily form pacts, or temporary agreements about ref-erent names, over the course of conversation. Young childrenfail to do so with peers, but recent evidence suggests that ex-plicit feedback from adults may improve their performance(Matthews, Lieven, & Tomasello, 2007). Do parents natu-rally provide such structure in their conversations with chil-dren? Using a director-matcher paradigm, we first show thatparents and children (ages 4, 6, 8) converge on increasinglyaccurate and efficient conversational pacts. Further, parents ofyounger children provide more interactive feedback. Finally,we analyze asymmetries in parents’ and children’s contribu-tions, finding that pacts tend to originate with the parent, butare simplified by younger children. Together, these resultssupport the idea that parents sensitively adapt their languageto their children’s developmental level to scaffold successfulcommunication.

Word Learning

A Computational Model of Early Word Learning from the Infant’s Point of View

Human infants have the remarkable ability to learn the asso-ciations between object names and visual objects from inher-ently ambiguous experiences. Researchers in cognitive scienceand developmental psychology have built formal models thatimplement in-principle learning algorithms, and then used pre-selected and pre-cleaned datasets to test the abilities of the mod-els to find statistical regularities in the input data. In contrast toprevious modeling approaches, the present study used egocen-tric video and gaze data collected from infant learners duringnatural toy play with their parents. This allowed us to capturethe learning environment from the perspective of the learner’sown point of view. We then used a Convolutional Neural Net-work (CNN) model to process sensory data from the infant’spoint of view and learn name-object associations from scratch.As the first model that takes raw egocentric video to simulateinfant word learning, the present study provides a proof of prin-ciple that the problem of early word learning can be solved,using actual visual data perceived by infant learners. More-over, we conducted simulation experiments to systematicallydetermine how visual, perceptual, and attentional properties ofinfants’ sensory experiences may affect word learning.

Decoding Eye Movements in Cross-Situational Word Learning via TensorComponent Analysis

Statistical learning is an active process wherein information isactively selected from the learning environment. As currentinformation is integrated with existing knowledge, it shapesattention in subsequent learning, placing biases on which newinformation will be sampled. One statistical learning task thathas been studied recently is cross-situational word learning(CSL). In CSL, statistical learners are able to learn the cor-rect mappings between novel visual objects and spoken labelsafter watching sequences where the two are paired togetherin referentially ambiguous contexts. In the present paper, weuse a computational method called Tensor Component Analy-sis (TCA) to analyze real-time gaze data collected from a set ofCSL studies. We applied TCA to learners’ gaze data in orderto derive latent variables related to real-time statistical learningand to examine how selective attention is organized in time.Our method allows us to address two specific questions: a) thesimilarity in attention behavior across strong vs. weak learn-ers as well as across learned vs. not-learned items and b) howthe structure of attention relates to word learning. We mea-sured learners’ knowledge of label-object pairs at the end of atraining session, and show that their real-time gaze data can beused to predict item-level learning outcomes as well as decodepretrained item knowledge.

Child-directed speech: the impact of variations in speaking rate on word learning

This study investigated how caregivers modulate their speaking rate according to children’s lexical knowledge and the context of the interaction, and how such adjustments affect children’s word learning. We studied a semi- naturalistic corpus where caregivers talked about different toys with their 3-4 years old children. The toys were known or unknown to the child, and present or absent from the environment. We found that caregivers talked about unknown toys with a slower speaking rate than known ones. When toys were absent, caregivers also tended to slow down for the toy’s name, although they produced the whole utterance faster. Crucially, the results of a subsequent recognition task for children showed that caregivers’ greater adjustment in speaking rate between known and unknown words predicted better immediate learning. Our findings suggest that caregivers modify their speaking rate in a helpful manner when the situation is more demanding, which assists children in word learnin

Active Word Learning through Self-supervision

Models of cross-situational word learning typically character-ize the learner as a passive observer, but a language learn-ing child can actively participate in verbal and non-verbalcommunication. We present a computational study of cross-situational word learning to investigate whether a curious wordlearner who actively influences linguistic input in each contexthas an advantage over a passive learner. Our computationalmodel learns to map words to objects in real images by self-supervision through simulating both word comprehension andproduction. We examine different curiosity measures as guid-ing input selection, and analyze the relative impact of eachmethod. Our results suggest that active learning leads to higheroverall performance, and a formulation of curiosity which re-lies both on subjective novelty and plasticity yields the bestperformance and learning stability.

Predicting Age of Acquisition in Early Word Learning Using Recurrent NeuralNetworks

Vocabulary growth and syntactic development are known tobe highly correlated in early child language. What determineswhen words are acquired and how can this help us understandwhat drives early language development? We train an LSTMlanguage model, known to detect syntactic regularities that arerelevant for predicting the difficulty of words, on child-directedspeech. We use the average surprisal of words for the model,which encodes sequential predictability, as a predictor for theage of acquisition of words in early child language. We com-pare this predictor to word frequency and others and find thataverage surprisal is a good predictor for the age of acquisitionof function words and predicates beyond frequency, but notfor nouns. Our approach provides insight into what makes agood model of early word learning, especially for words whosemeanings rely heavily on linguistic context.

Attention and Faces

Modeling temporal attention in dynamic scenes: Hypothesis-driven resourceallocation using adaptive computation explains both objective trackingperformance and subjective effort judgments

Most work on attention (in terms of both psychophysical experiments and computational modeling) involves selection instatic scenes. And even when dynamic displays are used, performance is still typically characterized with only a singlevariable (such as the number of items correctly tracked in Multiple Object Tracking; MOT). But the allocation of attentionin daily life (e.g. during foraging, navigation, or play) involves both objective performance and subjective effort, and canvary dramatically from moment to moment. Here we attempt to capture this sort of rich temporal ebb and flow of attentionin a novel and generalizable adaptive computation architecture. In this architecture, computing resources are dynamicallyallocated to perform partial belief updates over both objects (in space) and moments (in time) flexibly and according totask demands. During MOT this framework is able to explain both objective tracking performance and the subjective senseof trial-by-trial effort.

Foraging in the Virtual Himalayas: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Factors in Search

Foraging over land for resources was central to the evolutionof search processes and decision-making for many organisms,including humans. The processes underlying natural foragingbehaviors are foundational to cognition. However, in the field,it is difficult to collect detailed and accurate measures of searchbehaviors and hard to manipulate search conditions. We usedGoogle Earth and the Unity 3D platform to recreate a patchof the Himalayan foothills with ancient temples used as way-points for travelers on foot. Two hundred players recruited viaMTurk moved over the landscape with realistic speed, energyusage, and perceptual conditions to find as many temples aspossible given a limited energy budget. Half were constrainedby the need to return to a home base to report found temples,and half were not. When search paths were analyzed in termsof segment distributions, players who found relatively moretemples (high scorers) more closely followed the theoreticallyoptimal L ́evy walk that balances exploration and exploitation,regardless of the home base. This intrinsic pattern was alsofound in perceptual search intervals, with high scorers lean-ing more towards exploration. By contrast, when search pathswere analyzed as wholes, an extrinsic pattern was found in thatplayers ranged farther without a home base, and this differ-ence was more pronounced for high scorers. We conclude thatL ́evy-like patterns are intrinsic and effective in terms of pathsegments and perceptual intervals, but overall search behavioradapts to extrinsic factors and constraints.

Face Selectivity in Social (But Not Perceptual) Areas of the Infant Brain

Humans are profoundly social creatures. We depend on others for survival and crave social interactions. Faces are thegateway to many typical social interactions. One of the most replicable results in cognitive neuroscience is the selectiveresponse of some cortical regions to faces in humans and other social primates. Specifically, the fusiform face area (FFA)is a region in the ventral temporal cortex (VTC) that is selectively responsive to faces. To determine whether infantsshow early cortical responses to faces, we recruited 86 human infants (2.1-11.9 months) to participate in an awake infantfunctional resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment. We obtained usable fMRI data from 49 infants (2.1-9.7 months) whilethey watched videos of faces, bodies, objects, and scenes, 30 of whom (2.5-9.4 months) had enough data for a functionalregion of interest (fROI) analysis. A group random effects analysis revealed significantly higher responses to faces thanobjects in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), superior temporal sulcus (STS), ventral temporal cortex (VTC), andsubcortical areas of the infant brain. Additionally, the fROI analysis revealed face selective responses in the STS, MPFC,and VTC but not lateral occipital cortex or subcortical areas of the infant brain. Thus, we provide the first evidence offace selective responses in the infant brain and demonstrate that social regions (STS and MPFC) respond selectively at thesame time as perceptual regions (VTC).

Poster Session 1

Tracing the emergence of gendered language in childhood

Are gender associations in general language reflected in thewords spoken to and by children? Previous work has sug-gested that language reveals gender differences in discourse,speech style, language use and acquisition. Work in artificialintelligence has shown that word embeddings trained on largecorpora reflect human gender associations. We connect thiswork to developmental psychology by exploring whether gen-der associations in word embeddings are present in the linguis-tic input and output of children, and if so, how early genderedlanguage emerges. We present a computational method thatquantifies the gender associations of words and use a corpus ofchild-caretaker speech to show that these gender associationscorrelate significantly with those in word embeddings. Wediscover that gendered word use emerges in English-speakingchildren around age 2, and the gender associations cannotbe explained solely by variables including word length, fre-quency, concreteness, and valence.

Self-Other Similarity Modulates the Socially-Triggered Context-Based PredictionError Effect on Memory

The mind is a prediction machine, using prior experiences and current information to constantly make predictions aboutthe future. This feature of the cognitive system has numerous consequences for long-term memory. Here, we are interestedin the effects these predictions have on memory when invalidated (i.e., prediction errors) during social interactions witheither similar or dissimilar social sources. We designed an experiment in which both similar and dissimilar social sources(speakers) recounted experiences similar with those of the listeners but with a different outcome than those of the listeners.We measured participants memory for both their own and the two social sources experiences. In two experiments, we foundthat context-based prediction errors triggered during social interactions dont affect the listeners memory for their ownexperiences but decrease the listeners memory for the similar speakers experiences compared to the dissimilar speakersexperiences. This finding has important implications for close relationships.

A Meta-Analytic Review of Verbal Overshadowing Effect on Insight ProblemSolving Using Bayes Factors

There has been a debate on the process of insight problem solving. The special-process view posits that insight problemsolving processes are implicit unlike non-insight problem solving. The business-as-usual view, on the other hand, assumesthat the same processes as non-insight problem solving are involved in insight problem solving. To reconcile them, we canrely on the evidence on the verbal overshadowing effect on insight problem solving. However, there is a methodologicalproblem on how to determine whether the verbal overshadowing effect has emerged. The purpose of the present studywas to solve the problem using Bayes Factors. We reanalyzed the data presented in the previous studies examining theeffects of verbalization on insight problem solving. The results showed that some studies inappropriately concluded thatthe verbal overshadowing effect was not obtained. We also discussed possible moderating variables of the effect.

Covert attention shift by Sequence-space synesthesia (SSS): a cognitive grammarapproach

Some people experience sequences like numbers allocated to a specific part of space which is well-known as sequencespace synesthesia. On the other hand, covert attention is orienting of attention without the head, eyes, or body movementis the (mental) moving of attention toward a stimulus. Here, We used previous findings in sequence space synesthesia byusing an auditory number sequence numbers and covert spatial attention together with cognitive grammar theory includingprofiling to assess the possibility to shift covert attention towards a specific part of a bistable picture. Our participants were14 years old adolescents learning English at the pre-intermediate level in a school in Tehran which went through within-subject experiment. Results showed shorter reaction time for a sentence with trajectory congruent with covert attended partof the bistable picture compared to the condition without such attentional shift by t value as -4.466 within 95% confidenceinterval.

When and how do toddlers in rural Western Kenya understand the referentialnature of pictures?

Humans possess a remarkable capacity to create and understand abstract representations, such as pictures. U.S. 18-month-olds understand that pictures refer to objects; however, less is known about how this understanding develops. We testthe hypothesis that understanding the representational nature of pictures requires frequent experience with pictures, byworking with rural Kenyan toddlers with few visual symbols in their early environments.We taught rural Kenyan toddlers a novel word (dax) for a picture of a novel object. We then presented the picture and theobject to toddlers and asked them to point to the dax, reasoning that toddlers would select the object if they understoodthat pictures are representations of objects.Surprisingly, only half the sample learned the novel word. Moreover, the toddlers who learned the word selected be-tween the picture and object randomly. We discuss follow-up studies to continue exploring the development of pictorialcompetence across early environments.

Improving Multi-Agent Cooperation using Theory of Mind

Recent advances in Artificial Intelligence have produced agents that can beat human world champions at games like Go,Starcraft, and Dota2. However, most of these models do not seem to play in a human-like manner: People infer others’intentions from their behaviour, and use these inferences in scheming and strategizing. Here, using a Bayesian Theory ofMind (ToM) approach, we investigated how much an explicit representation of others’ intentions improves performancein a cooperative game. We compared the performance of humans playing with optimal-planning agents with and withoutToM, in a cooperative game where players have to flexibly cooperate to achieve joint goals. We find that teams with ToMagents significantly outperform non-ToM agents when collaborating with all types of partners: non-ToM, ToM, as well ashuman players, and that the benefit of ToM increases the more ToM agents there are. These findings have implications fordesigning better cooperative agents.

Restricted Access to Working Memory Does Not Prevent CumulativeImprovement in a Cultural Evolution Task.

Some theories propose that human cumulative culture is dependent on System 2 cognitive processes. We aimed to restrictaccess to adults executive functions via a dual-task paradigm, to assess whether this reduced their ability to improveupon information provided by a computer model. 206 participants completed a grid-search task in conjunction with aworking-memory task and a matched control, with the aim of outperforming an example attempt observed vicariously,presented on the computer. Participants behaviour was then used to simulate the outcome if the task was iterated overmultiple generations. Simulations run using the data showed that, across all conditions, participant behaviour would leadto cumulatively increasing scores over successive generations. However, scores plateaued without reaching the maximum.Overall, the task did not provide clear evidence that working-memory directly facilitates cumulative cultural evolution.However, differences between conditions may have been masked by offloading task demands to the concurrent working-memory task.

Is She a Good Teacher? Children Learn to use Meaningful Gesture as a Marker of aGood Informant

To learn from others, children rely on cues (e.g., familiarity) toinfer who will provide useful information. We extend thisresearch to ask whether children will use an informant’sinclination to gesture as a marker of whether they are a goodperson to learn from. Children (N=459, ages 4-12 years)watched videos in which actresses made statementsaccompanied by meaningful iconic gestures, beat gestures, orno gestures. After each trial, children were asked “Who do youthink would be a good teacher?” (good teacher- experimentalcondition) or “Who do you think would be a good friend?”(good friend-control condition). Results show children dobelieve that someone who produces iconic gesture would makea good teacher over someone who does not, but this is only laterin childhood and only if a child has the propensity to seegesture as meaningful. The same effects were not found in thegood-friend condition.

Beyond Pattern Completion with Short-Term Plasticity

In a Linear Associative Net (LAN), all input settles to a singlepattern, therefore Anderson, Silverstein, Ritz, and Jones (1977)introduced saturation to force the system to reach othersteady-states in the Brain-State-in-a-Box (BSB). Unfortunately,the BSB is limited in its ability to generalize because itsresponses are restricted to previously stored patterns. We presentsimulations showing how a Dynamic-Eigen-Net (DEN), a LANwith Short-Term Plasticity (STP), overcomes thesingle-response limitation. Critically, a DEN also accommodatesnovel patterns by aligning them with encoded structure. We traina two-slot DEN on a text corpus, and provide an account oflexical decision and judgement-of-grammaticality (JOG) tasksshowing how grammatical bi-grams yield stronger responsesrelative to ungrammatical bi-grams. Finally, we present asimulation showing how a DEN is sensitive to syntacticviolations introduced in novel bi-grams. We propose DENs asassociative nets with greater promise for generalization than theclassic alternatives.

Does Children’s Visual Attention to Objects Influence their Verb Learning?

Children benefit from comparing events when learning verbs, but it is unclear whether variability across events is helpfulor harmful. Additionally, no prior study has tested childrens visual attention to specific objects under different variabilityconditions. A Tobii x30-120 tracked 21/2-year-olds(n=36) and 31/2-year-olds(n=34) visual attention as they watchedevents that showed no change (control), events with varied tools (Tool condition) or events with varied affected objects(Affected Object condition) when learning a verb. Children pointed to one of two new events at test; repeated for two moreverbs. Results showed children could extend the verbs, but were more successful with age. Analyses of looking patternsin the learning phase show childrens attention to specific objects varied by condition, and that reduced looking to the toolwas linked to less success at test. Results are important to better understand processes that underlie verb learning, andlanguage development as a whole.

Model gender influences emotion categorization

Perceivers view facial configurations as belonging to emotion categories, though the features of facial cues to emotionvary continuously. Little is understood about what factors beyond facial musculature influence these categorizations. Weinvestigated how an emoters gender influences how emotional cues are perceived. Eighty-four adults categorized morphedemotional faces of male and female models sampled from a neutral-angry continuum. Participants had a lower thresholdfor categorizing female faces as upset (X2=16.618, p¡.001), particularly for configurations that were closer to the angryend of the continuum. Even when provided explicit feedback on their responses, participants continued to be more likelyto identify a face as angry for female, as compared to male, models (X2=11.561, p¡.001). Therefore, judgments of emotionwere influenced by both the emotional cues displayed by a model and also the models identity. These results highlighthow the social context influences how individuals readand therefore respond toanger.

A Large-Scale Analysis of Attentional Deployment across One Hundred Sessions of Adaptive Multitask Training

Human cognition is routinely challenged by today’s multitasking demands which require continuous attentional deployment to multiple task components in parallel. While practice-based multitasking training has been shown to improve multitasking performance, little is known about how attention should be best deployed for optimal training. To this end, we leveraged a large-scale dataset from an online cognitive-training platform to investigate individual differences in task learning across long-term training. We developed an index of attentional deployment that specifies the temporal dynamics of learning for each component of the multitask and calculate distance maps between clusters of users to specify distinct learning styles. While long-term practice improved the multitasking performance of all participant groups, participants who focused on learning one task component earlier and more emphatically, benefited from superior learning gains throughout the entirety of training.

I don’t know if you did it, but I know why: A ‘motive’ preference at multiple stagesof the legal-investigative process

What makes an explanation satisfying? Much work hasinvestigated explanatory preferences for things like animalsand artifacts, but how do explanation preferences manifest ineveryday life? Here, we focus on the criminal justice system asa case study. In this domain, outcomes critically depend on howmembers of the system (e.g., lawyers, jurors) generate andinterpret explanations. We investigate lay preferences for twodifferent classes of explanations: those that appeal to‘mechanistic’ aspects of a crime (i.e., how the culpritcommitted the crime) vs. ‘teleological’ aspects of that crime(i.e., the purpose of the crime). In two experiments, wedemonstrate that people have a systematic preference for'motive' accounts of crimes (analogous to a teleologypreference) at different stages of the investigative process. Wediscuss these findings in light of a broad literature on thecognitive basis of explanation preferences. We also discussimplications for the criminal justice system.

An evidence accumulation model of motivational and developmental influencesover sustained attention

Sustaining focus is difficult, but it is under our control.Previous research has found that people’s ability to sustainattention depends on external incentives and changes overthe lifespan. However, previous research has made limitedprogress in characterizing the specific cognitive mechanismsinvolved in sustained attention. These mechanisms areinvestigated in the current experiment, which uses driftdiffusion modeling to re-analyze a series experiments onsustained attention. In Experiment 1, we found that incentivesinfluence information processing (noise) but not decisionstrategy (threshold). In Experiment 2, we found that noiseand threshold have distinct development trajectories, andthat while older adults have noisier accumulation, they arebetter at sustaining attention. These results help providemechanistic insight into recent findings in sustained attention.

Role of Working Memory in Language Activation during Visual Scene Processing

The current study examined the role of working memory in language activation during visual processing. Twenty-sixnative English speakers searched for a visual target while completing a concurrent linguistic memory task, a concurrentspatial memory task, or in the absence of dual-task demands. Linguistic activation was measured by comparing visualfixations to phonologically-overlapping items and control items whose names did not overlap with the target. Participantsexperienced significant phonological competition across all conditions, but memory load impacted the timing of competitorco-activation (delayed and more sustained under spatial load), as well as the magnitude (attenuated under both spatial andlinguistic loads) compared to the no-load condition. We conclude that linguistic representations are accessed duringvisual search even with concurrent cognitive loads, but that working memory influences the degree of language-basedcompetition, possibly by modulating the activation and maintenance of linguistic and spatial information.

Limitations of Statistical Learning: the Case of Paradigmatic Relations

Extensive statistical learning literature suggests that regularities between co-occurring items can be learned implicitly.However, little is known whether higher-order statistics, such as paradigmatic relations, can be learned implicitly. Paradig-matic relations link items that may not co-occur but share each others patterns of co-occurrence. For example, A and Bare paradigmatically related when they both co-occur with C (e.g., A-C, B-C). Therefore, paradigmatic relations could,in principle, be implicitly learned through co-occurrence regularities. Here, we modified a contextual cueing task, wheresome of the targets independently appeared within distractors that had the same spatial configuration. We found that onlyparticipants who noticed the repeating distractor patterns tend to learn the paradigmatic relations, while most participantswere able to learn the simple co-occurring regularities. Our findings imply that the ability to learn simple co-occurrenceregularities is not sufficient for forming paradigmatic relations and that explicit attention may be critical.

Pretend Play and Childrens Self-Regulation and Language Skills: AnInterventional Study

The impact of play on childrens cognitive skills has gained interest lately. This study examines the efficacy of a pre-tend play intervention on the self-regulation and language skills of four- to five-year-olds with English as an additionallanguage. During pretend play an individual uses ones imagination to project a mental representation onto reality. Thesample included 151 children who were randomized into three groups: (a) Pretend play; (b) Art activities; (c) Typicalcurriculum. The intervention included sixteen 30-minute sessions in groups of six children. The design of the pretendplay intervention is based on storybook reading with an adult, and subsequent role-playing with props related to the story.During storybook reading explicit phonological awareness and vocabulary instruction were provided for the target words.In terms of the results, the children in the pretend play group had significantly higher post-test phonological awarenessscores than children who were exposed to typical curriculum.

Gaze behavior in a review-a-definition task

A requirement definition document (RDD) in software development should define the necessary and sufficient conditionfor the software to satisfy. It is preferable to review and guarantee the quality of the RDD. It is, however, not easyto evaluate the goodness of the reviewer, due to various review styles and the logical complexity of such a document.Therefore, we developed a test set for the review task of the RDD and investigated the reviewers gaze behavior. The testset includes the four logical relationships between the definition and instances, and our analysis revealed that validation ofthe necessary condition is relatively easier than validation of the sufficient condition. Moreover, reviewers gaze patternswere concentrated more on a certain part of sentences when the review was successful. It may suggest that the reviewsuccess can be predicted by the reviewing eye gaze fixations on sentences with the relatively higher information gain.

Testing the immediate effects of transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) onface recognition skills.

In the present study, we tested the effects of anodal tDCS deliveredover the Fp3 (for 10mins at 1.5mA) on the face inversion effect(better recognition for upright vs inverted faces) while participantsperformed an old/new recognition task. We recruited three groupsof participants (n=72) and randomly assigned them toexperimental conditions. In the anodal Study Phase conditionparticipants received the tDCS stimulation during the learningphase only. In the anodal Recognition Phase condition,participants received the anodal stimulation during the recognitiontask only. In the control group participants received shamstimulation (during the study or recognition phase). Consistentwith previous research, the results showed that anodal stimulationreduced the inversion effect by impairing recognition of uprightfaces. Critically, in both anodal conditions the inversion effect wassignificantly reduced compared to sham, and no difference wasfound between the two anodal conditions. Upright faces in eachanodal condition were recognized significantly worse than sham.This suggests that the tDCS-induced effects on face recognitionare immediate and affect both learning and performance. Weinterpret the results based on the account of perceptual learningand previous work on tDCS and the inversion effect.

Auditory, Visual, and Speech Category Learning in the Same Individuals

Category learning is a fundamental process in human cognition. Recent efforts have attempted to adapt theories developedin vision to the auditory domain. However, no study has directly compared auditory and visual category learning in thesame individuals. Using a fully within-subjects approach, we trained participants on non-speech auditory, visual, andnon-native speech categories in a single day. By comparing category learning behavior, the ability to generalize to novelcategory exemplars, and leveraging decision bound computational models, we found that while individuals demonstratedsimilar learning across the auditory and visual modalities, there were distinct perceptual biases that influenced learning ofnon-speech auditory categories. Further, there were substantial individual differences in performance across the three tasks.This study presents a novel comparison of category learning across modalities in the same individuals and demonstratesthat although commonalities exist, there is some domain-specificity to category learning.

Partner-specific adaptation in disfluency processing

Disfluency leads listeners to expect an upcoming reference tounfamiliar objects. In two experiments, we examined if thisexpectation is adapted based on the way disfluency has beenused in the discourse. Participants listened to instructions tolook at an object on a screen containing familiar and novelimages. We manipulated the co-occurrence of disfluency andreference to novel vs. familiar objects. In the predictivecondition, disfluent expressions referred to novel objects, andfluent expressions referred to familiar objects. In the non-predictive condition, fluent and disfluent trials referred toeither familiar or novel objects. Participants’ gaze revealed thatlisteners more readily predicted familiar images for fluent trialsand novel images for disfluent trials in the predictive conditionthan in the non-predictive condition. Listeners adapted theirexpectations about upcoming words based on recentexperience with disfluency. Disfluency is not invariablyprocessed, but is a cue adapted within the local context.

Inverse Rendering Best Explains Face Perception Under Extreme Illuminations

Humans can successfully interpret images even when they have been distorted by significant image transformations. Suchimages could aid in differentiating proposed computational architectures for perception because while all proposals predictsimilar results for typical stimuli (good performance), they differ when confronting atypical stimuli. Here we study twoclasses of degraded stimuli – Mooney faces and silhouettes of faces – as well as typical faces, in humans and severalcomputational models, with the goal of identifying divergent predictions among the models, evaluating against humanjudgments, and ultimately informing models of human perception. We find that our top-down inverse rendering modelbetter matches human percepts than either an invariance-based account implemented in a deep neural network, or a neuralnetwork trained to perform approximate inverse rendering in a feedforward circuit.

Great expectations: Evidence for graded prediction of grammatical gender

Language processing is predictive in nature. But how do people balance multiple competing options as they predict upcoming meanings? Here, we investigated whether readers generate graded predictions about grammatical gender of nouns. Sentence contexts were manipulated so that they strongly biased people's expectations towards two or more nouns that had the same grammatical gender (single bias condition), or they biased multiple genders from different grammatical classes (multiple bias condition). Our expectation was that unexpected articles should lead to elevated reading times (RTs) in the single-bias condition when probabilistic expectations towards a particular gender are violated. Indeed, the results showed greater sensitivity among language users towards unexpected articles in the single-bias condition, however, RTs on unexpected gender- marked articles were facilitated, and not slowed. Our data confirm that difficulty in sentence processing is modulated by uncertainty about predicted information, and suggest that readers make graded predictions about grammatical gender.

iSome Determinants of Chunk Size in Sequential Behavior:Individual Differences in the Transcription of Alphanumeric Strings

Studies have shown that temporal chunk measures in transcrip-tion tasks can be used to assess competence in various domains.However, in other tasks, chunking strategies and thus perfor-mance differences can be highly variable across participants.If such individual differences are also large in transcriptiontasks this would undermine the use of chunk-based competencemeasures. Using four stimuli with fixed spatial structures thisexperiment demonstrates that there is good consistency inchunking strategy across 52 participants in two types of tran-scription tasks. The experiment spans 16,000 data points.

The Perception-Action Loop in a Predictive Agent

We propose an agent model consisting of perceptual and pro-prioceptive pathways. It actively samples a sequence of per-cepts from its environment using the perception-action loop.The model predicts to complete the partial percept and propri-ocept sequences observed till each sampling instant, and learnswhere and what to sample from the prediction error, withoutsupervision or reinforcement. The model is exposed to twokinds of stimuli: images of fully-formed handwritten numer-als/alphabets, and videos of gradual formation of numerals.For each object class, the model learns a set of salient locationsto attend to in images and a policy consisting of a sequence ofeye fixations in videos. Behaviorally, the same model givesrise to saccades while observing images and tracking whileobserving videos. The proposed agent is the first of its kindto interact with and learn end-to-end from static and dynamicenvironments to generate realistic handwriting with state-of-the-art performance.

Sleep-associated consolidation in app-based language learning

Neuro-cognitive models of word learning propose a role for sleep in consolidating new words, yet evidence for sleep-associated memory benefits outside of experimental contexts is scarce. This study compared wake- and sleep-associated memory changes in data from Memrise, a publicly available language-learning app. Memory for foreign words and phrases remained very high in accuracy across a 7-12 hour delay, and there were no differences in forgetting between wake and sleep. However, learners were quicker to arrive at the correct translation after a period of sleep compared to wake. This sleep-associated benefit was seen for words but not phrases, and could not be fully accounted for by circadian differences in completion time. As such, we demonstrate that the behavioural benefits of sleep on vocabulary can be observed in real-world language learning, and discuss the promise for combining small- scale lab studies with naturally occurring datasets to understand learning outcomes.

Experiential Explanations in Iterated Learning

Explanations can be divided into two categories: those that appeal to general principles (abstractive) and those that tella concrete story (experiential) (Aronowitz & Lombrozo, 2019). Most psychological research has focused on abstractiveexplanations, identifying the benefits of abstraction for transfer (Ratterman, Genter & DeLoache, 1987;1989), prediction(Pacer & Lombrozo, 2017), and even cooperation (Burgoon, Henders & Markman, 2013). So why do we sometimesexplain in a less abstract, more narrative mode? Study 1 (N = 195) and Study 2 (N = 843) explore scientific explanationsand find that abstractive and experiential explanations (matched for quality) are transmitted along a chain of people withcomparable fidelity. However, over repeated transmission, experiential explanations become significantly more abstract -whereas abstractive explanations do not drift. Study 3 turns from science to human behavior to test the hypothesis thatexperiential explanations have mnemonic and other cognitive advantages in more narrative domains.

Game on: Mastery Orientation Through the Lens of a Challenging Video Game

Video games are failure-rich spaces that provide a unique lens into how individuals react to failure in challenging environments. In this study, we utilize Cuphead, a notoriously challenging video game to demonstrate a unique behaviorally driven approach to understanding how an individual reacts to failure. Using measures of mastery orientation and data-driven retrospective interviews, we show that individuals who exhibit more mastery-oriented behaviors and more mastery-oriented behaviors before a helpless-behavior are more likely to show a higher game mastery orientation score, and that individuals that abandon a level before completion are more likely to show a lower game mastery orientation score. This introduces video games as a fruitful environment for understanding mastery orientation, a behaviorally driven approach to understanding how individuals react to failure, and provides a glimpse into how individuals react to failure in a challenging video game.

Personal Identity and Online Communities

How has the diffusion of online communities changed howtheir users construct, view, and define their identity? In thispaper, we choose to approach this issue by considering twoparticular philosophical problems related to personal identity:1) The Characterization Question, namely “which actions, ex-periences, beliefs, values, desires, character traits, and so oncan we attribute to a given person?” 2) “How do self-other re-lations affect the ethical implication of identity construction?”To address them, we adopt a comprehensive framework com-posed of cognitive niches and cognitive niche construction the-ories, and we discuss different philosophical and technologicalnotions. In particular: the Filter Bubble problem, the conceptof affordances, and the Sartrean idea of Bad Faith.

The Meaning-Sound Systematicity Also Found in the Korean Language

Recent studies of meaning-sound systematicity haveconsistently found a small but significant positive correlationbetween semantics and phonology. The current study addsfurther evidence from an etymologically distinct language,Korean. Through multiple methods, the study shows thatsimilar sounds tend to have similar meanings in Koreanmonosyllables. Several cultural aspects of the language are alsoquantified. Pure Korean words return stronger meaning-soundcorrelation than Sino-Korean words, which is attributable tothe higher portion of homonyms in Sino-Korean. The mostfrequent words show the strongest systematicity, whichpermeates all of the monosyllables. Certain types of vowelsseem to contribute to this effect.

Measuring neural correlates of infant statistical learning using functionalnear-infrared spectroscopy

Statistical learning may be a key component of language learning in infancy, yet its neural basis is not well established. Thegoal of this study was to measure prefrontal cortical activity during auditory statistical learning, and to determine whetherthis activity predicted infants learning of statistical structure. Using non-invasive functional near-infrared spectroscopy(fNIRS), we recorded changes in blood oxygenation in lateral and medial prefrontal cortex in 8.5-10.5 month old infants(n=34) while they were exposed to statistical speech patterns. The stimuli consisted of 20-second videos of infant-directedspeakers speaking in either a statistical pattern or in a repeated syllable string. We found a positive association betweenright lateral prefrontal cortex activation during exposure to novel statistical speech structures, and subsequent learning ofthese patterns. These results contribute to growing evidence that prefrontal cortical activity during infancy is measurableand correlated with learning.

The “Fraction Sense” Emerges from a Deep Convolutional Neural Network

Fractions are a critical building block for the development ofhuman mathematical cognition, but the origins of this conceptare not well-understood. Recent work has found that a wholenumber sense is present in deep convolutional neural networks(DCNNs) pre-trained for object recognition and uses them asa model for investigating human numerical cognition. Do DC-NNs also have a fraction sense? If so, is it dependent or in-dependent of whole number processing? We investigated theneural sensitivity of a pretrained DCNN to both whole num-bers and fractions. We replicated and extended previous re-search that the sense of whole number emerges in a differentDCNN architecture. Further, we showed that DCNN is alsosensitive to fraction value, i.e., the ratio of numerosities. Test-ing this model, our results suggest that the fraction sense relieson the whole number sense.

Is there a spatial Dunning-Kruger effect? And how is it influences by gender?

Performance on spatial tests is not only a matter ability; it is also influenced by peoples confidence and belief of ability.Although we know that training can improve spatial performance, we know relatively little about the influences of beliefsand expectations on the efficacy of training. Here we investigated men and womens performance on a mental rotationtask and their prediction of their performance. We also examined whether providing information about different strate-gies influenced performance. The results demonstrate a spatial Dunning-Kruger effect; both men and women consistentlyoverestimated their performance. Womens estimates were lower than mens estimates were. Importantly, training influ-enced men and womens predictions of their performance in opposite directions; training increased mens confidence (butnot their performance), whereas training decreased womens confidence (but not their performance). The results suggestthat expectations and beliefs about spatial performance need to be considered when explaining training effects and sexdifferences

Pictures facilitate recognition and retrieval speeds of associations between wordsin a second language and referents

The purpose of this study was to identify associative processes for words in a second language and their referents. ThirtyJapanese participants learned associative conditions for novel words in Chinese and pictorial referents (CP), as well asnovel words in Chinese and words in Japanese (CJ), against a condition of only novel words in Chinese (C). After thelearning phase, participants conducted 2 retrieval tasks for word recognition and 3 recognition tasks for source-monitoringof the referents. Correct answers for each recognition task were provided to participants after each trial. Although correctanswers in all the conditions gradually increased in both the recognition and retrieval tasks, there were no significantdifferences among these conditions. In contrast, recognition and retrieval speeds were faster for CP than CJ. These findingssuggest that pictures contribute to recognition and retrieval speeds of associations between words in a second languageand referents.

The effect of cheerleading chants on time estimation performance

It is not clear how cheering chants affect time perception which can be critical for sport performance. Here we measuredthe performance participants estimated second-order length of duration using a conventional psychophysical task. In thecontrol condition, five participants were required to produce 1, 3, 5 or 10 seconds of target durations by pressing a button ina gymnasium where nobody except for experimenters came in. In the testing condition, a group of cheerleaders appearedand chanted for 20 seconds after each block. The participants were required to complete otherwise the same task as thecontrol condition. The order of conditions was counterbalanced. The percentages of errors of estimated time was 4.229.58,-24.318.81, -24.9117.06, -22.4921.83 for 1, 3, 5 and 10 second of target durations in the control condition. Those valueswere 16.1322.54, -10.7716.22, -11.878.95 and -12.385.74 in the testing condition. In summary, the chants increased theduration participants produced.

Motion recognition with biologically plausible spiking neural networks

Although artificial deep learning based neural networks have recently achieved impressive results on a range of realisticpattern recognition problems, it is still not completely clear how this problem is solved by the hierarchy of spiking neuronsin the brain which has inspired the deep learning approach in the first place. To achieve high accuracy on real-worldproblems artificial deep neural networks are trained using backpropagation, which is known to be biologically implausible.Recently Lillicrap et al. have proposed Feedback Alignment as a more biologically realistic algorithm able to train a deephierarchy of spiking neurons. In this work we examine whether a spiking deep neural network using such a biologicallyplausible learning algorithm is able to achieve good recognition accuracy on realistic motion recognition tasks.

The Emergence of Action-grounded Compositional Communication

Classical models of the emergence of compositionality in communication focused on the compositional nature of the en-vironment (Cangelosi, 2001; Cornish et al., 2008). Here we advance a model in which compositional structure emergesfrom integrating environments properties with agents actions. We take as a starting point Cangelosis (2001) model, wherea population of agents searched for edible mushrooms. Given opportunity to communicate, they evolved a system inwhich combinations of signs were sensorily grounded in combinations of mushroom properties. We modify this modelby grounding the communication also in agents’ actions. With this, we are able to evolve communication systems con-taining meaningful compositions of mushroom properties and agent actions. We investigate how such compositions canfacilitate a) learning the communication protocol, b) learning the adequate behavior policy. This kind of sensory-motorcompositionality seems better suited for coordinating navigation in dynamic environments.

Can preschoolers use probability to infer others desires?

Probability influences our social inferences. Here, we explored whether preschoolers use probabilistic information to inferothers desires. Sixty 3-year-olds were shown stories where one character went to a gumball machine with mostly redgumballs and just a few purple ones and another character went to a machine with the reverse distribution. Both charactersreceived a red gumball. Children in one between-subjects condition were asked who wanted a red gumball and children ina control condition were asked who knew they would get a red gumball. Children mostly selected the character who wentto the machine with more red gumballs when asked about desires but not when asked about knowledge. This suggests that3-year-olds can use proportions to infer others desires.

Lexical Associations in a Native and Non-Native LanguageAffect Retrieval-Induced Forgetting

Recent work suggests that speakers’ lexical networks in theirnative and secondary languages are organized somewhatdifferently, with native languages showing greatersystematicity. We here test this claim in a new way, bymaking use of the “Retrieval-induced forgetting” effect(RIF). Specifically, practicing previously encodedinformation through rehearsal is expected to result in bettermemory for that information, regardless of which languagethe information is encoded. The RIF effect involves thesuppression of information that is associated with thepracticed information but is itself unpracticed. Since RIF isunderstood to rely on the association between the practicedand unpracticed memories, we predict it will be weaker whenapplied in a language with weaker or less systematicallyorganized lexical associations. Results confirm that while theexpected practice effect was evident in participants’ nativeand second languages, the RIF effect was only significant inparticipants’ native language. We discuss the relevance andimplications of this finding for second language speakers.

How children learn non-obvious conceptual information from caregivers innaturalistic settings

A long-standing question in cognitive development asks how young children learn non-obvious conceptual information(i.e., information that is not directly perceptible). For artifacts, this non-obvious information includes the categories itemsfall into (Rhodes, Gelman & Karuza, 2014), and their functions (Matan & Carey, 2001). We investigated how childrenlearn non-obvious information about novel artifacts from their caregivers during naturalistic interactions in a living his-tory museum. Forty caregiver-child dyads (Ages: R=4;22-8;0,Mage=5.98 years) visited two exhibits for 8 minutes each(i.e., a heritage store and house). Using a series of GEEs and correlational analyses, we found caregivers used differentpedagogical techniques to teach their children about different artifact properties. Namely, they used causal (rs=.49,p¡.001)and procedural information (rs=.60,p¡.001) to describe an artifacts function, but used questions (rs=.79,p¡.001) and com-parisons (rs=.64,p¡.001) to discuss an artifacts identity. These patterns are compatible with the broader literature on howchildren learn non-obvious information best (Gelman, 2009).

Reducing the Perceived Reliability of an External Store Reduces Susceptibility toExternal Store Manipulation.

Offloading cognition to external stores is practiced ubiquitously in daily life (e.g., counting on fingers, writing lists), yetis a relatively new area of investigation within cognitive science. Previous experiments have assessed the benefits anddownfalls, including participants lowered memory for offloaded information that is no longer available (Gardony et al.,2013; Sparrow et al., 2011). In addition, when offloading, individuals appear susceptible to manipulations of their externalstore (Risko et al., 2019). Here we report an experiment investigating how the perceived reliability of an external storeaffects individuals susceptibility to manipulation of that store. Consistent with previous research, results suggest thatthe majority of participants do not notice an item inserted into their external store. However, once cued to this event,individuals do become more likely to subsequently notice a manipulation of their external store. Implications of thisresearch for our understanding of distributed memory systems will be discussed.

Ask or Tell: Balancing questions and instructions in intuitive teaching

Teaching is an intuitive social activity that requires reason-ing about and influencing the mind of others. A good teacherforms a belief about the knowledge of their student, asks clar-ifying questions, and gives instructions or explanations to tryto induce a target concept in the student’s mind. We proposePartially Observable Markov Decision Processes (POMDPs)as a model of intuitive human teaching. According to this ac-count, teachers make pedagogical decisions with uncertaintyabout the knowledge state of their student. In two behavioralexperiments, human participants were tasked with balancingassessments (asking questions) and instructions to help teach astudent to build a tower of colored blocks. Human behavior inthe task was compared to the performance of a computerizedteaching algorithm optimized to solve the equivalent POMDP.Our results show that humans favor asking questions and estab-lishing common ground during teaching even at an economiccost and increase question asking as uncertainty grows.

Probability and processing speed of scalar inferences is context-dependent

Studies addressing the question of whether scalar inferencesgenerally incur a processing cost have yielded conflicting re-sults. Constraint-based accounts, which seek to unify theseconflicting results, make a prediction which we test here: theprobability of an interpretation and the speed with which it isprocessed depends on the contextual support it receives. Wemanipulated contextual support for the scalar inference in twotruth-value judgment experiments by manipulating a lexicalfeature (presence of partitive “of the”) and a pragmatic fea-ture (the implicit Question Under Discussion). Participants’responder type – whether their majority response was prag-matic (reflecting the inference) or literal (reflecting its absence)– was the main predictor of response times: pragmatic re-sponses were faster than literal responses when generated bypragmatic responders; the reverse was true for literal respon-ders. We interpret this as further evidence against costly infer-ence accounts and in support of constraint-based accounts ofpragmatic processing.

Effects of Voiced Initial Consonants in Japanese Sound-Symbolic Words:Experiment 3

Previous studies have hypothesized that Japanese sound-symbolic words with voiced initial consonants (SSWV; e.g.,boroboro) rather than those with voiceless initial consonants (SSWVL; e.g., horohoro) or semi-voiced initial consonants(SSWSV; e.g., poroporo) induce stronger evaluations of the quality of psycholinguistic features. To investigate this hy-pothesis, we asked 36 Japanese participants to evaluate 13 psycholinguistic features (familiarity, visual imagery, auditoryimagery, haptic imagery, arousal, preference, disgust, hardness, softness, heaviness, lightness, fastness, and slowness) withSSWV, SSWVL, and SSWSV using 5-point semantic differential scales. All the initial consonants involved h (f; SSWVL),p (SSWSV), or b (SSWV). The experimental results showed that SSWV included higher levels of visual imagery, auditoryimagery, haptic imagery, arousal, disgust, hardness, and heaviness over SSWVL or SSWSV (ps ¡ .05). Taken together,these findings suggest that SSWV induces psychological and physical quality evaluations more than SSWVL and SSWSV.

Nonlinear Probability Weighting Can Reflect Attentional Biases in Sequential Sampling

Nonlinear probability weighting allows cumulative prospect theory (CPT) to account for seminal phenomena in riskychoice (e.g., the certainty effect). The attentional drift diffusion model (aDDM) formalizes that attentional biases canshape preferences as a sequential sampling process. We simulated choices between safe and risky options using the aDDMwith varying attentional biases to safe or risky options and modeled these choices with CPT. Changes in preferences dueto attentional biases were systematically reflected in the parameters of CPT’s weighting function (curvature, elevation).We demonstrate that this also holds empirically, in the sampling paradigm in decision from experience. Hence, nonlinearprobability weighting can arise from option-specific attentional biases in information search. This challenges commoninterpretations of probability-weighting parameters, suggests novel attentional explanations for empirical phenomena as-sociated with characteristic shapes of CPT’s probability-weighting function, and adds to the integration of two prominentcomputational frameworks for decision making.

Co-occurrences and temporal distribution of caregivers indexical multimodal cuesin real-world interactions

When caregivers talk to their children, they can also look, point or manipulate the objects they are talking about. Thesemultimodal indexical cues can help the child disambiguate the referred object from potential targets in the environmentduring word learning. In fact, most naming episodes are modulated by some multimodal cues. In the work we presenthere, we use data from a semi-naturalistic corpus of caregiver-child interactions (ECOLANG corpus) where caregiverstalk to their children about objects that are new or known to the child. We focus on caregivers production and ask: (i)how often caregivers use any of the multimodal cues when naming the referent for new vs. known objects; (ii) what thetemporal relationship between multimodal cues and naming episodes is; (iii) whether there is a relationship between thecue usage (and its temporal distribution) and word learning.

A Visual Recall Paradigm to Assess Implicit Statistical Learning

Implicit statistical learning, whereby regularities between stimuli are detected without conscious awareness, is importantfor language acquisition. This form of learning has often been assessed using measures that require conscious decisionmaking or explicit reflection (e.g., 2AFC tasks). We aimed to measure statistical learning more implicitly. We leveraged thefact that frequently co-occurring stimuli may be chunked into a single cognitive unit, reducing working memory demands.We developed an artificial grammar in which sequences contained pairs of stimuli which always co-occurred (chunks)and more variable between-chunk transitions. In a novel visual recall paradigm, participants were asked to rememberand recreate sequences of serially presented images. Recall of predictable sequences improved over the course of theexperiment. However, recall dropped to initial levels when participants were presented with random sequences containingno predictable chunks. This approach represents a valuable method to measure statistical learning implicitly, withoutrequiring conscious reflection.

Better together: Exploration prior to instruction facilitates rule-learning andmodifies attention to demonstration

Debates assessing the merits of independent exploration and pedagogical instruction have been extensive. We compareeach of these learning environments against exploration followed by instruction to assess benefits to procedural learningand abstract rule-learning. Ninety-nine six-year-olds learned about novel locks and keys by either independently exploringprior to receiving instruction, proceeding to instruction without exploration, or acting without instruction. Children whoreceived instruction did not differ in procedural knowledge. However, children who explored prior to instruction were sig-nificantly more likely to learn the rules than children who did not explore or did not receive instruction. Childrens visualattention during instruction indicated that those who explored looked proportionally more to the stimuli as the experi-menter demonstrated. This suggests that the value of exploration is perhaps in preparing the learner for later information.Therefore, these results suggest that there is particular value for conceptual learning in the combination of exploration withinstruction.

Belief revision in a micro-social network: Modeling sensitivity to statisticaldependencies in social learning

In both professional domains and everyday life, people mustintegrate their own experience with reports from social networkpeers to form and update their beliefs. It is therefore importantto understand to what extent people accommodate the statis-tical dependencies that give rise to correlated belief reportsin social networks. We investigate adults’ ability to integratesocial evidence appropriately in a political scenario, varyingthe dependence between the sources of network peers’ beliefs.Using a novel interface that allows participants to express theirprobabilistic beliefs visually, we compare participants against anormative Bayesian standard. We find that they distinguish thevalue of evidence from dependent versus independent sources,but that they also treated social sources as substantially weakerevidence than direct experience. The value of our elicitationmethodology and the implications of our results for modelinghuman-like belief revision in social networks are discussed.

How Do Verbs Change Their Meaning? Evidence for Minimal Subtraction

Verb metaphor has received little attention compared to noun metaphor. But verbs may be more likely to take onmetaphoric meanings than are nouns. One indication of this is verb mutability verbs are more prone to adapt theirmeanings under semantic strain than nouns (Gentner & France, 1988; King & Gentner, 2019). We tested the minimalsubtraction hypothesis (Gentner & France, 1988), which proposes (1) domain-specific dimensions of a verbs meaning areadjusted before abstract relational structure, and (2) degree of adjustment increases with strain. In three experiments, wecollected paraphrases of simple sentences and using word2vec found progressively greater abstraction of verb (but notnoun) meaning with strain. For example, a typical paraphrase of The wagon limped was The cart creaked along; a typicalparaphrase of The fantasy limped was The imagination faltered, reflecting greater abstraction of limped. These findingssupport the minimal subtraction account of verb metaphoric extension.

English speakers (in)ability to explicitly recognize agent and patient categories

Adults represent events in terms of abstract participant roles (e.g., when Edith eats chocolate, Edith is an agent and thechocolate is a patient) (Rissman & Majid, 2019). English, however, lacks commonly-known labels for these roles, whichmay make the distinction less accessible to people. We presented 42 English-speakers with 24 pictures of an agent actingon a patient (e.g., one person kicking another). A red dot marked the agent in half the pictures and marked the patientin the other half. We asked participants to sort the pictures into two piles using whatever criteria they liked. After threeopportunities to sort the pictures, only 55% of participants sorted into agent/patient piles. When the remaining 45% weregiven the agent/patient piles, only half were able to explain the basis for the sort. This suggests a disconnect betweenthe robustness of agent/patient categories in implicit processing and the availability of this seemingly basic distinction toexplicit reasoning.

The method of loci is an optimal policy for memory search

The method of loci is a powerful mnemonic technique for memorizing a list of unrelated items. With a pre-specified routein a familiar ”memory palace”, one can encode material by attaching items to loci along this route, and later effectivelyrecall them by mentally walking along the same route. Despite its efficacy, there is no existing model that explains whythe method of loci promotes memory improvement during memory search. To fill this gap, we provide a rational accountof why the method of loci improves memory. We define memory search as a task with the goal of minimizing retrievalcost, and demonstrate that the method of loci gives an optimal policy for this task. We discuss the implications of thisresult, and compare it with models of memory search without using mnemonic techniques.

Implicit questions shape information preferences

We ask questions about everything from why clocks tick towhy the sky is blue. Although people sometimes preferteleological explanations over mechanistic explanations inresponse to ‘why’ questions, why questions are ambiguous–referring either to a ‘how’ question or a ‘for what purpose’question. In this paper, we examine the relation between theseimplicit questions and explanation preferences. First, we askedwhether people have specific expectations regarding ‘why’questions: How do they interpret these ambiguous cases anddoes this vary across domains? Indeed, people have strong,domain-specific expectations that mirror well-documentedexplanation preferences. People also have preferences aboutwhich specific question they would prefer to have answered. Inother words, ‘why’ questions are ambiguous but not treated assuch — and this has consequences for downstream explanationpreferences. We explore these consequences in light of both thephilosophical and psychological literature on explanation.

Abstraction and Cognitive Flexibility in Collective Problem Solving: The Role of Diversity

Groups of interacting individuals are often found to have an advantage over individuals in contexts of complex problem solving. We suggest that social interaction allows group members to share diverse introspections, perspectives and strategies, promoting the formation of more abstract problem representations, which – in turn – apply more flexibly to new problem contexts. In a reinforcement learning task inspired by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), participants categorized aliens as friendly or dangerous based on an underlying rule specifying feature combinations. After a number of correctly categorized trials, the rule would change (without explicit notification). Participants could solve the task by learning every new rule, but could also discover an underlying abstract rule, which would facilitate faster recovery from local rule changes. We compared pairs of participants individually trained on different rules (diversity pairs), with pairs trained on the same rule (non-diversity pairs), and individuals. We found that diversity pairs outperformed non- diverse pairs and individuals. Our findings suggest that diversity in prior experience benefits groups, likely due to processes of abstraction and cognitive flexibility.

Age Effects in the Acquisition of Phonological Variation

We report a series of artificial language learning experiments designed to test child and adult learners abilities to acquirethree types of phonological variation. Previous work on experimental morphology (Hudson Kam & Newport 2005, 2009;Schuler, Yang & Newport, 2016) has found that young children turn inconsistent input into an invariant rule, while adultsreproduce and match variation in their input. Here we investigate whether phonological variation of three different types(deterministic conditioning, unconditioned variation, and probabilistic variation) exhibits a similar age pattern. We finda clear effect of age in grammatically-conditioned variability, with the youngest children showing a strong tendency toregularize to the stem form, adults probability-matching, and intermediate-aged children learning correct conditioning butnot matching the input probabilities. These results suggest, in accord with previous findings on morphology, that variationis not readily learned by young children and may instead be acquired as a separate process.

Impact of effort exertion on cognitive flexibility and stability

Impact of effort exertion on cognitive flexibility and stabilityAnna Mini Jos, Myles LoParco, A. Ross Otto*Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montral, CanadaEfficient task execution requires attention to task requirements while inhibiting distractors (cognitive stability) and adapt-ing to changes (flexibility). Previous studies have shown that individuals differ in their application of stable versus flexibleprocessing modes. Our study examined the impact of prior effort exertion on flexibility/stability trade-off.Participants performed a stability-flexibility paradigm, with pupil recording, before and after effort and no-effort manipula-tions were induced using different tasks. We analyzed the resulting change in preferences for stability/flexibility (voluntaryswitch rate).We found that the no-effort condition evoked a higher voluntary switch rate than baseline or after effort exertion. Partici-pants in the effort condition also showed higher response times and lower accuracy across trials. Pupil data shows that aftereffort exertion participants exert less effort in spontaneous switches and repeats. Additionally, the relationship betweenswitch cost (on forced-switch trials) and spontaneous switching rate increased after effort exertion. These results suggestthat stability/flexibility preferences can vary with prior effort exertion.

From Integers to Fractions: Developing a Coherent Understanding ofProportional Magnitude

Children display an early sensitivity to implicit proportions (e.g., 1 of 5 apples vs 3 of 4 apples) but have considerabledifficulty in learning the explicit, symbolic proportions denoted by fractions (e.g., 1/5 vs 3/4). Theoretically, reducingthe gap between representations of implicit vs explicit proportions would improve understanding of fractions, but littleis known about how the representations develop and interact with one another. To address this, we asked 163 third tofifth graders to estimate the position of proportionally-equivalent integers and fractions on number lines (e.g., 3 on 0-8number line vs 3/8 on 0-1 number line). We found that, with increasing age, children were more accurate and linear inrepresenting both integers and fractions. More importantly, childrens estimates of implicit and explicit proportions becamemore coherent, such that a childs estimates of fractions on a 0-1 number-line was a linear function of the same childsestimates of equivalent integers. This representational coherence independently predicted childrens fraction proficiency inother tasks, suggest- ing that building a coherent understanding of proportions is an educationally-important goal.

Feature selection in category learning

Research examining mechanisms underlying human categorization has reported that when learning novel categories, adultstend to selectively attend to the diagnostic features, whereas young children allocate attention to multiple features. Thisstudy further investigated mechanisms underlying children and adults category learning by measuring their accuracy andresponse time in classification tasks. Participants were trained with categories that have a single deterministically predic-tive feature and multiple probabilistic features, and they were tested with items varying in the number of features. Theresults indicated that with sufficient training, both adults and children relied exclusively on the deterministic feature regard-less of overall similarity. Importantly, a deterministic feature is both sufficient and efficient for learning new categories.Participants were as accurate and fast when classifying items with most probabilistic features missing as when classifyingitems with all features present. However, when the deterministic feature was inaccessible, their accuracy dropped, andresponse times slowed.

Preschoolers recognize that losses loom larger than gains.

People often over value their current property. For example,even young children will choose to keep their current propertyover trading it for property of similar utility (Hartley & Fisher,2018). In two experiments (N = 180), we examined howchildren aged 3 and 4 weigh the potential loss of existingproperty against the gain of property in their reasoning aboutothers’ actions. We found that by 4-years-old, children expectothers to prioritize the retention of existing property over theacquisition of new property. We suggest that this expectationreflects an understanding that people often value what theyalready own more than what they can potentially gain. Wediscuss the implications of our findings for competing theoriesof ownership reasoning, and for children’s reasoning about lossaversion.

Do We Need Neural Models to Explain Human Judgments of Acceptability?

Native speakers can judge whether a sentence is an acceptableinstance of their language. Acceptability provides a means ofevaluating whether computational language models are pro-cessing language in a human-like manner. We test the abilityof language models, simple language features, and word em-beddings to predict native speakers’ judgments of acceptabil-ity on English essays written by non-native speakers. We findthat much sentence acceptability variance can be captured by acombination of misspellings, word order, and word similarity(r = 0.494). While predictive neural models fit acceptabilityjudgments well (r = 0.527), we find that a 4-gram model isjust as good (r = 0.528). Thanks to incorporating misspellings,our 4-gram model surpasses both the previous unsupervisedstate-of-the art (r = 0.472), and the average native speaker(r = 0.46), demonstrating that acceptability is well capturedby n-gram statistics and simple language features.

People use inverse planning to rationally seek social information from objects

People use objects to make social judgments about traits of owners. Do people seek social information in a rational waysuggestive of Bayesian inverse planning? In two experiments, participants aimed to learn about a stranger. Each trialshowed two sets of objects; the stranger had chosen one from each set, but their choice was hidden. Participants judgedwhich would help them learn more about the stranger: Revealing their choice from set A or B? Participants selected setsrationally, identifying sets with a greater range of options as more informative: Larger sets over smaller; sets varying instyle over sets varying only in color (Exp.1). Participants also took into account constraints: They chose sets as moreinformative when all options were functional vs. when some were not (Exp.2). People consider the generative processbehind objects selection, using inverse planning to reason about the informational value of others objects.

Do expectations for causal patterns differ between domains? Studying physical,biological and psychological events across cultures

Fundamental theories of causal cognition suggest that causal inferences are guided by domain-specific knowledge inaddition to domain-general strategies used to draw causal conclusions. In particular, a divide seems to exist betweencausal judgments on physical versus psychological events. In line with these assumptions, domain-specific expectationsof causal patterns have been observed for psychological and physical events in a US-American context. The present studyintended to augment these findings by integrating (a) a cross-cultural perspective and by including (b) biological events aspart of an additional domain. Results replicated previous findings of domain-specific causal expectations in German andTurkish cultural contexts, but at the same time they indicated causal expectations for the biological domain to be partiallyless distinct.

Good-enough production with repetition

Good-Enough production is the case where lexical selection between alternative words is subject to accessibility effects.Recent evidence suggests the name for a picture depends partly on the phonological form of the last word said. If Good-Enough production reflects variability in name activation and competition among alternative names, then non-adjacentmanipulations should also affect picture naming probability and latency. Participants read aloud printed words and namedpictures one at a time, including target pictures with two highly probable names (Dominant and Secondary). In theRepetition (vs Control) condition, Secondary names were read aloud in early trials, and target pictures were named at least50 trials later. Earlier reading resulted in a higher probability of Secondary names for target pictures, compared to Controlparticipants, suggesting that lexical selection is subject to non-adjacent influences. Dominant naming latency was greaterunder earlier Secondary name reading, supporting an interactive, competitive lexical selection mechanism.

A role for working memory in shaping the action policy for reinforcement learning

During learning, humans recruit multiple cognitive mechanisms, including value-based reinforcement learning and ex-ecutive functions, like working memory. Recent research has begun to unmask connections between these two systems,proposing roles for attention and working memory in shaping underlying learning computations. Here, using a simpleinstrumental learning task, we provide evidence that working memory plays a role in establishing the correct state spacethat reinforcement learning operates over. We show that reinforcement learning is impaired when executive functioning istaxed by a secondary task and that this effect is especially pronounced when the two tasks are performed simultaneouslyrather than alternated. Computational modeling suggests that when the executive function is occupied, the reinforcementlearning system forms policy over a confused state-space. This study adds to a growing body of research proposing a morefundamental role for high-level executive processes in low-level reinforcement learning computations.

The role of literary metaphors in aesthetic appreciation

Research on empirical aesthetics has only recently included the study of literary texts, and not yet addressed the roleplayed by metaphors. We first created alternative versions of modern poems devoid of literary metaphors but equal inother properties to the original poems. The former are perceived as more beautiful. Second, we had participants readsentences extracted from poems and recorded their reading times and beauty ratings. Sentences could be literal, containdead metaphors, conventional, novel or extremely novel metaphors. Increasingly more novel and creative metaphors leadto increasing aesthetic appreciation, showing a clear linear relationship. Even extremely novel metaphors are appreciated,despite being more difficult to read. These results are only partially in line with current theories. Finally, aestheticappreciation is only partially driven by increasing cognitive load: metaphoricity plays a more important and substantialrole. The contribution of our results to extant theories will be discussed.

You Take the High Road, and I’ll Take the Low Road:Evaluating the Topographical Consistency of Cognitive Models

We present a novel framework for assessing the fit of cogni-tive models. Using this framework, we highlight limitationsof existing methods of model evaluation, and derive new ap-proaches to validating cognitive models. Tests of topographi-cal consistency emphasize how a model’s structure constrainsbehavior on pairs of coupled stimuli, even when point predic-tions on individual stimuli depend on estimates of the model’sfree parameters. By carefully selecting these coupled stim-uli such that they follow the distinct topography of the model,researchers can overcome some limitations of existing meth-ods. Finally, we provide a proof-of-concept example of how touse our approach to assess a model of multi-alternative, multi-attribute choice.

Early Environments and Exploration in the Preschool Years

Childrens exploration is driven by opportunities for learning, and past research has suggested rational explanations for howearly home experiences may affect childrens active learning (Yu et al., 2020) or willingness to wait for rewards (Kidd etal., 2013). However, less work has characterized the relationship between childrens environmental contexts and play. Wepooled exploratory play data from past experiments in our lab (M(age)=56mos; N=278), and correlated play behavior withmedian income and education in the childs home zipcode. Children from lower SES areas played significantly longer,more variably, and spent a lower proportion of time focusing on demonstrated functions which traded off with lengthand variability of play exclusively for children from lower educated areas. Importantly, home income is confounded withdaycare income; future work will disentangle distinct influences of family SES and daycare environment. This work layscritical groundwork for understanding early active learning across developmental contexts.

Retreat from overgeneralization errors: Broad verb classes are harder to inducethan narrow classes

One of the biggest puzzles in language acquisition is concerned with how children retreat from overgeneralization errorsin valence alternations, for example the ditransitive alternation. Pinker (1989) proposes that children are susceptible toovergeneralization when they acquire broad verb semantic classes initially and they recover when they acquire narrow verbclasses later. To empirically test this hypothesis, we devised a computational framework that automatically induces verbclasses from text data, by combining state-of-art word embeddings (Pennington, Socher & Manning, 2014) with graphalgorithms (Steyvers & Tenenbaum, 2005; Von Luxburg, 2007). We selected three representative valence alternationsfrom Levin (1993) and tested Pinkers hypothesis on five naturalistic language production corpora. Our results demonstratethat contrary to Pinkers predictions, broad verb classes are harder to induce than narrow classes and that semantic classesmay not be the primary mechanism that accounts for childrens retreat from overgeneralization errors.

Is probability utility correlation really correlation?An individual-level analysis of risk-reward heuristics

Utility and probability have been considered independentconstructs for decision making under uncertainty. However,many studies have suggested that people assume there is acorrelation between probability and utility. Some studies havedemonstrated that people appear to estimate the utility ofevents depending on their probabilities, and other studiesrecently indicated the existence of “risk-reward heuristics” thatassume a negative correlation between probability and utilityin the real world when inferring winning probabilities frompayoffs during decisions made under uncertainty. This studyaimed to explore the relationship between probability andutility by requiring participants to estimate both probabilitiesfrom payoffs and payoffs from probabilities under a gain orloss situation. The results indicated that when estimating valuesof payoffs from probabilities, participants’ judgments showedclear negative correlations between probability and utility bothin the gain and loss condition. However, when estimatingprobabilities from payoffs, this negative correlation betweenutility and probability was found only in a gain situation. Theseresults support the existence of risk-reward heuristics, and atthe same time, suggest a possibility that people have differentintuitions for the probability-utility relationship between thegain and loss domains.

Events Structure Information Accessibility Less in Children than Adults

Adults parse continuous experience into meaningful events, a process referred to as event segmentation. This segmentationin turn colors how experiences are construed content experienced within an event is held mentally in an accessible state,which is then dropped after an event boundary. However, little is known about whether children are similarly influencedby event boundaries. Here, we tested seven- to nine-year-old childrens and adults recognition of objects experienced eitherwithin or across event boundaries of two cartoons. We found that children and adults were both more accurate and fasterto correctly recognizing objects that last occurred within events than across an event boundary. We, however, additionallyobserved an interaction such that childrens access to recent experience was less influenced by event boundaries than adults.Thus, while the spontaneous segmentation of complex events emerges by middle childhood, event structure less reliablyshapes the active contents of childrens minds than adults.

Recurrent top-down synaptic connections at different spatial frequencies helpdisambiguate between dynamic emotions

The coarse-to-fine hypothesis posits that, in the Human visualsystem, a coarse representation of visual information is propa-gated quickly through the retina to the cortex, whereas a finer,more detailed representation is propagated more slowly. In aprevious study we showed that recurrent synaptic connectionshelp predict low intensity EFEs. Furthermore, a feedback loopcoming from coarser information processing is postulated toinfluence later processing of finer features. In this paper, weintend to examine the value of coarser information and recur-rence in the processing of dynamic Emotional Facial Expres-sions (EFE). In a step forward in studying the importance ofrecurrent connectivity in the coarse-to-fine model, we testedits advantage for discriminating emotions for different spatialfrequencies and facial expression intensities. Using ArtificialNeural Networks, we modeled recurrent synaptic connectionswith a recurrent feedback loop. Using a Gabor filter bank, wecomputed different levels of spatial frequency features. Our re-sults replicate the advantage of recurrence at first facial expres-sion intensities. Our main finding is that the recurrent model isalso better when predicting high spatial frequencies features.Additionally, mid-to-low spatial frequencies are more usefulto the prediction of EFEs. We conclude that feature process-ing feedback has a significant effect in disambiguating facialexpressions when information is particularly complex, i.e., athigh spatial frequencies and low EFE intensities.

Dynamical Feedback and Affordances-Constraints in Technology-MediatedLearning and Assessment: An in-Class Experimental Study

How do we assess learning in complex technology-mediated practices? How does the coordination of technological af-fordances and constraints mediate immediate performance and individual learning? In the technology-mediated practiceof programming, the compiler functions as a source of both affordances and constraints to the human cognitive agent.The compiler affords the compilation of executable programs and dynamically informative compiler feedback, while thecompiler also constrains acceptable code to a specific syntax. In this in-class experimental study, I investigate the contri-bution of compiler affordances and constraints to performance and learning in programming. The study results indicateaffordances as important facilitators of immediate performance. Conversely, constraints appear important mediators ofconceptual learning, which in turn facilitates internalized thinking decoupled from the original technological resource.The findings imply a need for teaching and learning activities that emphasize practicing resource-coordination and foran assessment practice that intelligently combines technology-mediated resource-rich tasks with decoupled resource-poortasks.

Personality Traits Moderate the Relationship between Statistical Learning Abilityand Second-Language Learners’ Sentence Comprehension

An accumulating body of evidence has demonstrated a tightcoupling between individual differences (ID) in statisticallearning ability (SL) and variation in language performancein child and adult native speaker populations, with some ini-tial evidence that this coupling extends to second language(L2) speakers. However, surprisingly little work has been con-ducted to assess potential interactions between SL and otherexperience-related and affective ID factors. Using a within-subjects design embedded in an ID framework, the presentstudy attempts to fill this gap by investigating whether the im-pact of SL ability on language is moderated by individual dif-ferences in personality traits and the amount of experience anindividual has had with the L2. The results of the study re-vealed a complex interplay between ID factors and variationin L2 comprehension of different types of complex sentencesindicating that the effect of SL ability on language comprehen-sion is moderated by personality traits.

Brainwave profiles of efficient versus inefficient working memory retrievals inhealthy older adults

General slowing of mental processing speed is hallmark of brain and cognitive aging. Thus far it has been limited under-standing in neural mechanisms underlying mental states during fluctuations between efficient versus inefficient cognitiveperformance within individual older adults. Here we examined electrophysiological responses during visual working mem-ory retrieval trials that are fast versus slow reactions. Wireless EEG along with accuracy and reaction times were recordedduring a modified delayed match-to-sample task in 17 cognitively normal older adults (age 65-95) from North America.Compared to trials that are faster than averaged (mean 584 ms), the late positive potentials during trials that are slowerthan average (mean 747 ms) showed increased responses to memory nonmatch distractors than those to object matchingmemory targets in frontal sites, as previously reported in older brains. Interestingly, the brainwaves during efficient andaccurate memory retrievals resemble those typically seen in younger adults.

From information-seeking actions (and their costs), adults jointly infer both whatothers know, and what they believe they can learn

We face a challenge when inferring what others know. Actions do not transparently reveal epistemic states: ignorantagents routinely ignore information too costly to obtain, and knowledgeable agents often confirm what they already knowwhen its convenient. We hypothesized that epistemic inferences are sensitive both to agents actions, and the underlyingutilities that best explain them. We tested this possibility in a simple task. Adults watched an explorer decide whetherto collect a map before searching an island for treasure. Participants (n=40) were asked to jointly infer how much theexplorer knew about the treasures location, and how much information the explorer believed the map had. Participantjudgments matched a computational model of epistemic inference structured around an expectation that agents rationallytradeoff information gain with information cost (r=0.86; 95%: 0.740.93, p¡.001). Our results suggest that adult Theory ofMind supports nuanced and graded epistemic inferences from observable action.

Flexible Strategy Use in Soar’s Tic-Tac-Toe

Modeling cognitive processes is one of the major tasks of cognitive science. This work presents a model of a studydescribed in Flexible Strategy Use in Young Childrens Tic-Tac-Toe (Crowley & Siegler, 1993) in which the authorsmade an attempt to characterize decision-making in a conflict-of-interests-like environment. In the experiments, kinder-garten/primary school children and an algorithm-based opponent played a series of games in Tic-Tac-Toe. The outcomesseemed to indicate the existence of a hierarchy of rules that is constructed with experience. Although already tested al-gorithmically, the simulation detailed in the paper was applicable to a narrow class of problems only. The model shownin this work was built using a cognitive architecture, i.e. computer-based structure mimicking the general functioning ofthe human mind. Concretely, we used a rule-based system Soar that operates in mental rules paradigm and in most partreplicated the results of the mentioned study.

Using Neuromyths to Explore Educator Cognition: A Mouse-Tracking Paradigm

Current theories of knowledge acquisition suggest that newlylearned knowledge does not always supplant prior knowledge,even when newly learned knowledge repairs errors. Newknowledge may suppress prior knowledge, particularly foroverlearned, explicit responses, creating internal competitionbetween knowledge elements. Competition between new and priorknowledge may be one reason misconceptions are highly resistantto repair. The present study examines misconceptions in a specificdomain: pre-service educators’ beliefs about neuromyths.Addressing misconceptions in pre-service educators is importantbecause these misconceptions are likely to be transmitted tostudents and may reduce the effectiveness of instruction. Acomputer mouse-tracking paradigm measured explicit beliefs inneuromyths as well as implicit uncertainty during thedecision-making process. The findings demonstrated thatpre-service educators often endorsed neuromyths but wereuncertain about the veracity of neurofacts. These findings add toour knowledge of misconceptions, their durability, anddemonstrate a need to address misconceptions in educatorpreparation.

Clustering as a precursor to efficient and near-optimal solution of small instancesof the Traveling Salesperson Problem (TSP)

Humans efficiently find near-optimal (i.e., near-minimum-length) tours when solving small instances of the TravelingSalesperson Problem (TSP), a problem hard for computers. We hypothesize that this is possible because they use thefollowing strategy: cluster the points, solve the smaller TSPs for each cluster, and then solve the TSP defined by theclusters. This study focused on the antecedent process of human clustering. 42 participants clustered 56 sets of 15 to 40points on two occasions. We found that human clustering is generally reliable (M Fowlkes-Mallows Index = 0.75) forall problem sizes. Reliability was higher for problems that showed statistical evidence of cluster structure versus no suchstructure, and was not affected when the problem was flipped for the second presentation. Thus, humans are sensitive tocluster structure, and clustering is a stable foundation for solving TSP instances. This sets the stage for future research onclustering-based TSP strategies.

Does time extend asymmetrically towards the past and the future? Across-cultural study

Is the human representation of time symmetrical or asymmetrical toward the past and the future? Some studies suggestthat we perceive the future as being closer, more attended and more valued than the past (indicating a future asymmetry).By contrast, asymmetries toward the past have been found in past-focused cultures. Yet, available evidence is still limitedand mixed. In the present work we searched for asymmetry in several temporal tasks (temporal distance, time discounting,temporal depth, and self-continuity) in a set of cultures that vary widely in their temporal focus (American, Spanish,Turkish, Chinese, Moroccan, Serbian, Bosniak and Croatian; total N=1075). The results supported an overall asymmetrytoward the future in all tasks, although it failed to be significant in most cultures when considered on their own. However,only self-continuity showed variations that can be explained by the contrast between past-focused versus future-focusedcultures.

Compositional Neural Machine Translation by Removing the Lexicon from Syntax Tristan Thrush

The meaning of a natural language utterance is largely determined from its syntax and words. Additionally, there isevidence from theories in semantics and neuroscience that humans process an utterance by separating some amount ofknowledge about the lexicon from the knowledge of word order. In this paper, we propose neural units that can enforcethis constraint over an LSTM encoder and decoder. We demonstrate that our model achieves competitive performanceacross a variety of domains including semantic parsing, syntactic parsing, and English to Mandarin Chinese translation. Inthese cases, our model outperforms the standard LSTM encoder and decoder architecture on many or all of our metrics. Todemonstrate that our model achieves a desired partial separation between the lexicon and syntax, we analyze its weightsand explore its behavior when different neural modules are damaged. When damaged, we find that the model displays theknowledge distortions that aphasics are evidenced to have.

Complexity/informativeness trade-off in the domain of indefinite pronouns

The vocabulary of human languages has been argued to support efficient communication by optimizing the trade-offbetween complexity and informativeness (Kemp and Regier, 2012). The argument has been based on cross-linguisticanalyses of vocabulary in semantic domains of content words such as kinship, color, and number terms. The present workextends this analysis to a category of function words: indefinite pronouns (e.g. someone, anyone, no-one, cf. Haspelmath,2001). We establish the meaning space and feature-based representations for indefinite pronouns, and show that indefinitepronoun systems across languages optimize the complexity/informativeness trade-off. This demonstrates that pressuresfor efficient communication shape both content and function word categories, thus tying in with the conclusions of recentwork on quantifiers (Steinert-Threlkeld, 2019). Furthermore, we argue that the trade-off may explain some of the universalproperties of indefinite pronouns, thus reducing the explanatory load for linguistic theories.

Feeling of Competence Affects Children’s Curiosity and Creativity

Creative potential in childhood predicts creative achievementslater in life. But relatively little is known about the factors andprocesses that promote creativity in children. A theoreticalframework by Carr, Kendal, and Flynn (2016) identified sev-eral factors, including curiosity and exploration, that might fa-cilitate creativity and innovation. Building on this framework,we propose another factor – children’s feeling of competence– that might affect both curiosity and creativity. In the presentstudy, 5- to 7-year-olds were induced feelings of high or lowcompetence by solving math problems. Next, they completedthree tasks that measured their curiosity and creativity. Thefindings showed that children who felt more competent ex-plored more on a novel toy and showed better creative prob-lem-solving abilities.

What I Like Is What I Remember: Memory Modulation and Preferential Choice

Memory is a crucial component of everyday decision making, yet little is known about how memory and choice processesinteract, and whether or not established memory regularities persist during memory-based decision making. In this paper,we introduce a novel experimental paradigm to study the differences between memory processes at play in standard listrecall versus in preferential choice. Using computational memory models, fit to data from two pre-registered experiments,we find that some established memory regularities (primacy, recency, semantic clustering) emerge in preferential choice,whereas others (temporal clustering) are significantly weakened relative to standard list recall. Notably, decision-relevantfeatures, such as item desirability, play a stronger role in guiding retrieval in choice. Our results suggest memory processesdiffer across preferential choice and standard memory tasks, and that choice modulates memory by differentially activatingdecision-relevant features such as what we like.

A hierarchical model of metacognition

I present a novel method of conceptualizing metacognition in a computational hierarchy. Metacognition is commonlydescribed as cognition acting on itself, and correlates with enhanced performance in memory, reasoning, emotional reg-ulation, and motor skills. Understanding metacognition requires surmounting two barriers: its high-level abstraction anddisputed terminology. To overcome these barriers I employ a computational cognitive architecture to first define the baseunits of cognition and how they come to act on themselves. Well-defined computational units are built up into a hierarchyof cognitive processes. These forms of cognition are then connected back to clarify the research literature. Each formis built into working models within ACT-R to support this hierarchical systems viability. The intention of this hierar-chical model is to help clarify the nature of metacognition by supplementing verbal cognitive definition with rigorouscomputational terminology.

Measuring memory integration: A metric tapping memory representation ratherthan inference

Our ability to link related events could be supported either byconnecting their representations in memory, or by storing themseparately but integrating their content when later drawinginferences. Here, we adapted classic memory contingencyanalyses to develop and validate an integration index designedto tap stored representations. We conducted three pre-registered experiments adopting this metric. We found positiverecall dependency for associations experienced both within thesame and across different events. Compared to a conventionalinference test, we found that recall dependency was moresensitive to a manipulation of memory integration. Leveragingrecall dependency to investigate individual differencesrevealed that better memory for contextual detail wasassociated with faster inference judgments, consistent withhigh-fidelity representations of related memories—but only forpeople who tended to store memories separately. Ourapproach, thus, provides an important tool to illuminate howrelated events are represented in memory.

Habitual Sleep Quality Moderated the Effects of Sleep Deprivation on EmotionRegulation by Third-Person Self Talk: Event-Related Potential (ERP) andBehavioral Findings

The current study investigated the impact of sleep deprivation (SD) on the use of third-person self-talk, a relatively effort-less strategy, to regulate emotion. Twenty-four participants (age = 22.75 2.68, 54.17% male, 33.33% good sleepers) com-pleted a cue-picture ERP paradigm after normal sleep and SD conditions, in which they viewed negative or neutral stimuliand reflected on their feelings using either the pronoun I or their name (third-person). We calculated post-instruction latepositive potential (LPP) that has been found sensitive to emotion regulation strategies and closely related to amygdalaactivity. While poor sleepers showed greater LPP amplitudes overall, F(1,7) = 17.50, p = .004, SD only increased the LPPfor negative picture trials among good sleepers but not poor sleepers, F(1,7) = 5.37, p = .054, suggesting that the effect ofSD on emotion regulation using third-person self talk was moderated by habitual sleep quality.

Spontaneous and Voluntary Analogical Retrieval During Problem-Solving andHypothesis Generation

Theoretical models of analogical retrieval implicitly assumethat the cognitive system continuously scans long-term memorybased on the contents of working memory (WM). Experiment 1revealed that when a target analog is presented in the context ofa problem-solving activity, a prompt to search for analogoussituations adds nothing over-and-above the probabilities ofbeing spontaneously reminded of an analogous problem.More exploratory in nature, Experiment 2 presents the firstexperimental evidence of analogical retrieval during hypothesisgeneration. Our prompt to search for analogous phenomenaincreased access to distant analogs, suggesting that hypothesis-generation does not reliably elicit a search for analogousphenomena. Results suggest that a search for analogous casesis not automatically triggered by the contents of WM, and thatthe nature of the tasks in which the analogs are embeddeddetermines whether a search for analogs will be initiated.

“We Need to Start Thinking Ahead”:The Impact of Social Context on Linguistic Norm Adherence

Human dialogue is governed by communicative norms thatspeakers are expected to follow in order to be viewed as coop-erative dialogue partners. Accordingly, for language-capableautonomous agents to be effective human teammates they mustbe able to understand and generate language that complieswith those norms. Moreover, these linguistic norms are highlycontext sensitive, requiring autonomous agents to be able tomodel the contextual factors that dictate when and how thosenorms are applied. In this work, we consider three key lin-guistic norms (directness, brevity, and politeness), and exam-ine the extent to which adherence to these norms varies underchanges to three key contextual factors (potential for harm, in-terlocutor authority, and time pressure). Our results, based ona human-subject study involving 5,642 human utterances, pro-vide strong evidence that speakers do indeed vary their adher-ence to these norms under changes to these contextual factors.

Sixteen-month-olds comprehend unanchored absent reference

A nascent understanding of absent reference emerges around 12months: provided with rich contextual support, infants look and pointto the location of a displaced object. When can infants understandabsent reference without contextual support? Using a proceduremodified from Hendrickson and Sundara (2017), 13- and 16-month-olds first listened to utterances containing familiar target words, whileviewing a checkerboard. Then, two objects – a referent and a distractor(e.g., a cup and a shoe) – appeared on the screen. Only 16-month-oldsdemonstrated a reliable looking preference for the referents, suggestingthat listening to the utterances activated their mental images of thereferents. These results establish that at 16 months, infants comprehendreference to absent entities without any contextual support.

Reasoning About Hidden Features: Individual Differences and Age-RelatedChange

Throughout development, humans infer unobserved properties of the objects they encounter. However, it is often ambigu-ous whether these inferences result from category-based reasoning or overall similarity to previously observed objects. Inthis study, we examined inferences about hidden properties in four-year-old children (N=36) and adults (N=44). We taughtparticipants three categories of artificial creatures. Each category had one critical feature, where one of its variations wasmore common to members of the category, while the other was more common overall. We found that, on average, bothgroups used within-category frequency to predict the value of an unseen critical feature. However, individual differencesrevealed distinctions between the groups. While adults who used within-category frequency for critical items used overallfrequency for other items, this correlation was qualitatively reversed in children. This suggests that some children weresensitive to category knowledge when predicting unseen features, but others likely used a novelty heuristic.

Implicit learning of purely non-linguistic sequences: the role of Brocas area

The relevance of Broca’s area to both language and non-linguistic sequence processing is well established. However, manyof the previous fMRI studies on artificial grammar learning use letter sequences as their non-linguistic stimuli. Since theletters are linguistic in nature, these may inadvertently activate language circuits independent of the artificial grammar. Inaddition, participants have been explicitly told before testing that they needed to classify sentences as either grammaticalor ungrammatical. Thus, it is possible that part or all of the activation of Broca’s reported in these studies is an artifact ofthese manipulations. In our current study, we used sequences of human faces instead of letters, and tested participants insuch a way that they were never aware they were even being tested. Nevertheless, most participants still showed evidenceof learning the non-linguistic artificial grammar, and their Broca’s area was also differentially active for ’grammatical’ vs.’ungrammatical’ sequences.

Reducing the illusion of explanatory depth: A new approach to boostingintervention

This study demonstrates a new approach for boosting peoples decision-making abilities. Previous studies have demon-strated that those who have less knowledge are more likely to make a scientifically biased decision. Therefore, we canexpect that providing them with more knowledge can reduce their biases. However, since people have difficulty changingtheir minds in response to knowledge that contradicts their opinions, it is unclear whether people accept and appropriatelyunderstand the provided knowledge. To more efficiently help these people, this study focused on the illusion of explanatorydepth, which means that the knowledge people think they have is greater than the knowledge they actually possess. Weconducted two experiments and demonstrated that (i) those who had a stronger illusion of explanatory depth were morelikely to make a logically biased decision, and (ii) by informing them of their illusion and providing them with objectiveknowledge, we could reduce their bias.

You Should Really Think This Through: Cross-Domain Variation in Preferences for Intuition and Deliberation

Decisions are often better when pursued after deliberation and careful thought. So why do we so often eschew deliberation, and instead rely on more intuitive, gut responses? We suggest that in addition to well-recognized factors (such as the costs of deliberation), people hold normative commitments concerning how decisions ought to be made. In some cases (e.g., when choosing a romantic partner), relying on deliberation (over intuition) could be seen as inauthentic or send a problematic social signal. In Experiment 1 (N = 654), we show that people in fact hold such domain-sensitive processing commitments, that they are distinct from reported descriptive tendencies, and that they contribute to predicting reported choice. In Experiment 2 (N = 555), we show that choosing intuitively vs. deliberately supports different inferences concerning confidence and authenticity, with the domain variation in inferences in Experiment 2 closely tracking the domain variation in normative commitments observed in Experiment 1. In Experiment 3 (N = 1002), we rule out an alternative explanation. These findings inform theories of judgment and decision-making, as well as efforts towards improving decision-making through critical thinking.

Ambiguity in Text Messages: “I Hate You for Using Emojis Inconsistently With Your Text in WhatsApp ”

This study investigates whether incongruency of valences between emoji and text in texting will promote stronger negative inference in readers. An experiment assessed participants’ judgments of the text messages by recording their response times and perceived valence from the messages (either positive or negative) under the following manipulations: positive or negative messages paired with an emoji that convey positive, negative or ambiguous/neutral emotions (i.e. the pairing of emojis and test may be congruent or incongruent in their valences). Compared with congruent text messages, we found that incongruency between emojis and texts promoted stronger negative inference and elicited a longer processing time, even in texts that conveyed a positive meaning or when the emoji itself was ambiguous/neutral. These results suggest that texts and emojis jointly influence the perceived mood of messages, hinting the importance of the effective use of emojis in order to convey intended meanings and emotions efficiently.

How many instances come to mind when making probability estimates?

Sampling-based models, which assume that people remember or simulate instances of events and count outcome propor-tions to make probability judgments, account for many apparent biases in human judgment. The success of such modelsis generally dependent on sample size, as particularly large or particularly small samples are often required for a modelto reproduce effects observed in data. Thus, systematically exploring the actual number of instances that people tend tosample is an important criterion in evaluating model credibility. Here we propose a method of estimating sample size byway of inter-judgment variance. We show through model recovery that this method will reliably recover the correct samplesize and subsequently apply the method to two well-supported models of human probability judgment, Probability TheoryPlus Noise and the Bayesian Sampler. Results indicate, in both cases, that human probability judgments are based on arelatively plausible (¡ 10) number of sampled instances.

Evidence of Self Referential Prioritization on the basis of Visual Features:Attributing Salience to Rule - Learning

Participants show faster and more accurate processing for arbitrary geometrical stimuli if they are paired with a self- rel-evant label (triangle = you). We ask whether participants only form self associations with specific exemplars (triangle,circle, square), or whether they analyse the stimuli in terms of visual features, (for e.g. no. of vertices = 3), and cangeneralise the learned associations with the entire category of the stimuli (say, all triangles). In our experiments, partici-pants showed the self referential advantage not only to previously exposed exemplars of the same category, but also novelstimuli that could be categorised on the basis of similar visual features. Interestingly, they could generalise not only on thebasis of a single rule, but also on the basis of a conjunction of more than one rule. These findings could be extended toexplain social categorisation in the real world through group memberships.

How People Examine Self/Others Learning History

We investigated how people examined the self or anothers learning history using a complex dynamic control task. Thirty-eight undergraduates were assigned to self or self-as-instructed-other conditions. Participants were asked to performthe task twice and describe their thinking as they examined the learning history provided in the second session. Theparticipants in the self condition were provided with their own learning history, whereas those in the self-as-instructed-other condition were presented with their learning history as if it were anothers. We compared their performance on thecontrol and structure tests between conditions. The results showed that performance on the control test improved oversessions regardless of the condition. The results also showed that the participants in the self-as-instructed-other conditionengaged in evaluation or guessing more often than those in the self condition, suggesting that there are differences in howpeople examine a persons learning history depending on its source.

A Computational Model of Learning to Count in a Multimodal,Interactive Environment

When learning to count, children actively engage with a varietyof counting tasks and observe demonstrations by more knowl-edgeable others. We investigate how a single neural network-based agent, situated in a multimodal learning environment,can learn from observing such demonstrations to perform mul-tiple number tasks such as counting temporally and spatiallydistributed objects, and a variant of the give-N task. We findthat i. the agent can learn different tasks that require counting,ii. learning progresses in similar stages for different tasks, iii.sequential learning of subtasks aids learning of the full task ofcounting spatially distributed objects, and iv. a mechanism forupdating memory when each object is counted emerges fromlearning the task. The work relies on generic deep learningprocesses in widely used neural network modules rather thanmechanisms specialized for mathematics learning, and pro-vides an architecture in which aspects of a sense of numberemerge from learning several different number related tasks.

The cognition of categorisation: nominal classification systems

Systems of nominal classification act as a functional means of categorisation, yet the number and type of categorieswithin these systems vary considerably across languages. The impact of vastly different classification systems on thecognitive representations of concepts is intriguing. We designed a suite of experiments to compare classifier systems in sixOceanic languages, chosen because their inventory of classifiers ranges from two to 23. Effective categorisation needs tobe informative to maximise communicative efficiency, but also simple to minimise cognitive load. Our sample languagesallow us to investigate the trade-off between the two principles of informativeness and simplicity to shed light on therelative optimality of their classification systems. Results from 122 participants across three experiments (free listing,card sorting, video vignettes) indicate that cognitive salience varies as a function of classifier inventory. We discuss theimplications of these results for the nature of nominal classification.

Does viewing Earth as a person and nature as intentionally designed impact beliefsabout the immorality of environmentally damaging acts?

This experiment explored how attributions of agency to the Earth (psychological and vitalist) and design-based views ofnature impact adults degree of environmental concern. Undergraduates (N=133) were randomly assigned to watch differentvideos. In the Person condition, the video described the Earth as a person with beliefs and desires. In the Animal condition,the Earth was described as a living being with non-intentional survival goals. The Control condition described the Earthas a physical-mechanical object. No significant differences were found between conditions in psychological attributionsto the Earth. However, analyses controlling for condition, gender and design attributions revealed a significant interactionbetween the Person Condition and psychological attributions to the Earth (=.29, p¡0.01): Relative to the Animal condition,participants in the Person condition who described the Earth in more psychological ways also had harsher judgements ofenvironmentally damaging acts. Analyses of the biocentric nature of these justifications are still ongoing.

Unpacking cognitive processes in additive and non-additive multiple-cue tasks

In this project we show how the cognitive processes, and the learning patterns, of participants performing a typicalmultiple-cue learning (MCL) task is affected by the format (numeric or verbal) of the cues and the criterion. In twoexperiments we investigated the hypothesis that the reliance on linear additive integration in MCL-tasks is especiallypronounced when cues are presented in a numeric rather than verbal format. The results support the hypothesis. Withnumeric cues, we replicate previous findings supporting a systematic shift from cue-abstraction and additive integrationof cues when the task is additive, to reliance on exemplar memory when the task is non-additive. However, when cuesare verbal, no systematic shift in cognitive process is evident, with participants in general relying on exemplar memoryregardless of the task structure. Consequently, the numerical format is advantageous for learning in the additive task butat times disadvantageous in the non-additive task.

An empirical investigation of adaptive search in problem solving

Using a novel dataset from the TopCoder platform we investigate solvers search for solutions as well as the role of expertisein shaping their problem solving process. We find that while some solvers on the platform do act according to the win-stay, lose-shift rule, skilled solvers are less likely to rely on this meta-heuristic. Somewhat counter-intuitively we find thatexperts make more smaller changes, that is, they change their solutions more often than non-experts, but when they do,they make smaller changes. This can be explained by the fact that experts seem to be able to come up with a good problemrepresentation early on, that doesnt require large adjustments.

Computational mechanisms for resolving misunderstandings

Imagine discussing yesterdays dinner with a friend: It wasn’t particularly tasty. Your friend concurs, it was very salty!Thinking you were talking about the appetizer (which wasnt salty at all), youre forced to reconsider which course yourfriend was talking about. Was the appetizer salty to her? Was she talking about the main course? People encounter mis-understandings in everyday conversation, yet quickly and seamlessly resolve them. How people do this is an explanatorychallenge: the thing being talked about (i.e., the referent) is often not physically present during the conversation. Hence,theres no easy way for interlocutors to establish common ground via ostensive signaling (e.g., by pointing at the dish). Wedevelop a model of speakers that use pragmatic reasoning to infer the referent inferred by listeners. We explore the perfor-mance of this model using agent-based simulated conversations. The results imply necessary and sufficient conditions forsuccessful updating.

Goal-adaptiveness in children’s cue-based information search

This paper investigates the emergence and development ofchildren’s ability to adapt their information search to differentgoals. In Study 1, 3- to 7-year-olds had to decide whether tostudy the arms or legs of two monsters to predict which wouldsucceed at a throwing vs. jumping challenge. Children’s abil-ity to adaptively select the relevant piece of information andtailor their search to the given goal increased with age, surpass-ing chance level around 4;6. Study 2 investigated additionaladaptation to distributions of, e.g., long arms in the search do-main. Preliminary results confirm the observed developmentaltrend in search adaptiveness and effectiveness, suggesting anability to tailor information search to the relevant distributionsin the environment. These studies provide first insights intothe development of adaptive information search given complexgoals, deepening our understanding of this key aspect of learn-ing, judgment and decision-making.

Automating validation of learning and decision making models using theCogniBench framework

Much of cognitive science is based on constructing, validating, and comparing formal models of the mind. Whereascoming up with new and useful models requires expertise and creativity, validating the proposed models and comparingthem against the state-of-the-art mainly requires a systematic, rigorous approach. The task of model validation is thereforeparticularly well-suited for the types of automation that have propelled other research fields (cf. impact of bioinformaticson biology). Here we propose a model benchmarking framework implemented as an open-source Python package namedCogniBench. Given a set of candidate models (which can be implemented in various languages), experimental obser-vations, and scoring criteria, CogniBench automatically performs model benchmarks and reports the resulting matrix ofscores. We demonstrate the potential of the proposed framework by applying it in the domain of learning and decisionmaking, which poses unique requirements for model validation.

Perseverance in risky goal-pursuit

From founding a new start-up to applying for a big grant, many activities involve pursuing risky goals with stark all-or-nothing outcomes and high uncertainty about the chances of succeeding in ones goal. These endeavors require patientperseverance, where time invested towards achieving a rewarding risky goal also implies the opportunity cost of forgoingsafer alternatives, such as working for a reliable wage with immediate rewards. How do people behave when choosingbetween such risky endeavors and safe alternatives, where the dynamic nature of the task has implications beyond expectedutility maximization? We present a new experimental paradigm, where by manipulating the relative rewards, task uncer-tainty, and the success threshold for achieving the risky goal, we are able to identify the environmental factors influencingperseverance. We then compare human behavior to the optimal strategy, along with a variety of boundedly rational policiesand heuristics that trade-off efficiently between complexity and performance.

Teasing apart encoding and retrieval interference in sentence comprehension:Evidence from agreement attraction

This study investigates interference effects in sentenceprocessing. A parade case involves agreement attraction,where the processing of a number mismatch between a verband its subject is eased by a number-matching lure (*Thekeytarget to the cabinetslure were rusty), relative to sentenceswhere neither noun matches the verb (*The key to the cabinetwere rusty). Existing accounts claim that this effect reflectserror-prone retrieval or misrepresentation of the target.Recently, a third account has been proposed which claims thatthe contrast between the two configurations reflects increaseddifficulty in the second sentence due to feature overwriting inthe encoding (both nouns are singular). We provide resultsfrom two self-paced reading experiments that isolate theeffects of feature overwriting and attraction by manipulatingthe presence of an agreement cue. Results showed a largerdifference within the configurations with a cue, which suggestthat attraction cannot be reduced to feature overwriting.

Modelling the Effect of Monetary Incentives on Recognition Memory

While anticipated rewards have been shown to impart enhancements on memory performance, it remains unclear whetherthese benefits reflect improved encoding or more cautious decision-making. In two experiments, participants (N=47, each)encoded complex videos depicting everyday episodes and were tested for their memory of various details. Importantly,participants were informed that each video was associated with either high (25 cents) or low (1 cent) reward at eitherencoding or retrieval. We found participants were more accurate for questions relating to high reward videos only whenreward information was presented at encoding. Memory performance and response-times were modeled using a driftdiffusion model to assess the effects of reward on decision parameters. The drift rate was found to be significantly largerfor high reward videos when compared to low reward videos, only when reward was presented at encoding. These resultssuggest that reward at encoding enhances memory selectivity for detailed episodic information.

Effects of Causal Determinism on Causal Learning Trajectories

Research on causal learning suggests that people are capable of learning nondeterministic causal relations, but might expectcausal relations to be deterministic in certain contexts. In two experiments, we demonstrated that peoples expectations ofcausal determinism are context-sensitive and can influence causal judgments in a sequential learning task. When the datawere deterministic (100% success) and participants expected the cause to be deterministic, their causal judgments wereat ceiling. When participants expectations were nondeterministic, causal ratings increased with accumulating positiveevidence. When the data were probabilistic (75% success), participants exhibited a high violation-of-expectation effectupon seeing the first failed event when they expected the causal relation to be deterministic, and much less so whentheir expectation was nondeterministic. We built a simple Bayesian model to explain participants violation-of-expectationeffect as a selection between two distinct hypotheses: that the causal relation in question is deterministic, and that it isnondeterministic.

An algorithm for estimating average magnitudes

Representing numbers spatially allows us to more quickly and accurately compute average magnitudes. For instance, abar graph lets us quickly estimate the average height of several values. What algorithm might we implement to find theaverage position of observations in space, and how might we leverage this algorithm for quick numeric estimates? Weasked subjects to estimate either the average spatial location of points on a line or the average value of written integers.We propose an iterative algorithm where the subject 1) makes a noisy estimate of the distance of each observation to avisual reference point, 2) infers the posterior of the average of those distances, and 3) updates the reference point to thenew posterior mean. Our algorithm correctly predicts that subjects accuracy and confidence decrease with the varianceof observations. We further investigate similarities and differences between the fitted models for spatial vs. numericaveraging.

Imitation inhibition training enhances perspective taking in preschoolers

Adults (Keysar et al, 2000) and children (Epley et al, 2004) sometimes commit egocentric errors when interpreting otherscommunication, if the self-perspective differs from the speakers perspective. Training imitation inhibition reduces egocen-tric error in adults (Santiesteban et al., 2011), presumably because it makes salient the distinction between self and other.As managing the self-other perspective difference may undergo developmental changes during preschool years (South-gate, in press), we tested whether a social imitation inhibition training may reduce egocentric mistakes in 3-6-year-oldchildren. Results with n=47 (of n=50 preregistered) children show that the imitation inhibition group selected the object towhich the speaker referred more often than children in a control condition (F(1,35)=5.346, p=.026). However, there wasan interaction with age (F(2,35)=3.805, p=.032): only 4-year-olds, but neither 3- nor 6-year-olds, were more accurate inthe inhibition group. Childrens reaction times and hesitation will be analyzed on the final sample.

Emotion, entropy evaluations and subjective uncertainty

A variety of conceptualizations of psychological uncertaintyexist. From an information-theoretic perspective, probabilisticuncertainty can be formalized as mathematical entropy. Cog-nitive emotion theories posit that uncertainty appraisals andmotivation to reduce uncertainty are modulated by emotionalstate. Yet little is known about how people evaluate proba-bilistic uncertainty, and about how emotional state modulatespeople’s evaluations of probabilistic uncertainty and behaviorto reduce probabilistic uncertainty. We tested intuitive entropyevaluations and entropy reduction strategies across four emo-tion conditions in the Entropy Mastermind game. We used theunified Sharma-Mittal space of entropy measures to quantifyparticipants’ entropy evaluations. Results suggest that manypeople use a heuristic strategy, focusing on the number of pos-sible outcomes, irrespective of the probabilities in the proba-bility distribution. This result is surprising, given that previouswork suggested that people are very sensitive to the maximumprobability when choosing queries on probabilistic classifica-tion tasks. Emotion induction generally increased participants’heuristic assessment. The uncertainty associated with emo-tional states also affected game play: participants needed fewerqueries and spent less time on games in high-uncertainty thanin low-uncertainty emotional states. Yet entropy perceptionswere not related to subjectively reported uncertainty, numer-acy or entropy knowledge, suggesting that entropy perceptionsmay form an independent psychological construct.

Hierarchical temporal organization of speech in children and adolescents whostutter

With 10 to 20 sounds per second, fluent speech requires extremely skilled motor coordination. Therefore, young speakerswith an immature or malfunctioning speech production system may be particularly challenged by the temporal aspects offluent speech. In the present study, we examine nested temporal bout structure (Abney et al., 2014) to investigate howyoung speakers (children 9-12; adolescents 13-17 years old) who do and do not stutter might differ in their temporalorganization of speech during reading. Allan Factor analyses show that nested clustering of peak amplitudes at shorttime-scales (¡ 300 ms) differs between children and adolescents, pointing to developmental differences in the temporalorganization of syllabic structure. Greater nested clustering at longer timescales (¿ 300 ms 10 s) was characteristic ofstuttering, particularly in adolescents whose stutter risks to persist into adulthood. We discuss these findings in light oftheories of stuttering and the acquisition of fluent speech

Passing the Moral Turing Test

The translation problem in moral AI asks how insights into human norms and values can be translated into a form suitablefor implementation in artificial systems. I argue that if my answer to a question about the human mind is right, thenthe translation problem is more tractable than previously thought. Specifically, I argue that we can use principles fromreinforcement learning to study human moral cognition, and that we can use principles from the resulting evaluative moralpsychology to design artificial systems capable of passing the Moral Turing Test (Allen, 2000). I illustrate the core featuresof my proposal by describing one such environment, or gridworld, in which an agent learns to trade-off between monetaryprofit and fair dealing, as characterized in behavioral economic paradigms. I conclude by highlighting the core technicaland philosophical advantages of such an approach for modeling moral cognition more broadly construed.

Semantic chunks save working memory resources: computational and behavioral evidence

It is now well-established that long-term memory (LTM) knowledge, such as semantic knowledge, supports the temporary maintenance of verbal information in working memory (WM). This is for instance characterized by the recall advantage observed for semantically related (e.g. leaf - tree - branch) over unrelated (e.g. mouse - wall - sky) lists of items in immediate serial recall tasks. However, the exact mechanisms underlying this semantic contribution remain unknown. In this study, we demonstrate through a convergent approach involving computational and behavioral methods that semantic knowledge can be efficiently used to save attentional WM resources, thereby enhancing the maintenance of subsequent to-be-remembered items. These results have critical theoretical implications, and support models considering that WM relies on temporary activation within the LTM system.

Optimal Attentional Allocation in the Presence of Capacity Constraints in VisualSearch

There is large agreement among vision scientists that biolog-ical perception is capacity-limited and that attentional mecha-nisms control how that capacity is allocated. Despite the factthat Bayesian models generally do not include capacity limits,many researchers model perceptual attention as the result ofoptimal Bayesian inference. This inconsistency arises becausevision science currently lacks a feasible and principled com-putational framework for characterizing optimal attentional al-location in the presence of capacity constraints. Here, weintroduce such a framework based on rate-distortion theory(RDT), a theory of optimal lossy compression developed in theengineering literature. Our approach defines Bayes-optimalperformance when an upper limit on information processingrate is imposed. Here, we compare Bayesian and RDT ac-counts in a visual search task, and highlight a typical shortcom-ing of unlimited-capacity Bayesian models that is not sharedby RDT models, namely that they often over-estimate task-performance when information-processing demands are in-creased. In this study, we asked human subjects to find eitherone or two targets in a collection of distractors in a single-fixation search task. We predicted relative performance be-tween one- and two-target conditions based on both RDT andBayesian models. Performance differed between conditions ina way that was well accounted for by the capacity-limited RDTmodel but not by the capacity-unlimited Bayesian model.

Perceived Agency of a Social Norm Violating Robot

In this experiment, we investigated how a robot’s violation ofseveral social norms influences human engagement with andperception of that robot. Each participant in our study (n = 80)played 30 rounds of rock-paper-scissors with a robot. In thethree experimental conditions, the robot violated a social normby cheating, cursing, or insulting the participant during game-play. In the control condition, the robot conducted a non-normviolating behavior by stretching its hand. During the game,we found that participants had strong emotional reactions toall three social norm violations. However, participants spokemore words to the robot only after it cheated. After the game,participants were more likely to describe the robot as an agentonly if they were in the cheating condition. These results implythat while social norm violations do elicit strong immediate re-actions, only cheating elicits a significantly stronger prolongedperception of agency.

Preschoolers Are Sensitive to Their Performance Over Time

Tracking one’s performance over time is essential to efficientself-guided learning but it is not clear whether young childrencan accurately monitor their past performance. Here, welooked at whether 4-6-year-olds can use the trajectory of theirpast performance to allocate future resources. Across fourexperiments (N = 274), we found that children were sensitiveto their rate of change in past performance: Children assignedto a condition in which they got better over time were morelikely to take on challenges and teach others than children inconditions where they got worse or stayed the same.Furthermore, children privileged their rate and direction ofchange more than their total or final score. These resultssuggest that young children monitor their rate of improvementand can use this information to guide their future efforts.

Neonatal imitation of caregivers at home: A feasibility pilot

The practical relevance of neonatal imitation for social development has remained largely unaddressed as most studieshave been conducted in highly controlled, laboratory conditions. Utilizing the Lookit online infant experiment platform,we aim to demonstrate the feasibility of measuring neonatal imitation of caregivers in the home environment. Our between-subjects design, adapted from Meltzoff and Moore (1983), focuses on two of the most commonly studied neonatal gestures,tongue protrusion and mouth opening. Caregivers and their newborn are videotaped as caregivers model either gestureto their newborn. Coders, who are blind to condition, record newborns gesture frequencies. To analyze these data,we ultimately plan to specify a Bayesian hierarchical log-linear model testing whether the frequency of each neonatalgesture increased when caregivers modeled that specific gesture. Pilot data collection and behavioral coding are currentlyunderway and will focus on inter-rater reliability, attrition, and recruitment rates of online data collection for neonatalimitation.

Do Infants Think That Agents Choose What’s Best?

The naïve utility calculus theory of early social cognitionargues that by relating an agent’s incurred effort to the expectedvalue of a goal state, young children and infants can reasonabout observed behaviors. Here we report a series ofexperiments that tested the scope of such utility-basedreasoning adopted to choice situations in the first year of life.We found that 10-month-olds (1) did not expect an agent toprefer a higher quantity of goal objects, given equal action cost(Experiment 1) and (2) did not expect an agent to prefer a goalitem that can be reached at lower cost, given equal rewards(Experiment 2a and 2b). Our results thus suggest that younginfants’ utility calculus for action understanding may be morelimited than previously thought in situations where an agentfaces a choice between outcome options.

WG-A: A Framework for Exploring Analogical Generalization andArgumentation

Reasoning about analogical arguments is known to be subjectto a variety of cognitive biases, and a lack of clarity aboutwhich factors can be considered strengths or weaknesses ofan analogical argument. This can make it difficult both to de-sign empirical experiments to study how people reason aboutanalogical arguments, and to develop scalable tutoring toolsfor teaching how to reason and analyze analogical arguments.To address these concerns, we describe WG-A (Warrant Game— Analogy), a framework for people to analyze analogical ar-guments based on Bartha’s (2010) Articulation Model of ana-logical argumentation. We carry out two experiments designedto probe WG-A’s effectiveness in improving participants’ abil-ity to reason about analogical arguments and argumentation ingeneral, and argue that WG-A is a promising approach, thoughit is in need of further development.

Recognition memory influenced by grammar

The validity of verbal working memory depends on language experience-independent capacities. We tested how grammat-ical knowledge impacts memory in the absence of overt language production and while controlling for semantic meaningof word pair stimuli. Native English speakers (n=129) completed: (1) ratings of unattested noun-noun compounds (e.g.ice-wallet) on meaningfulness; the (2) Author Recognition Test, measuring language experience; and (3) an old/newrecognition task, where previously presented noun-noun compounds appeared in either old (ice-wallet) or new (wallet-ice)orders. Order of nouns in compounds either resembled order consistently found in English (i.e. typical noun modifier +typical head noun) or was reversed. If grammatical knowledge affects verbal working memory, consistency with natu-ral language should predict old ratings, controlling for meaningfulness ratings and old status. As predicted, participantswere more likely to rate consistent compounds as old compared to reversed. All analyses pre-registered on OSF prior toexperimenter access to data.

Examining a developmental pathway of early word learning: From qualitative characteristics of parent speech, to sustained attention, to vocabulary size

The quality of parent speech has been argued to impact child language growth above and beyond quantity. One potential mechanism tying online experience to long-term vocabulary development is sustained attention to targets of parent speech. We recruited thirty-five parent-toddler dyads to participate in free toy play while wearing head-mounted eye trackers. Parent speech was categorized based on its referential nature, syntax, and communicative intent. Parent referential speech positively related to both vocabulary size and online patterns of sustained attention. Speech categorized based on communicative intent also showed relations with vocabulary size and sustained attention, but specific types of speech impacting each differed. These results support the hypotheses that qualitative characteristics of parent speech relate to both long-term language growth and online sustained attention and provide tentative evidence for the broader hypothesis that sustained attention is the mechanism tying online experience to long- term language growth.

Classification of cognitive problem-solving strategies using MVPA on pre-solutionEEG data

There are two strategies that can be employed to solve a problem: analysis and insight. Analysis is the incremental,conscious search for a solution, as in hypothesis testing; insight involves the unconscious restructuring of a problemrepresentation followed by the sudden, conscious realization of the solution (Aha! phenomenon). We attempted to discoverfeatures of neural activity during problem solving that could predict which type of cognitive strategy people used on eachtrial of an anagram task. We used Multivariate Pattern Analysis (MVPA) on 64-channel pre-solution EEG recording thathas been time-frequency transformed. Searchlight was employed in which neighboring time-frequency points within asliding window were used to train a Naive-Bayesian classifier across electrodes to determine the features with the bestclassification accuracy. In addition, Support Vector Machine was trained using principal components, which resulted inimproved classification accuracy than Searchlight, suggesting more distributed nature of informative features in the data.

Children affirm the possibility of improbable events when they are similar to a known event

Children often judge that strange and improbable events are impossible, whereas adults usually accept the possibility of such events. This shows that children’s reasoning about possibility is immature, but it remains unclear how children reason about the possibility of improbable events. We explore whether children use a novel event’s similarity to a known event to infer whether the event can happen. We told 4- to 6- year-olds (N=120) either ordinary or improbable facts and then asked if a related improbable event was also possible. The facts contained no causal information that could be extended to the occurrence of a similar event. Children who heard improbable facts more often agreed that similar improbable events were possible than children who heard ordinary facts. This suggests that the mere knowledge that an event can happen influences children’s beliefs about the possibility of other unfamiliar-but- similar events.

Investigating the Benefits of Pre-Questions on Lecture-Based Learning

Prior laboratory research has shown the positive benefits of answering pre-questions on learning. Specifically, pre-questions have been shown to increase learning from subsequent pre-questioned material presented either in a readingor in a lecture format compared to a non-pre-questioned group. However, it is not yet clear whether these learning bene-fits translate into larger lecture-based classrooms and whether they can facilitate transfer to non-pre-questioned material.Moreover, there are few classroom studies, utilizing pre-questions, that explore these effects. We investigated the effectof pre-questions on learning during a large lecture course. Students who received pre-questions performed better on endof lecture quiz questions compared to students who did not receive pre-questions. Consistent with prior laboratory andclassroom studies, this effect was primarily for the pre-questioned information and there was no immediate effect onnon-pre-questioned information. We discuss the implications of the results for theories of learning and applications toeducation.

A Computational Approach to Perception and Language in Autism Based onSelf-Organizing Maps

A Self-Organizing Map (SOM) is a type of artificial neural network. Artificial neurons in the SOM form local assembliesthat become specialized in responding to categories of stimuli. Assemblies emerge through competition and cooperationbetween artificial neurons. Here we present a SOM aimed to model autism by means of increasing cooperation betweenneurons in the map. Descriptions of local hyperconnectivity in neuronal circuits in ASD make our implementation bio-logically sound. Remarkably, the change in low-level processing of our model, led to high level atypicalities mirroringASD behavior. Increasing cooperation produced deficient organization of neuronal assemblies accounting for fragmentedrepresentations of perceptual categories, idiosyncratic use of word labels, and atypical shape bias in lexical development.The results of our model successfully matched the behavioral performance of children with ASD in a categorization task,and shed light on how to understand the atypical development of the neurocognitive profile of ASD.

Efficient navigation using a scalable, biologically inspired spatial representation

We present several experiments demonstrating the efficiencyand scalability of a biologically inspired spatial representationon navigation tasks using artificial neural networks. Specifi-cally, we demonstrate that encoding coordinates with SpatialSemantic Pointers (SSPs) outperforms six other proposed en-coding methods when training a neural network to navigate toarbitrary goals in a 2D environment. The SSP representationnaturally generalizes to larger spaces, as there is no definitionof a boundary required (unlike most other methods). Addition-ally, we show how this navigational policy can be integratedinto a larger system that combines memory retrieval and self-localization to produce a behavioural agent capable of findingcued goal objects. We further demonstrate that explicitly incor-porating a hexagonal grid cell-like structure in the generationof SSPs can improve performance. This biologically inspiredspatial representation has been shown to be able to producespiking neural models of spatial cognition. The link betweenSSPs and higher level cognition allows models using this rep-resentation to be seamlessly integrated into larger neural mod-els to elicit complex behaviour.

Can Group Knowledge Diversity be Created On-the-Fly?:Effects of Collaboration Task Design on Performance and Transfer

Research on human collaboration has suggested thatknowledge diversity improves group performance in complextasks such as design, problem solving and forecasting.However, in educational settings it is important to also askwhether learning and transfer for individuals within the groupis enhanced or hindered by diversity in collaborative workgroups. We compare performance in a transportation networkdesign task for two types of collaborative groups, andcompare their performance to that of individuals. In onegroup condition (Distributed Knowledge) each dyad memberhas been trained on a different subtask of a complex jointdesign problem in advance of the collaborative activity. Thesedifferent training tasks should predispose the two dyadmembers to adopt different perspectives, issues, and designstrategies, thus generating greater cognitive diversity for thegroup. In the other group condition (Shared Knowledge) bothdyad participants experienced the same training involvingboth subtasks. Task performance results show a group versusindividual advantage in performance, but a non-significantdifference in performance between the two group knowledgediversity conditions. The group knowledge manipulation didaffect group process, as measured by time spentcollaborating, number of turns taken, and number of wordsspoken. The findings suggest that group diversity canpromote individual learning and transfer when sufficient timeis allowed for discussion and group work.

Spatial Alignment Facilitates Visual Comparison in Children

Visual comparison is a key process in everyday learning.Matlen et al. (2020) recently proposed the Spatial AlignmentPrinciple, based on the broader work of structure-mappingtheory in comparison. According to the principle, visualcomparison is more efficient when pairs are arranged in directplacement: i.e., so that the visuals are juxtaposed orthogonallyto their structural axes. In this placement (a) the intendedrelational correspondences are readily apparent, and (b) theinfluence of potential competing correspondences isminimized. Thus, this placement should make the relationalalignment maximally easy to notice. The results of a same-different task in adults supported this claim. The current studyasks whether the Spatial Alignment Principle applies inchildren’s visual comparison. 6-year-old children performed asame-different task for visual relational patterns. The resultsindicated that direct placement led to faster and more accuratecomparison, both for concrete same-different matches(matches of both objects and relations) and for purely relationalmatches.

The Temporal Structure of Event Knowledge in the Mind in Relation to AutisticTraits

How the mind represents event knowledge, a persons knowledge of events and situations in the world, is the subject ofcompeting theories. Proposals range from an event being represented as a linear order of activities, to a hierarchical struc-ture of scenes of related activities, or in a more fluid computational framework. Additionally, atypical event knowledgeis thought to correlate with Autism Spectrum Disorder. 140 participants (20 per event) ordered normed activity lists for80 events (e.g., taking money out of an ATM, going to a professional baseball game, baking a cake). Network analysessuggest that the temporal structure of events is rich, not strictly linear, and varies across individuals. Furthermore, wecomputed a consensus ordering for each event from participants activity sequences. We calculated deviations from thatordering for each participant, and correlated deviations with a battery of trait inventories to further investigate differencesamong individuals representations of event structure.

Do Language Effects on Attention Persist in Complex Task Contexts?

Is the influence of language on attention previously found in controlled, single-task lab contexts reduced or absent whenother factors (i.e. goals) influence attention, as in everyday life? The current studies examined whether language effects oneye-movements and recall emerge in richer task conditions. Experiment 1 examined English speakers use of agentive/non-agentive language during scene description on memory for the agent, similar to Fausey and Boroditsky (2011) whilealtering scene complexity and adding eye-tracking. Experiment 2 contrasted the standard describe task with one moretypical of everyday scene processing: predicting what happens next. Eye-tracking results from Experiment 1 supportan influence of language on distribution of attention. However, the absence of a significant memory difference in bothexperiments suggests that the language effect is not robust enough to have a meaningful impact on memory in rich taskconditions. Ultimately, the data suggested even the original effects are difficult to obtain.

Are Polysemy Effects Modulated by Sublexical, Lexical, and Semantic Factors?

Most words are polysemous, denoting related but distinct senses (e.g., chicken referring to an ANIMAL or to FOOD).Jager, Green, and Clelland (2016, LCN) reported facilitatory effects of polysemy on lexical processing that interacted withword frequency and type of task. We undertook a broader investigation of interactions between polysemy and severalsublexical, lexical, and semantic properties of words, to determine whether such interactions could explain inconsistenteffects of polysemy reported in the literature. Estimating degree of polysemy using dictionary sense counts, we studied theinteraction between polysemy and these other properties when predicting performance in lexical decision and semantic cat-egorization mega-studies. We observed interactions between polysemy and both lexical and semantic, but not sublexical,variables. Our results, while not replicating the exact effects reported by Jager and colleagues, highlight the importance ofdeveloping models of semantic ambiguity that take into consideration interactions with other psycholinguistic propertiesof words.

Joint Acquisition of Path and Manner Action Description

The present study examines language patterns in theformation of common ground in collaborative action tasks.Based on the classic Clark and Wilkes-Gibbs’ (1986)paradigm for object descriptions, we examined dialoguebetween pairs of participants as they work cooperatively tomaneuver a remote control car following both manner andpath instructions. Overall, we replicated Clark andWilkes-Gibbs’ (1986) results in the domain of action in thedecline of word count, verb phrases, turn taking, and numberof errors committed, with diminishing returns after one trial.However, we also document specific language reductions inpath related actions, but not in manner related actions. Wesuggest that path actions particularly depend on compositionaldescriptors of the environment, consistent with thecontemporary conceptualization of action (Barsalou, 2009).

Untangling Semantic Similarity:Modeling Lexical Processing Experiments with Distributional Semantic Models.

Distributional semantic models (DSMs) are substantially var-ied in the types of semantic similarity that they output. Despitethis high variance, the different types of similarity are oftenconflated as a monolithic concept in models of behaviouraldata. We apply the insight that word2vec’s representationscan be used for capturing both paradigmatic similarity (sub-stitutability) and syntagmatic similarity (co-occurrence) to twosets of experimental findings (semantic priming and the effectof semantic neighbourhood density) that have previously beenmodeled with monolithic conceptions of DSM-based seman-tic similarity. Using paradigmatic and syntagmatic similaritybased on word2vec, we show that for some tasks and typesof items the two types of similarity play complementary ex-planatory roles, whereas for others, only syntagmatic similar-ity seems to matter. These findings remind us that it is im-portant to develop more precise accounts of what we believeour DSMs represent, and provide us with novel perspectiveson established behavioural patterns.

Characterizing the mechanisms of instructed reinforcement learning with fMRIpattern-similarity analysis

Past work has made conflicting proposals about the mechanisms underlying instructed reinforcement learning (RL)specifically,that prefrontal cortex, representing instruction, either biases, attenuates, or overrides learning signals in the brain. Weleverage the sensitivity of pattern-similarity analysis of fMRI data to distinguish between the qualitative features of theseaccounts. Participants learn the value of six novel stimuli after receiving false information that one is of high value. Wetrack markers of value learning in visual cortex during a value-independent perceptual judgement task presented betweenintervals of RL. We predict that with learning, the correlation between activation patterns for similarly valued stimuli willincrease. To characterize influences on learning, we examine how the rate at and direction in which these patterns changein similarity will be influenced by explicit instruction about stimulus value. This work will help us identify the principlecognitive and neural mechanisms underlying instructed RL.

Promoting Pro-Climate Actions: A Cognitive-Constraints Approach

Most Americans do not view climate change as an imminent threat. The present paper harnessed the power of two cog-nitive constraints essential to belief formation and revision coherence and causal invariance to guide the developmentof educational materials to foster pro-climate actions. Building on insights from philosophy, cognitive psychology, andanthropology, our materials presented questions on a range of everyday and otherwise personally relatable events to par-ticipants in 10 U.S. states with the highest level of climate skepticism. Participants answered the questions, explainedtheir answers, and received feedback featuring scientific explanations. The latter typically deviate from participants own(invoking the causal-invariance constraint), and are more coherent (invoking the coherence constraint). In support of ourapproach, although our intervention materials did not mention climate change or mitigating actions, they raised willingnessto take pro-climate actions, but did so only when the components hypothesized to enable a coherent pro-climate-actionnarrative were included.

The attentional demands of learning by doing: A developmental study

Research suggests learning by doing yields better outcomes than passive instructional activities (e.g., reading). Currently,the attentional demands of learning by doing are not well understood, which has important implications for youngerlearners. We investigated the developmental trajectory of learning by doing with eighty-five primary students (Mage=6.64years) who listened to a lesson about insects. Participants were presented with contrasting animal-pairs (e.g., ant—pillbug)and learned about insect features. Attention to the lesson was measured as the proportion of time fixating on the lesson. Apost-test assessed recall for lesson content and transfer. First-graders exhibited comparable recall after passive and activepractice, whereas kindergarteners benefited from passive practice. Interestingly, for transfer items first-graders benefitedfrom passive practice whereas kindergarteners benefited from active practice. Transfer performance was related to learnersattention during the task suggesting that learning by doing might depend on the development of attention.

Interactions Between Categorization and Intuitive Physics

Functioning in the world requires information about objects properties. People perceive object mass using perceptualcues when the material is observable. Here, we examine how people predict an objects motion when its material isunobservable, but predictable from cues learned via category learning. When given an ambiguous object, people tend topredict properties based on the propertys propensity in the most likely category. But, recent work has found that givenan ambiguous cue, people will integrate over categories (as rational agents should) in a variety of contexts. In our study,we investigate how uncertainty in categorization affects continuous judgments in the domain of intuitive physics. Weincorporate real materials (like wood and iron) into a category learning framework and test peoples judgments about thedistance a payload travels in two scenarios before and after category learning. Our results are equivocal, but suggest thatpeople do integrate in these scenarios.

From two to many: The role of executive functions in young children’s generalization of novel object names in a comparison design

In this study, 4-year-old children were tested in an object name generalization task with a stimulus comparison design. Performance in the generalization task was correlated with performance in a vocabulary test and three executive function tasks assessing inhibition, flexibility, and working memory. Correlational analyses revealed a significant association with flexibility but not with inhibition, working memory or vocabulary test. We interpret the results in terms of a capacity to flexibly generate novel dimensions rather than inhibiting irrelevant dimensions. Individual differences in working memory and inhibition did not significantly influence performance in the word extension task. Moreover, the absence of correlation with the vocabulary performance supports the idea that children did not rely on existing knowledge to find out the relevant dimension.

The Impact of Mobile Usage Patterns on Risk-Taking Behavior

Among the popular press, excessive smartphone usage is often broadcast as being associated with adverse outcomes, in-cluding greater risk taking, poor social adjustment, and impaired cognitive functioning. However, there is scant empiricalevidence that supports these claims. Our study investigated whether the duration of smartphone ownership (exposure)affects smartphone usage pattern (screen-time), and whether their interaction is associated with risk-taking behavior (Ben-thin Risk Perception questionnaire). We found that those with lower screen-time reported engaging in a higher frequencyof risky activities like vandalism of property, B = -4.80, SE = 1.65, t = -2.91, p ¡ 0.01. Screen-time was inversely asso-ciated with risk taking among individuals characterized by less exposure, B=4.66, SE=2.01, t=2.32, p=0.03. Altogether,these early findings illustrate how the impact of screen-time on real-life behaviors may not be as one-sided as mass mediaportrays.

People view humans as existing for purposes and condemn those who fail to fulfillthem

People often endorse explanations in terms of purposes or goals (e.g., pencils exist so that people can write with them), evenwhen these teleological explanations are scientifically unwarranted (e.g., water exists so that life can survive on Earth). Inthe present research, we explore teleological endorsement in a novel domainhuman purposeand its relationship to moraljudgments. Across two studies, we find evidence that people endorse the claim that humans exist for a purpose (e.g.,to procreate, to help others) and that these beliefs relate to moral judgments against purpose violations (e.g., condemningthose who do not procreate, or do not help others). We also find evidence of a bi-directional causal relationship: teleologicalclaims about a species result in moral condemnation of purpose violations, and stipulating that an action is immoralincreases endorsement that the species exists for that purpose.

Reasoning About Equations with Tape Diagrams: Do Differing Visual FeaturesMatter?

Diagrams are a potentially valuable tool for helping students understand mathematical concepts and procedures. Onetype of diagram that is sometimes used to depict mathematical relationships is tape diagrams, which depict quantitiesin continuous strips. This study investigated whether tape diagrams with different visual features differentially supportreasoning about equations, and explored whether people have preferences for tape diagrams with different visual features.Undergraduates (N = 50) were asked (1) to generate equations to correspond with tape diagrams with varying visualfeatures, and (2) to select the diagram they preferred from pairs that differed in visual features. Variations in visualfeatures (color, presence of outer lines, and position of the constant) did not affect participants success at generatingequations to correspond to the tape diagrams. However, participants displayed systematic preferences for most visualfeatures considered. Future research should examine the effects of these visual features on performance while solvingequations.

Do you see what I see? Children’s understanding of perception and physical interaction over video chat

How do children reason about people presented over video chat? Video chat is a representation, like a picture; but is also a real social interaction (the partner sees and hears you). Do children understand the nuanced affordances and limitations of video chat? We tested 4-year-old children’s reasoning, asking if a person over video chat (vs. a live person; photograph) could see, hear, feel, and physically interact through the screen. Children judged that a person over video chat can see, but cannot feel nor receive an object, through the screen. The person over video chat was judged to hear more often than a photograph, but less often than a live person. Preschool children are not limited to considering a stimulus fully representational, or fully present; instead, they understand video chat as a medium that blurs the boundaries of representation and reality, allowing for a mixture of life-like affordances and picture-like limitations.

Understanding Children’s Speech Productions: Man Versus Machine

Young childrens speech pronunciations deviate systematically from adult forms. For example, onsets are often simplified(e.g., stop becomes top), unstressed syllables frequently deleted (e.g., spaghetti becomes getti), and certain segments arecommonly replaced with other ones (e.g., rice becomes wice). The current study examined how well adults and a popularautomatic speech recognition system (i.e., Siri) deal with these deviations. The same 12 children were recorded producing32 words in isolation at three ages: 2.5, 3.5, and 5.5 years. 12 adults were also recorded. These recordings were presentedto 48 young adults, 7 mothers, and Siri for transcription. All listeners performed worst with 2.5-year-old productions,and humans outperformed Siri with all ages (p ¡ 0.001). Mothers demonstrated the highest accuracy with 2.5-year-oldproductions (86%). Additionally, Siri made distinctive transcription errors with childrens speech. These errors may reflectthe systems lack of training with young childrens voices.

From Tangled Object Manifold to Temporal Relation Manifolds

In this paper, we extended the DiCarlo & Cox 2007 tangled object manifold framework of object recognition to bet-ter address the unsupervised nature of category learning. We developed a novel Markov chain-based similarity metricthat formally connects aspects of manifold untangling with trace learning. Using these developments, we replaced un-observable labels and artificial category boundaries with our observable Markov chain walk based similarity metric as atheoretically grounded target for unsupervised category untangling. Further, we developed a new rationale for how neu-ronal input windows should be chosen for an untangling algorithm using this new framework. This new framework formanifold untangling and trace learning allowed us to synthesize aspects of simple cell learning, complex cell learning,and axonal development theories, into a high-level theory of how the visual cortex learns to separate object categories at acomputational level.

The Same or Different? Capacity Limitations in Visual Imagery versus VisualMemory of Simple Structured Objects

Visual mental imagery and visual memory appear to utilise similar brain networks. However, limited research has in-vestigated how similar the systems are in terms of capacity limits. Capacity limits of visual working memory (VWM)and visual short-term memory (VSTM) have been the focus of considerable research, but to our knowledge none has at-tempted to ascertain the number of objects that can be simultaneously imagined. This study aimed to provide estimatesof imagery capacity and explore how this relates to the capacity of visual memory. Participants completed three tasks thatexplored imagination, VWM and VSTM, respectively. Set size was manipulated similarly in each task enabling modellingof imagination and visual memory capacity. Capacity estimates were similar in the two visual memory tasks and higherthan that of imagination. The relations between these tasks are discussed alongside the theoretical implications about themechanisms underpinning imagery and visual memory.

“Girls Are as Good as Boys” Implies Boys Are Better,But Only in the Absence of Explicit Awareness

The statement “Girls are as good as boys at math” appears toexpress gender equality, but research has shown that peopleinfer a gender difference from such statements: the group inthe complement position (boys) is judged to be superior. Arepeople aware that the syntax of these statements influencestheir judgments and do these framing effects generalize toother groups and inferences? We addressed these questions byreplicating and extending previous work, showing that (1)syntactic framing effects extend to politically chargedinferences about religious groups and terrorism, and (2) themajority of people recognize subject-complement statementsas influential in their judgments, but framing effects are foundonly in those who fail to recognize this influence. Those whodo cite this syntax as influential tend to show a reverseframing effect, suggesting they may be sensitive to the biasimplicit in such statements and consciously act to resist it.

Adaptive vs. Fixed Spacing of Learning Items:Evidence from Studies of Learning and Transfer in Chemistry Education

Spacing presentations of learning items across time improvesmemory relative to massed schedules of practice – thewell-known spacing effect. Spaced practice can be furtherenhanced by adaptively scheduling the presentation of learningitems to deliver customized spacing intervals for individualitems and learners. ARTS - Adaptive Response-time-basedSequencing (Mettler, Massey, & Kellman 2016) determinesspacing dynamically in relation to each learner’s ongoing speedand accuracy in interactive learning trials. We demonstrate theeffectiveness of ARTS when applied to chemistry nomenclaturein community college chemistry courses by comparing adaptiveschedules to fixed schedules consisting of continuouslyexpanding spacing intervals. Adaptive spacing enhanced theefficiency and durability of learning, with learning gainspersisting after a two-week delay and generalizing to astandardized assessment of chemistry knowledge after 2-3months. Two additional experiments confirmed and extendedthese results in both laboratory and community college settings.

Principled connections guide semantic feature production

When people think about the features of scissors, they often spontaneously recall a central feature of scissors: they cutthings. They tend not to recall other features of scissors, e.g., that they have handles. The present paper posits a novelexplanation for the behavior: the features people recall first and most often reflect semantic generalizations of kinds. Arecent taxonomy of such generalizations suggests that people represent privileged links between kinds and their featuresknown as principled connections (Prasada et al., 2013). Principled connections can reflect norms, and one way to diagnosethe presence of a principled connection is to test the acceptability of sentences of the form all normal Xs have featureY, as in all normal cars have four wheels. We tested whether participants accept generalizations about the normality offeatures produced in a semantic feature production task. Two experiments provided participants with generalizations aboutfeatures listed first and most often as well as features that people list less frequently. Both experiments found that peoplereadily accepted generalizations about the normality of frequently produced features. The results corroborate the view thatprincipled connections help people recall the features of conceptual categories.

Integration of visual and spoken cues in a virtual reality navigation task

When integrating information in real time from multiplemodalities or sources, such as when navigating with the helpof GPS voice instructions along with a visual map, a decision-maker is faced with a difficult cue integration problem. Thetwo sources, in this case visual and spoken, have potentiallyvery different interpretations or presumed reliability. Whenmaking decisions in real time, how do we combine cues com-ing from visual and linguistic evidence sources? In a sequenceof three studies we asked participants to navigate through aset of virtual mazes using a head-mounted virtual reality dis-play. Each maze consisted of a series of T intersections, ateach of which the subject was presented with a visual cue and aspoken cue, each separately indicating which direction to con-tinue through the maze. However the two cues did not alwaysagree, forcing the subject to make a decision about which cueto “trust.” Each type of cue had a certain level of reliability(probability of providing correct guidance), independent fromthe other cue. Subjects learned over the course of trials howmuch to follow each cue, but we found that they generallytrusted spoken cues more than visual ones, notwithstandingthe objectively matched reliability levels. Finally, we showhow subjects’ tendency to favor the spoken cue can be mod-eled as a Bayesian prior favoring trusting such sources morethan visual ones.

Vowel raising in Bengali inflectional morphology: Interactions of orthography andphonology

We investigate the processing of inflected verbs in Bengali. The word forms involve an interaction of orthography andphonology: the 1st Person singular is formed from the 3rd Person by adding the suffix /-i/. For stem vowels [, e, , o]this causes the stem vowel to be raised. For [e, o] this is reflected orthographically, but not for [,]. We examine thisin a cross-modal priming study and an eye tracking task where an auditory first-syllable fragment is matched to eitherthe 1st or 3rd Person visual form. We show that orthography plays an important role, with mismatching forms beingless effective as primes, and fragment completion being easier for patterns with different orthography. For words withno orthographic difference, manual responses to fragment completion were at chance, but eye tracking revealed distinctmatch vs. mismatch processing. We discuss implications for roles of orthography and phonology in lexical access.

A Biologically Plausible Spiking Neural Model ofEyeblink Conditioning in the Cerebellum

The cerebellum is classically described in terms of its role inmotor control. Recent evidence suggests that the cerebellumsupports a wide variety of functions, including timing-relatedcognitive tasks and perceptual prediction. Correspondingly,deciphering cerebellar function may be important to advanceour understanding of cognitive processes. In this paper, webuild a model of eyeblink conditioning, an extensively studiedlow-level function of the cerebellum. Building such a modelis of particular interest, since, as of now, it remains unclearhow exactly the cerebellum manages to learn and reproducethe precise timings observed in eyeblink conditioning that arepotentially exploited by cognitive processes as well. We em-ploy recent advances in large-scale neural network modelingto build a biologically plausible spiking neural network basedon the cerebellar microcircuitry. We compare our simulationresults to neurophysiological data and demonstrate how therecurrent Granule-Golgi subnetwork could generate the dynam-ics representations required for triggering motor trajectoriesin the Purkinje cell layer. Our model is capable of reproduc-ing key properties of eyeblink conditioning, while generatingneurophysiological data that could be experimentally verified.

Cardinal Direction Knowledge in 6-12-year-old Children

Cardinal directions refer to the four main points of direction in geographical space: north, south, east, and west. Efficientnavigation requires some basic knowledge about cardinal directions. We evaluated developmental changes in cardinaldirection knowledge in real space. Tested in an unfamiliar indoor environment with a window view, 94 children aged 6-12years old were asked to point to North and then point to East. We proposed 7 developmental stages based on knowingthe horizontal plane of cardinal directions, the inter-relationships between them, and how to identify north using referenceframes. Our classification scheme classified all participants and was sensitive to age differences. Our results suggested thatidentifying north was more difficult than knowing the inter-relationships. Many children were not able to use an allocentricreference frame effectively. Overall, our study demonstrates the utility of our classification scheme and the importance ofevaluating cardinal direction knowledge development in children.

Redder reds, redder purples, but not redder blues: color gradability knowledgeamong blind and sighted adults

A key characteristic of color perception is that it both categorical and continuous. This is reflected in graded color ad-jective use. This red fruit is redder than the other red fruit sounds more natural than this red fruit is redder than the bluefruit (Kennedy & McNally, 2010). We examined the contribution of first-person sensory experience to color gradabilityunderstanding by working with congenitally blind adults. Blind (n=20) and sighted (n=15) adults rated the naturalness ofstatements describing two objects of the same color (two red mugs), dissimilar colors (red mug, blue mug) or similar col-ors (red mug, purple mug). Both groups judged redder as most natural for two red objects, least for objects with differentcolors (red/blue) and intermediate for objects with similar colors (red/purple). Color similarity had a larger effect for thesighted group. Understanding color gradability does not require first-person perception.

Reliable Idiographic Parameters From Noisy Behavioral Data: The Case ofIndividual Differences in a Reinforcement Learning Task

Behavioral data, though has been an influential index oncognitive processes, is under scrutiny for having poorreliability as a result of noise or lacking replications ofreliable effects. Here, we argue that cognitive modeling canbe used to enhance the test-retest reliability of the behavioralmeasures by recovering individual-level parameters frombehavioral data. We tested this empirically with theProbabilistic Stimulus Selection (PSS) task, which is used tomeasure a participant’s sensitivity to positive or negativereinforcement. An analysis of 400,000 simulations from anAdaptive Control of Thought - Rational (ACT-R) model ofthis task showed that the poor reliability of the task is due tothe instability of the end-estimates: because of the way thetask works, the same participants might sometimes end uphaving apparently opposite scores. To recover the underlyinginterpretable parameters and enhance reliability, we used aBayesian Maximum A Posteriori (MAP) procedure. We wereable to obtain reliable parameters across sessions (IntraclassCorrelation Coefficient ~ 0.5), and showed that this approachcan further be used to provide superior measures in terms ofreliability, and bring greater insights into individualdifferences.

Student Learning Trajectories and Knowledge Transfer in Early MathematicalEquivalence Interventions

Many students fail to develop adequate understanding of mathematical equivalence in early grades, which impacts lateralgebra learning. Work from McNeil and colleagues proposes that this failure is partly due to the format of traditionalinstruction and practice with highly similar problems, which encourages students to develop ineffective mental models ofproblem types (McNeil, 2014, McNeil & Alibali, 2005). In the current study, we explore students learning trajectoriesin two matched equivalence interventions. We show that, relative to an active control, the principle-based treatmentintervention gives rise to a greater number of successful learners, a designation that, in turn, leads to improved performanceon distal transfer assessments. We further demonstrate a predictive relationship between students engagement with theintervention, via workbook completion, and likelihood of becoming a successful learner. Our findings have implicationsfor early detection of learning and subsequent scaffolding for low-performing students.

Can toddlers learn causal action sequences?

Toddlers, like older children and adults, can learn cause-effect relationships between a single action and its outcome.However, causality in the real-world is more complex. We investigate whether toddlers can learn, from observing an adultsdemonstration, that a sequence of two actions is causally necessary for producing an effect. In Experiment 1, toddlers andpreschoolers (N=142; ongoing) saw evidence that a 2-action sequence was necessary to make a puzzle-box dispense asticker, before trying to get stickers themselves. Preliminary results indicate that older children produce more sequencesthan younger children. Experiment 2 (N=42; ongoing) is examining whether 1- and 2-year-olds behave differently from inExperiment 1 when the demonstration provides evidence that a sequence of actions is not necessary (specifically, that thesecond action alone is causally effective). Although preliminary, our findings suggest that the ability to accurately infercausal structure from action sequence demonstrations may develop over early childhood.

Schrödinger’s Category: Active Learning in the Face of Label Ambiguity

Research on active category learning—i.e., where the learnermanipulates continuous feature dimensions of novel objects andreceives labels for their self-generated exemplars—has routinelyshown that people prefer to sample from regions of the space withhigh class uncertainty (near category boundaries). Prevailingaccounts suggest that this strategy facilitates an understanding of thesubtle distinctions between categories. However, prior work hasfocused on situations where category boundaries are rigid. Inactuality, the boundaries between natural categories are often fuzzyor unclear. Here, we ask: do learners pursue uncertainty samplingwhen labels at the boundary are themselves uncertain? To answerthis question, we introduce a fuzzy buffer around a target categorywhere conflicting labels are returned from two ‘teachers,’ then weevaluate how sampling and representation are affected. We find that,relative to the rigid boundary case, learners avoid uncertainty,opting to sample densely from highly certain regions of the targetcategory as opposed to its boundary. Subsequent generalization testsreveal that the sampling strategies encouraged by the fuzzyboundary negatively affected participants' grasp of categorystructure, even outside the fuzzy buffer zone.

Chaining and the process of scientific innovation

A scientist’s academic pursuit can follow a winding path.Starting with one topic of research in earlier career, one maylater pursue topics that relate remotely to the initial point.Philosophers and cognitive scientists have proposed theoriesabout how science has developed, but their emphasis is typi-cally not on explaining the processes of innovation in individ-ual scientists. We examine regularity in the emerging order of ascientist’s publications over time. Our basic premise is that sci-entific papers should emerge in non-arbitrary ways that tend tofollow a process of chaining, whereby novel papers are linkedto existing papers with closely related ideas. We evaluate thisproposal with a set of probabilistic models on the historicalpublications from 70 Turing Award winners. We show that anexemplar model of chaining best explains the data among thealternative models, mirroring recent findings on chaining in thegrowth of linguistic meaning.

Neural Correlates of Hand Representation in Virtual Flight Simulation

Virtual reality environments provide valuable opportunities for cognitive scientists to investigate complex cognitive func-tions in ecologically valid environments. For example, it is unclear if visual representation of the users body is requiredto evoke optimal performance. This study examined the effects of hand representation in a virtual flight simulation usingbehavioural and biometric data. Event-Related Potentials, Event-Related Spectral Perturbations, and mental workload re-sponses were measured using wireless electroencephalography across the hand presence conditions. Workload indices andneural activity in the parietal region was not significantly affected by the presence of hands, yet lower alpha levels werefound across all cortical regions. Findings are relevant to cognitive scientists as they show that the virtual representationof hands is important as it increases task engagement, while not taxing mental workload or spatial processes in the brain.

Infants use imitation but not comforting or social synchrony to evaluate those insocial interactions

In order to understand social relationships, humans must recognize cues of affiliation. When infants see interactionsbetween abstract, animated characters, they use imitation, helping, comforting, and exerted effort to predict who willapproach whom. Moreover, infants attend to and reach for characters who imitate other characters and those who helpothers. The present research builds on these findings and asks whether infants reach for human-animated puppets withdistinct and variable human voices who imitate, are imitated by, comfort, are comforted by, or move synchronously with aperson. At 12 months, infants reached more often for puppets who imitate a humans sound, and also for those who werenot targets of imitation. In contrast, infants did not reach more for puppets who comforted or synchronized their motionswith a human actor. By 12 months, therefore, infants show differentiated responses to different acts of social engagementby those whose social interactions they observe as third parties.

Scaling Uncertainty in Visual Perception and Estimation Tasks

Demographic perceptionthe perception of social quantities of geopolitical scale and social significancehas been extensivelystudied in cognitive and political science (Citrin & Sides, 2008; Gilens, 2001; Herda, 2013). Regular patterns of over-and under-estimation emerge which have historically been attributed to social factors such as fear of specific minorities(Gallagher, 2003; Wong, 2007). Other work has suggested that these patterns result from the psychophysics of the percep-tion of proportions (Landy, Guay & Marghetis, 2018). A Bayesian formulation suggests that biases in the estimation ofboth social proportions and simple visual properties result from a common source: hedging uncertain information towarda prior. Similar to work done by Zhang and Maloney (2012), we present a novel lab paradigm and two experiments thatfocus specifically on manipulating uncertainty in a simple (dot estimation) task, verifying the core assumptions of theBayesian approach.

Staying and Returning Dynamics of Sustained Attention in Young Children

Sustained attention is a dynamic process with rich temporalstructure. Eye-tracking provides a tool for capturing rich tem-poral data relevant to sustained attention, but extracting rele-vant insights from this rich data is nontrivial. This paper stud-ies eye-tracking data collected from children, aged 3-5, per-forming the TrackIt task, a visual object tracking paradigm de-signed for studying sustained attention development in youngchildren. Building on a hidden Markov model paradigm re-cently proposed for analyzing eye-tracking data with TrackIt,we explore characterizations of participant behavior, such ascontinuously maintaining attention on an object and transition-ing attention between objects, that provide richer insights thantask performance alone. In particular, our results suggest thatimprovement in TrackIt performance that accompanies devel-opment in this age range may stem more from improved abilityto return to task after distractions, rather than from improve-ments in ability to continuously maintain attention on the task.

Not all Errors are the Same: The Role of Cognitive Effort in Cross-SituationalWord Learning

Errors are usually viewed as detrimental to learning. Yet, recent proposals suggest that errors may create desirable difficul-ties and thereby improve learning. We evaluated these proposals in the context of cross-situational word learning. Duringeach learning trial, adults saw two images and heard two words. In the Error1 condition, the first word was unexpectedbased on prior experience and the second was expected. The referent of the unexpected word could only be establishedafter hearing the expected word. In the Error2 condition, the expected word came first, which made it easier to learn themapping of the subsequent unexpected word. There was no difference between the conditions; however, expected wordswere only learned significantly better than unexpected words in the Error2 condition. This suggests that the structure ofthe learning environment modulates the impact of errors.

Does the effect of labels on sustained attention depend on target familiarity?

The ability to sustain attention on a target in the presence of distractors is critical for learning and development. Recentwork has suggested that labeling a target object facilitates childrens performance in tasks requiring attentional selection,with a proposed mechanism relying on the enhancement of the target representation in working memory. In this pre-registered study, we tested this hypothesis by examining the effect of label familiarity on sustained attention. If labelsinfluence how strongly targets are represented in working memory, then more familiar labels should show a larger benefitrelative to less familiar labels. We discuss the results in the context of theories of language and cognition, and theircontribution to understanding the mechanisms supporting the development of selective sustained attention.

Prior beliefs about the evidentiary weight of crime scene data impacts jurorverdicts

Jurors operate as legal fact-finders, incorporating multiple pieces of evidence into their decisions. Prior work suggestsjurors may not be able to distinguish flaws in scientific evidence (Schweitzer & Saks, 2012) or properly assess the relia-bility of evidence (Thompson, 1989; Kaasa et al., 2007). However, it remains unclear how much probative value is givento individual evidence types. What prior weights do jurors place on different types of evidence and how do these predicttheir decisions? We built a Bayesian model of how people weigh individual pieces of evidence and used this to predictguilt ratings. Consistent with previous work, we found people have trouble distinguishing differences in quality amongevidence, and assign similar probative value to even flimsy types of evidence. The model also revealed individual weight-ing of evidence can exert large, and sometimes problematic effects on decisions to convict. We discuss the implications ofour findings for legal decision-making.

Childrens expectations of reciprocity in referential communication

Speakers often violate conversational expectations by offering less information than listeners need (Grice, 1975). Althoughchildren appear sensitive to such violations as comprehenders (Gweon & Asaba, 2018; Katsos & Bishop, 2011), it isunclear how they would respond to them in a reciprocal conversational setting. Here, we ask whether children tailor theinformativeness of their speech based on the informativeness of an interlocutor in a prior interaction. In an informativenessrating task, 4- and 5-year-old children evaluated the utterances of an informative and an under-informative interlocutor.Then, in a referential communication task, roles were reversed, and children produced referential descriptions for either theinformative or the under-informative interlocutor. Results showed that although children were sensitive to conversationalviolations, they did not tailor their utterances to their interlocutors informativeness. Although preliminary, these findingssuggest that cooperative expectations in linguistic exchanges might differ from those underlying broader (non-linguistic)social action.

Using Experience Sampling to Investigate Affect at Encoding and Episodic Memory

Intensive longitudinal data was collected through theconcurrent use of a passive experience sampling (ES)smartphone application collecting objective measures ofexperience, and an ecological momentary assessment (EMA)app collecting self-reported affect. After a week-longretention interval, participants completed a memory testgenerated from paired ES and EMA data. Participants wereasked to select the GPS location at the time of a paired targetevent from four alternatives. Correct retrieval was notpredicted by self-reports grouped by negative valence/higharousal or negative valence/low arousal. Positivevalence/high arousal reported at encoding predicted greaterprobability of incorrect responses. Conversely, positivevalence/low arousal predicted greater probability of correctidentification of target. At retrieval, choice was predicted bydissimilarities in discrete emotions between target anddistractors, suggesting the use of affect as a contextualmechanism.

Downloading Culture.zip: Social learning by program induction

Cumulative culture depends on the fidelity of learning be-tween successive generations, and the robustness with whichthe lessons of one generation apply to the problems of the next.How do humans accomplish these twin goals? We formalizesocial learning as a kind of program induction, and provide anexperimental test of a key prediction. To do this, we exploit akey fact: When humans learn from others, in addition to ob-serving inputs and outputs we often observe the process thatled to that output. For instance, when preparing a meal, wedon’t just observe a pile of vegetables and then a ratatouille.Instead, we observe a causal process that transforms those in-gredients into a finished food. Here, we use probabilistic pro-grams to represent causal processes and show that the observa-tion of an execution trace speeds up program induction, evenwhen learning from only a single example. This model pre-dicts that the inferences and behavior of people will be struc-tured by these execution traces. In two behavioral experiments,we show that human judgments and behavior are affected bythe execution trace in the systematic ways predicted by our for-mal model. These findings shed light on the mechanisms thatunderlie high fidelity social learning in humans, and unify therole of emulation and imitation in social learning.

The best-laid plans of mice and men: Competition between top-down andpreceding-item cues in plan execution

There is evidence that the process of executing a plannedutterance involves the use of both preceding-context and top-down cues. Utterance-initial words are cued only by the top-down plan. In contrast, non-initial words are cued both bytop-down cues and preceding-context cues. Co-existence ofboth cue types raises the question of how they interact duringlearning. We argue that this interaction is competitive: itemsthat tend to be preceded by predictive preceding-context cuesare harder to activate from the plan without this predictivecontext. A novel computational model of this competition isdeveloped. The model is tested on a corpus of repetitiondisfluencies and shown to account for the influences onpatterns of restarts during production. In particular, this modelpredicts a novel Initiation Effect: following an interruption,speakers re-initiate production from words that tend to occurin utterance-initial position, even when they are not initial inthe interrupted utterance.

Intuitive theories of persuasion shape engagement in discussion of polarizingtopics

Misinformation promoting scientific misconceptions can spread rapidly in ways it once couldn’t, and discussions of thistrend now appear to shape nearly all discourse about polarizing topics (e.g., politics, science denial). What effects mightthis recent trend have on peoples intuitive theories of how others learn and assimilate new evidence? Furthermore, howdo these theories shape engagement in discussion of polarizing issues? To shed light on these questions, we conducteda series of exploratory studies (Experiments = 4; N = 1176) which demonstrate two key results. First, people do notthink that misinformation is more likely to influence people’s beliefs than accurate statistical information, contrary to ourpredictions. Second, and importantly, we found that the more likely someone is to say information (whether accurateor inaccurate) can change other peoples beliefs, the more likely they are to debate important social issues in an effort tocorrect their misconceptions.

Devaluation of Unchosen Options: A Bayesian Account of the Provenance andMaintenance of Overly Optimistic Expectations

Humans frequently overestimate the likelihood of desirableevents while underestimating the likelihood of undesirableones: a phenomenon known as unrealistic optimism. Previ-ously, it was suggested that unrealistic optimism arises fromasymmetric belief updating, with a relatively reduced codingof undesirable information. Prior studies have shown that areinforcement learning (RL) model with asymmetric learningrates (greater for a positive prediction error than a negativeprediction error) could account for unrealistic optimism in abandit task, in particular the tendency of human subjects topersistently choosing a single option when there are multi-ple equally good options. Here, we propose an alternativeexplanation of such persistent behavior, by modeling humanbehavior using a Bayesian hidden Markov model, the Dy-namic Belief Model (DBM). We find that DBM captures hu-man choice behavior better than the previously proposed asym-metric RL model. Whereas asymmetric RL attains a measureof optimism by giving better-than-expected outcomes higherlearning weights compared to worse-than-expected outcomes,DBM does so by progressively devaluing the unchosen op-tions, thus placing a greater emphasis on choice history inde-pendent of reward outcome (e.g. an oft-chosen option mightcontinue to be preferred even if it has not been particularly re-warding), which has broadly been shown to underlie sequentialeffects in a variety of behavioral settings. Moreover, previouswork showed that the devaluation of unchosen options in DBMhelps to compensate for a default assumption of environmentalnon-stationarity, thus allowing the decision-maker to both bemore adaptive in changing environments and still obtain near-optimal performance in stationary environments. Thus, thecurrent work suggests both a novel rationale and mechanismfor persistent behavior in bandit tasks.

Do Environmental Resource Distributions Affect Attentional Styles?

How are attentional styles and spatial search strategies related? Analytical attention is directed towards focal elements,while holistic attention is distributed over the whole field. These styles bear similarities with exploitative and exploratoryspatial search strategies, where the agent either spends more time in local resource patches or covers more of the fieldand spends less time in individual patches. Moreover, both mechanisms are affected by the statistics of the environment:diffuse resources lead to exploratory search while visual crowdedness evokes holistic attention. We hypothesize that searchstrategies and attentional styles are guided by related mechanisms. To test this, we prime people with a diffuse-resourcesforaging task (exploration) or a clumpy-resources foraging task (exploitation). Priming is followed by field-dependencytasks to measure subjects attentional styles. We predict that diffuse resources create similar effects to visual crowdedness,inducing holistic attention in subjects, as well as exploration.

Toddlers and preschoolers use relational concepts to solve problems

Contrary to decades of previous research, one recent study suggests that preschoolers can rapidly learn and transfer avariety of relational concepts (e.g., bigger than; smaller than; opposite of) when those relations are operationalized asthe beginning and ending states of causal transformations performed by agents (Goddu, Lombrozo, & Gopnik, in press).Without causal framing, children reverted from relational reasoning to object matching, consistent with findings from manyprevious (non-causal) paradigms. Here, we investigate whether three-year-olds (Experiment 1) and 24- to 30-month-oldtoddlers (Experiment 2) are able to learn and apply relational concepts in a behavioral task where they themselves mustintervene to solve a problem using relational reasoning. Results indicate that children as young as two years of age areable to rapidly learn and generalize relational concepts and use them productively to solve new problems.

Modeling the Effect of Driver’s Eye Gaze Pattern Under Workload: GaussianMixture Approach

This paper puts forward a Gaussian Mixture Model (GMM) foreye gaze behavior under workload and applies it to the analy-sis of gaze distributions in an automotive context. Specifically,it extends earlier work on Information Constrained Control(ICC) (Hecht, Bar-Hillel, Telpaz, Tsimhoni, & Tishby, 2019)(Hecht, Telpaz, Kamhi, Bar-Hillel, & Tisbhy, 2019) (Hecht etal., 2015) (Hecht, Telpaz, Kamhi, Bar-Hillel, & Tishby, 2018)by generating an ICC GMM derivative. We suggest a mea-sure for workload estimation based on the Kullback Leiblerdivergence (Dkl ) between tested eye gaze distributions and areference workload-free distribution. This derivative assumesdiagonal Gaussians that are distant from each other. Underthese assumptions, we achieve an analytical measure that hassignificantly fewer parameters than discrete grid-like distribu-tions (Hecht, Bar-Hillel, et al., 2019). Testing our measureon eye gazing data collected during real world driving experi-ments in a highway environment confirms the effectiveness ofthis approach.

On the Psychology of Resource Monitoring

This research aims to understand when, why, and how people monitor resources such as money, time, and calories. Over-all, we find that people monitor money more closely than time or calories, but this varies by time horizon. While time andcalories were monitored most closely over a day, monitoring of money peaked at a month. Examining several possiblemediators of engagement with resource monitoring, we find the factors that impact ones engagement with resource moni-toring varies both by resource and by time horizon. One mediating factor that seems particularly important is the numberof budget categories people create. We find the degree to which people engage in resource monitoring is positively relatedto the number of categories they form. This research has implications for any behavior that involves tracking resources,such as financial decision-making, dieting, time management, and goal pursuit more generally.

A Simple Computational Model of Salience Map Formation in the Brain

Many convolutional neural network (CNN)-based approaches are excellent functional models of visual attention, but lackcognitive and biological interpretations. In this work, I offer novel, cross-disciplinary justification for the Deep Gaze 1model, which calculates salience as a weighted average of feature maps from a pre-trained CNN. In the cognitive realm,experiments demonstrate that visual attention depends on multiple levels of real-world features (edges, text, faces). Thisis well-modeled using features from a naturalistically-trained CNN. Furthermore, neuroscience research strongly suggeststhat visual attention is computed in the superior colliculus, using information from multiple levels of the ventral visualstream; all information flow in Deep Gaze follows analogous pathways. To encourage broader adoption of this model,whose source code remains unpublished, I offer a readable implementation with minor changes for biological plausibility.It is validated on the MIT1003 dataset using features from MobileNetV2, with results comparable to the original DeepGaze.

Integrating semantics into developmental models of morphology learning

A key challenge in language acquisition is learning morpho-logical transforms relating word roots to derived forms. Tra-ditional unsupervised algorithms find morphological patternsin sequences of phonemes, but struggle to distinguish validsegmentations from spurious ones because they ignore mean-ing. For example, a system that correctly discovers ”add /z/”as a valid morphological transform (song-songs, year-years)might incorrectly infer that ”add /ah.t/” is also valid (mark-market, spear-spirit). We propose that learners could avoidthese errors with a simple semantic assumption: morpholog-ical transforms approximately preserve meaning. We extendan algorithm from Chan and Yang (2008) by integrating prox-imity in vector-space word embeddings as a criterion for validtransforms. On a corpus of child-directed speech, we achieveboth higher accuracy and broader coverage than the purelyphonemic approach, even in more developmentally plausiblelearning paradigms. Finally, we consider a deeper semanticassumption that could guide the acquisition of more abstract,human-like morphological understanding.

On the Predictive Power of Neural Language Models for Human Real-TimeComprehension Behavior

Human reading behavior is tuned to the statistics of natural lan-guage: the time it takes human subjects to read a word can bepredicted from estimates of the word’s probability in context.However, it remains an open question what computational ar-chitecture best characterizes the expectations deployed in realtime by humans that determine the behavioral signatures ofreading. Here we test over two dozen models, independentlymanipulating computational architecture and training datasetsize, on how well their next-word expectations predict humanreading time behavior on naturalistic text corpora. Consistentwith previous work, we find that across model architecturesand training dataset sizes the relationship between word log-probability and reading time is (near-)linear. We next evalu-ate how features of these models determine their psychometricpredictive power, or ability to predict human reading behav-ior. In general, the better a model’s next-word expectations(as measured by the traditional language modeling perplexityobjective), the better its psychometric predictive power. How-ever, we find nontrivial differences in psychometric predictivepower across model architectures. For any given perplexity,deep Transformer models and n-gram models generally showsuperior psychometric predictive power over LSTM or struc-turally supervised neural models, especially for eye movementdata. Finally, we compare models’ psychometric predictivepower to the depth of their syntactic knowledge, as measuredby a battery of syntactic generalization tests developed usingmethods from controlled psycholinguistic experiments. Onceperplexity is controlled for, we find no significant relationshipbetween syntactic knowledge and predictive power. These re-sults suggest that, at least for the present state of natural lan-guage technology, different approaches may be required to bestmodel human real-time language comprehension behavior innaturalistic reading versus behavior for controlled linguisticmaterials designed for targeted probing of syntactic knowl-edge.

Do you see what I see?A Cross-cultural Comparison of Social Impressions of Faces

Research has suggested that social impressions of faces madeby Western and Eastern people have different underlying di-mensionalities. However, the individual level consistency, thegroup-level agreement of rater groups, and the interactionsbetween face ethnicity, rater ethnicity, and social impressiontraits remain largely unknown. In this paper, we perform alarge-scale data-driven cross-cultural study of facial impres-sions, and illustrate the idiosyncrasies and similarities behindCaucasian and Asian participants in their social impressions offaces from both ethnicity groups. Our study illustrates multi-ple interesting findings: (1) Asians rate faces lower on mostpositive traits, compared with Caucasian raters, and they havemore diverse opinions than Caucasians. (2) Caucasian faces re-ceive higher average ratings on social impression traits relatedto warmth due to the preponderance of smiles in Caucasianimages, but similar mean scores on traits related to capability,compared to Asian faces. (3) Caucasians and Asians disagreemost on capability related traits, especially on “responsible”and “successful.” Opinions on these two traits diverge moreon Asian than on Caucasian faces. Our findings provide newinsights on the nuances of cross-cultural differences in socialimpressions of faces.

Identifying Individual Differences in Sensemaking and Information Foraging

Prior research has distinguished between acquiring new/related knowledge (information foraging) and restructuring col-lected data (sensemaking), demonstrating that both substantially contribute to the comprehension of unknown information.These behaviors are critical cognitive abilities that can lead to scientific success and to innovation. Yet, little is knownabout whether there are individual differences in these behaviors. We provide a paradigm to study how these cognitiveabilities are utilized as participants attempt to understand the causal structure of a fictitious islands ecosystem (e.g., Whatis making the animals sick?). Some causal structures are directly posed by the environment, and can be discovered by in-formation foraging, whereas others can only be derived by sensemaking by merging or splitting the causes and/or effectsof already acquired information. We expect to see individual differences in information foraging and in sensemaking asreflected by the type of structures reported and time spent collecting or assessing the data.

Uncovering Category Representations with Linked MCMC with people

Cognitive science is often concerned with questions about ourrepresentations of concepts and the underlying psychologicalspaces in which these concepts are embedded. One methodto reveal concepts and conceptual spaces experimentally isMarkov chain Monte Carlo with people (MCMCP), whereparticipants produce samples from their implicit categories.While MCMCP has allowed for the experimental study of psy-chological representations of complex categories, experimentsare typically long and repetitive. Here, we contrasted the clas-sical MCMCP design with a linked variant, in which each par-ticipant completed just a short run of MCMCP trials, whichwere then combined to produce a single sample set. We foundthat linking produced results that were nearly indistinguishablefrom classical MCMCP, and often converged to the desired dis-tribution faster. Our results support linking as an approach forperforming MCMCP experiments within broader populations,such as in developmental settings where large numbers of trialsper participant are impractical.

Do people fit to Benford’s law, or do they have a Benford bias?

Smith (2015) describes an explosion of interest in Benford’s law, that for data from many domains the first digits have a log distribution. Few studies have similarly asked whether the numbers people generate fit to Benford’s law, but recent data show a reasonable fit. This paper argues that testing for fit to Benford’s law is the wrong question for behavioural data, instead we should think in terms of a “Benford bias” in which the first-digit distribution is distorted towards Benford’s law. We propose calculating the effect size of this bias by testing a linear contrast weighted by Benford’s law. Analyses of existing data sets yielded effect sizes of 0.43-0.52. Applying this approach to a new task extended the scope of Benford bias to predicting outputs of a linear system and found an effect size of .40. Benford bias may be a ubiquitous influence on judgments and decisions based on numbers.

How to navigate everyday distractions: Leveraging optimal feedback to trainattention control

To stay focused on their chosen tasks, people have to inhibit distractions. The underlying attention control skills canimprove through reinforcement learning, which can be accelerated by giving feedback. We applied the theory of metacog-nitive reinforcement learning to develop a training app that gives people optimal feedback on their attention control whilethey are working or studying. In an eight-day field experiment with 99 participants, we investigated the effect of this train-ing on peoples productivity, sustained attention, and self-control. Compared to a control condition without feedback, wefound that participants receiving optimal feedback learned to focus increasingly better (f = .08, p ¡ .01) and achieved higherproductivity scores (f = .19, p ¡ .01) during the training. In addition, they evaluated their productivity more accurately (r =.12, p ¡.01). However, due to asymmetric attrition problems, these findings need to be taken with a grain of salt.

Can neural networks acquire a structural bias from raw linguistic data?

We evaluate whether BERT, a widely used neural network forsentence processing, acquires an inductive bias towards form-ing structural generalizations through pretraining on raw data.We conduct four experiments testing its preference for struc-tural vs. linear generalizations in different structure-dependentphenomena. We find that BERT makes a structural general-ization in 3 out of 4 empirical domains—subject-auxiliary in-version, reflexive binding, and verb tense detection in embed-ded clauses—but makes a linear generalization when tested onNPI licensing. We argue that these results are the strongest ev-idence so far from artificial learners supporting the propositionthat a structural bias can be acquired from raw data. If this con-clusion is correct, it is tentative evidence that some linguisticuniversals can be acquired by learners without innate biases.However, the precise implications for human language acqui-sition are unclear, as humans learn language from significantlyless data than BERT.

Modeling manipulative language use

We propose an extension to probabilistic pragmatic models to include a dimension that allows for the modeling of argu-mentative language use. Within our extended Rational Speech Act model, argumentative strength stands for a statisticalmeasure of observational evidence which impacts a speakers utterance choice. More concretely, our model recasts speakerutility in terms of a weight parameter which varies between being purely informative and purely argumentative. We fitthe extended RSA model to empirical data from a novel production experiment. Our initial results suggest that there isroom for argumentativity on top of informativity in formalizations of pragmatic language reasoning. Crucially, we see thatthe relationship between the two is not straightforward, as the model fails to capture instances of human behavior whichare more manipulative than expected by the suggested informativity-argumentativity trade-off. All in all, our explorationprovides us with interesting insights about this relationship.

Measuring the costs of planning

Which information is worth considering depends on how much effort it would take to acquire and process it. Fromthis perspective peoples tendency to neglect considering the long-term consequences of their actions (present bias) mightreflect that looking further into the future becomes increasingly more effortful. In this work, we introduce and validatethe use of Bayesian Inverse Reinforcement Learning (BIRL) for measuring individual differences in the subjective costsof planning. We extend the resource-rational model of human planning introduced by Callaway, Lieder, et al. (2018) byparameterizing the cost of planning. Using BIRL, we show that increased subjective cost for considering future outcomesmay be associated with both the present bias and acting without planning. Our results highlight testing the causal effectsof the cost of planning on both present bias and mental effort avoidance as a promising direction for future work.

Popular Peers Promote Prosocial Behavior

Dispositions for prosociality undergo major changes during adolescence, a period of increased sensitivity to peer influenceand incipient internalization of societal norms. However, the proximate mechanisms favoring the development of prosocialpreferences are poorly understood. Here, we show that high-status peers affect adolescents prosocial decision making.Participants repeatedly chose to either donate money to a charity or keep it for themselves and could revise their decisionupon observing the (opposite) decisions of either a high-status or low-status peer from their classroom. Participantstended to conform to peer behavior, reversing their initial preference. Importantly, this pattern was especially strong whenobserving high-status peers, suggesting that norm signalling from high-status peers can be instrumental for the diffusionof prosocial behavior. Using a novel incentivized paradigm in a naturalistic setting, we provide experimental evidencefor the role of high-status peers in the development of prosocial preferences and outline a potential path for interventionsaimed at spreading prosocial norms.

Asking questions with a big impact: Adapting to other interpretations of gradableadjectives

When communicating, people adapt their linguistic representations to those of their interlocutors. Previous studies haveshown that adaptation also works at the semantic level, with listeners aligning their interpretations of vague expressionssuch as quantifiers to those by a certain speaker. While adaptation has been found to arise by passive exposure to the inter-locutors linguistic representations, we hypothesize that actively seeking information could boost this effect. In particular,asking clarification questions can be helpful to reduce the uncertainty about someone elses interpretation. We focus on thegradable adjectives big and small and show that, in line with previous findings, speakers can align their representations tothose by their interlocutors. Moreover, this effect is boosted when people are given the possibility to ask questions. Thoughparticipants can generally ask for useful information, we observe that this ability improves as the interaction progresses.

Exploring Exploration: Comparing Children with Agents in Unified ExplorationEnvironments

Research in developmental psychology consistently shows that children explore the world thoroughly and efficiently andthat this exploration allows them to learn. While much work has gone into developing methods for exploration in machinelearning, artificial agents have not yet reached the standard set by their human counterparts. In this work we propose usingDeepMind Lab as a platform to directly compare child and agent behaviors and to develop new exploration techniques.We tested 60 children aged 4-6 examining two conditions that emulate how current reinforcement learning algorithmslearn using dense and sparse rewards and the children are then asked to find a goal in various mazes. These tasks providedata that can easily be compared to algorithms and we evaluate turn-by-turn moves the children do to what the Intrinsic-Curiosity-Module and Depth-First-Search algorithm do in the same exact maze. We show specifically where and whenchildren differ from the algorithms.

Poster Session 2

A theoretically driven meta-analysis of implicit theory of mind studies: The role offactivity

The capacity for Theory of Mind (ToM) allows us to repre-sent others’ understanding of the world independently fromour own and then explain and predict their actions in terms oftheir understanding. Researchers have often focused on tryingto find evidence for an implicit theory of mind system: one thatemerges early in human ontogeny and operates mandatoriallyin adults. In this paper, we ask how the recent methodologi-cal push towards replication can be used as a tool that bearson a key theoretical distinction in implicit Theory of Mind,namely the distinction between factive and non-factive ToMrepresentation. Unlike other meta-analyses, our primary inter-est is not the overall replicability of theory of mind findings.Instead, we ask whether the replicability of implicit theory ofmind tasks depends in part on whether they measured factiveor non-factive ToM. We find that, to the extent that there isreplicable and robust evidence for implicit ToM, that evidencelargely comes from tests that investigated factive ToM repre-sentations. This analysis is a proof of concept of the broaderpotential for using replication attempts to ask theoretically mo-tivated questions.

Automatic Detection of Cross-language Verbal Deception

The assessment of how a deceptive message is produced in dif-ferent languages has received little attention, with the majorityof studies focused on the English language. Moreover, thereis no agreement about the stability of linguistic clues of deceitacross different languages. In this paper, we address this issueby analysing both theory-driven linguistic markers of decep-tion (cognitive load hypothesis) and standard text categorisa-tion features. After compiling a multilingual corpus of bothhonest and deceitful first-person opinions regarding five differ-ent topics, we assessed the cross-language applicability of fourdifferent features sets in within-topic, cross-topic and cross-language binary classification experiments. Results showedpromising classification performances in all the three experi-ments with few exceptions. Interestingly, linguistic markersof deceit linked to the cognitive load hypothesis exhibited thesame trend in the two languages under investigation and thecross-language evaluation highlighted their usefulness in spot-ting deceit between different languages.

Inherent and Emergent Biases of Vocal Learning Timeframes in Zebra Finches

Language acquisition researchers have demonstrated that human infants tend to learn some sound classes before others.However, similar biases acting on classes of sounds have not yet been demonstrated in a birdsong model. Here, I detail thelearning strategies of four zebra finches at both the level of the song and the level of the syllable. Although some syllables,namely introductory notes and transient chirps, appear to emerge along regular timeframes, the learning strategy chosenby the bird also has a strong influence on each syllables ontogeny. Syllables imitated earlier in a songs development tendto be imitated more accurately than syllables derived later in the learning process.

“Take the Middle” – Averaging Prior and Evidenceas Effective Heuristic in Bayesian Reasoning

When humans revise their assumptions based on evidence, theyprocess information on the (un)certainties of the situation. Thisprocess can be modeled by a (mathematically optimal) Bayes-ian reasoning strategy. Humans typically deviate from thisnorm and apply heuristic strategies, often by only partially pro-cessing the available information (e.g., neglecting base rates).From a perspective of ecological rationality, such heuristicspossibly constitute viable cognitive strategies in certain situa-tions. We investigate the adequacy of a cognitively plausibleheuristic strategy, which amounts to approximately averagingthe probability information on prior hypotheses and evidence.We compare this strategy to optimal Bayesian reasoning and toinformation-neglecting strategies by exploring the situationalparameter space (number of hypotheses, prior and likelihoodvalues). Finally, we frame this in the context of teachers’ diag-nostic judgments on students’ potential misconceptions (pri-ors) based on students’ solutions (evidence) and interpret theresulting accuracy of decisions within the ecology of informalstudent assessment.

Investigating the Role of Verb Frequencyin Factive and Manner-of-speaking Islands

Frequency plays a central role in human cognition, and in lan-guage processing in particular. There is growing evidence thatacceptability judgements are shaped by the statistics of theinput. In this paper, we focus on a type of constraint opera-tive in long-distance dependencies (e.g. wh-questions, relativeclauses, topicalizations, etc.) which has been claimed to re-sult from verb subcategorization frequency effects. We takea closer look at this hypothesis, and conclude that it does notaccount for the sentence acceptability contrasts. Rather, theevidence we find suggests that the acceptability of these depen-dencies hinges on clause-level semantic-pragmatic factors.

Intention Inference in a Dynamic Multi-Goal Environment

Navigating the social world relies upon the human capacity for mentalizing, or attributing intentions to social agents.Unfortunately, currently available commercial robots still lack such awareness of human intentionality. Building uponrecently-proposed Bayesian models of Theory of Mind (ToM), we propose a ToM model that can handle intention inferencein dynamic, fast-changing environments like a hospital, where staff have to attend to objectives and emergencies as theyarise. Our model infers and maintains a distribution over possible intentions, and uses the posterior predictives to forecastfuture trajectories, which is essential for robot motion planning. We show that our model performs excellently at inferringthe intentions and trajectories of human players controlling a nurse agent in a simulated environment. This work lays thefoundation for robots that can co-work with humans in dynamic, social environments with high-stake goals.

Reinforcement of Semantic Representations in Pragmatic Agents Leads to theEmergence of a Mutual Exclusivity Bias

We present a novel framework for building pragmatic artificialagents with explicit and trainable semantic representations, us-ing the Rational Speech Act model. We train our agents onsupervised and unsupervised communication games and com-pare their behavior to literal agents lacking pragmatic abilities.For both types of games pragmatic but not literal agents evolvea mutual exclusivity bias. This provides a computational prag-matic account of mutual exclusivity and points out a possi-ble direction for solving the mutual exclusivity bias challengeposed by Gandhi and Lake (2019). We find that pragmaticreasoning can cause the bias either by promoting lexical con-straints during learning, or by affecting online inference. In ad-dition we show that pragmatic abilities lead to faster learningand that this advantage is even stronger when meanings to becommunicated follow a more natural distribution as describedby Zipf’s law.

Propositional versus Associative Views of Sentence Memory

Propositional accounts assume sentences are encoded in terms of a set of arguments bound to role-fillers in a predicate,but they never specify how the role representations form in the first place. Dennis (2005) shows an alternative way tocapture role-information based on simple associations derived directly from experience in the Syntagmatic-Paradigmatic(SP) model. We argue that the evidence for the propositional view is not well-founded and explore the possibility for apure associative encoding of proposition-like information. We differentially manipulate overlap in target and distractorsentences, embedded in narratives, and directly place the propositional account against the SP view. Our first experimentprovides some evidence for an SP account, however the second experiment supports the propositional view. Our finalexperiment provides results that are difficult to explain with either account. Overall, our results support the propositionalview and show mixed evidence for the SP account.

Individual adaptation in teamwork

Teamwork in Team Space Fortress, a real-time cooperative task, was studied by analyzing the performance of participantspaired with different partners. To defeat the fortress, a player taking the role of bait approaches within the fortress rangeof fire causing the fortress to lower its shield to fire, thereby becoming vulnerable to attack by a partner playing therole of shooter. A novel design exchanging partners within four person groups allowed the identification of adaptationsand isolation of individual contributions to team performance. Team performance was determined by factors at bothindividual and team levels. Using subjective similarity rankings collected on Amazon Mechanical Turk, we constructedhigh-dimensional embeddings of similarity between team trajectories. Results showed that team members who adaptedmost, led to improved team performance. In re-pairings of partners better individual performance did not necessarily leadto better team performance again supporting the need for adaptivity in human machine teaming.

Reducing retrieval time modulates the production effect

Memory is reliably enhanced for information read aloud compared with information read silentlythe production effect.Three preregistered experiments examined whether the production effect arises from a time-consuming recollective processoperating at test that benefits items that were produced at study. To accomplish this, participants were required to respondwithin a short deadline under the assumption that a time-consuming recollective process would be less able to operatewhen less time is available. If so, the production effect under speeded responding instructions should be reduced relativeto a standard nonspeeded condition. Results generally supported this prediction. However, even under speeded respondinginstructions, there was a robust production effect, potentially suggesting that other, more rapid, processes also contributeto the production effect.

Using Signal Detection Theory to Investigate the Role of Visual Information in Performance Monitoring in Typing

This paper uses the signal detection theory (SDT) to investigate the contribution of visual information to two monitoring-dependent functions, metacognitive awareness of errors and error corrections. Data from two experiments show that complete removal of visual outcome results in a mild decrease in error awareness and a much more significant decrease in correction rates. Partially restoring visual information by including positional information (as in masked password typing) causes a modest but statistically significant improvement in correction performance. Interestingly, participants treat the change to the quality of information differently across the tasks, with more conservative behavior (avoiding false alarms) in the correction task. These findings show the SDT’s ability to quantify, in a graded manner, the contribution of specific types of information to monitoring in complex tasks, while also providing additional information about how participants handle the change to the quality of information in a task-dependent manner.

A Model of Fast Concept Inference with Object-Factorized Cognitive Programs

The ability of humans to quickly identify general conceptsfrom a handful of images has proven difficult to emulate withrobots. Recently, a computer architecture was developed thatallows robots to mimic some aspects of this human ability bymodeling concepts as cognitive programs using an instructionset of primitive cognitive functions. This allowed a robot toemulate human imagination by simulating candidate programsin a world model before generalizing to the physical world.However, this model used a naive search algorithm that re-quired 30 minutes to discover a single concept, and becameintractable for programs with more than 20 instructions. Tocircumvents this bottleneck, we present an algorithm that emu-lates the human cognitive heuristics of object factorization andsub-goaling, allowing human-level inference speed, improvingaccuracy, and making the output more explainable.

Adaptive Sampling Policies Imply Biased Beliefs:A Generalization of the Hot Stove Effect

The Hot Stove Effect is a negativity bias resulting from theadaptive character of learning. The mechanism is that learn-ing algorithms that pursue alternatives with positive estimatedvalues, but avoid alternatives with negative estimated values,will correct errors of overestimation but fail to correct errorsof underestimation. Here we generalize the theory behind theHot Stove Effect to settings in which negative estimates do notnecessarily lead to avoidance but to a smaller sample size (i.e,a learner selects fewer of alternative B if B is believed to be in-ferior but does not entirely avoid B). We demonstrate formallythat the negativity bias remains in this set-up. We also showthat there is a negativity bias for Bayesian learners in the sensethat most such learners underestimate the expected value of analternative.

Investigating the impact of social and biological cues in children’s perception ofhumanoid robots

Imitation plays a key role in learning cultural knowledge. Young children imitate human models as well as humanoidrobots, even when their actions are clearly non-functional to achieve a given goal. This so-called overimitation is possiblymotivated by the desire to socially affiliate. This study clarifies the impact of social cues (greeting, eyes, friendly voice)and smooth, dynamic body motion of humanoid robots on rates of overimitation. In one condition, we remove all socialcues. In another condition, we change the dynamics of robot movement to be less biological. Overimitation rates will becompared across all three conditions (social & biological, non-social & biological, non-social & non-biological) to learnmore about important model characteristics that support cultural learning. Children aged 5-6 participated in this study. Wediscuss results and implications for using humanoid robots in interactive settings with children.

A Neural Network Model of the Effect of Prior Experience with Regularities onSubsequent Category Learning

A popular dual systems theory of category learning argues thatthe structure of categories in perceptual space determines themechanisms that drive learning. However, less attention hasbeen paid to the nature of the perceptual dimensions definingthe categories. Researchers typically assume that there is adirect, linear relationship between experimenter-definedphysical input dimensions and learners’ psychologicaldimensions, but this assumption is not always warranted.Through a set of simulations, we demonstrate that, based on thenature of prior experience, the psychological representations ofexperimenter-defined dimensions can place drastic constraintson category learning. We compare the model’s behavior toseveral human studies and make conclusions regarding thenature of the psychological representations of the dimensionsin those studies. These simulations support the conclusion thatthe nature of psychological representations is a critical aspectto understanding the mechanisms that drive category learning.

Process and Content in Decisions from Memory

We present a general framework for building formal models of naturalistic memory-based decision making. Our frame-work implements established theories of memory search and decision making within a single integrated cognitive system,and uses computational language models to quantify the thoughts over which memory and decision processes operate. Itcan thus describe both the content of the information that is sampled from memory, as well as the processes involved inretrieving and evaluating this information in order to make a decision. The models within our framework can be fit torecall and choice data, and can quantitatively predict choice probability, length of deliberation, retrieved thoughts, andthe effects of decision context. We showcase the power and generality of our framework by applying it to study riskperception, consumer behavior, financial decision making, ethical decision making, legal decision making, food choice,and judgments about well-being, society, and culture.

Can Changes in Inhibitory Control Explain Child-Level Theory of Mind Development?

A central canon in theory of mind research is that between theages of three and four a drastic performance difference inchildren’s understanding occurs. However, the reason for the‘three to four shift’ has yet to be settled. One account, Theoryof Mind Mechanism (ToMM) theory (Leslie, 1994), posits thatchange in inhibitory power can account for this difference. Thisis supported by a recent computational implementation of thetheory, showing that differences in inhibitory power canaccount for age differences at an aggregate level (Wang,Hemmer, & Leslie, 2019). However, as Baker et al. (2016)point out, established findings are entirely based on group-aggregated findings, yet computational and developmentalprocesses do not take place in the ‘aggregated mind’. Whatremains largely unexplored is what happens at the level of theindividual child. Here we combine the computationalimplementation of ToMM with data from Baker et al., 2016,who assessed longitudinal developmental change in Theory ofMind performance by repeated testing of individual child overthe three-to-four shift period on standard ‘Sally and Anne’ falsebelief tasks, to obtain a cumulative record for each child.Specifically, we found that children’s age was not directlyinformative of developmental change in theory of mindreasoning. Instead, the main contributor to theory of mindperformance at the individual learner level is inhibitory power.

The effect of book syntactic complexity on caregiver and child language profileduring shared book reading

Shared book reading positively effects language development, yet the causal pathways of this relationship are not un-derstood. Evidence shows that the book complexity modulates caregiver talk, but the link between the book linguisticcomplexity and child syntactic development remains unclear (Noble, et al. 2017). This project describes the speech gen-erated during book reading to see how it differs from typical child-directed speech and whether the picture-book sentencecomplexity is present in the speech that children hear. 10 families with children aged 30-37 months (MBCDI raw vocab-ulary 350-675 out of 680 total) recorded 6-12 picture-book reading sessions in their homes. The books were controlledfor word length (short: 125 words vs long: 1472 words) and syntactic complexity according to the 8 categories analyzedin Montag (2019): complex (17 tokens) vs. simple (0-7 tokens). The caregiver and child speech syntactic complexitymodulation as a function of picture-book syntactic complexity will be discussed.

Investigating the effects of transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) onFace Recognition skills as indexed by the Composite Face Effect.

In this study we show that a particular form of neuro-stimulation can affect face recognition skills impairing participantsperformance on a face recognition task without affecting the composite face effect. Using a Face-Matching task (n=48)traditionally used to study the composite face effect (better recognition of the top half of an upright face when in compositewith a congruent vs and incongruent bottom half) we confirm that anodal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS;double-blind and between subjects study) delivered over the left DLPFC at Fp3 (10 mins at 1.5mA) affects overall facerecognition performance for upright faces. But no effect of the tDCS was found on the composite face effect itself. Weinterpret our results in the light of previous literature on the tDCS effects on perceptual learning and face recognition,suggesting that different mechanisms are involved in the face inversion effect and the composite face effect.

Global Warming, Nationalism, and Reasoning With Numbers:Toward Techniques to Promote the Public’s Critical Thinking About Statistics

The increase of misinformation in the public sphere over thepast decade represents an urgent societal issue, given thechallenge of distinguishing veridical facts from false ormisleading information. The present experiment’s resultsindicate that people are reliant on numerical information in theirdetermination of whether a statistic related to global warming isrepresentative or misleading. Of particularly practicalsignificance, the results also demonstrate that showingparticipants a mixed set of revealing and misleading globalwarming statistics leads to an increase in global warmingacceptance, rather than sowing confusion (or some sense that alldata are equally dubious or compelling). Replicating priorresults, nationalism and global warming acceptance are in anegative relationship. We also describe the background, design,and assessment of a curriculum intended to help the generalpublic better distinguish between representative and misleadingstatistics about anthropogenic climate change. The findingshighlight numerically-driven inferencing as a useful paradigmfor the assessment of information relating to global warming andenvironmental risk.

Contribution of first-person sensory experience to thinking about seeing: Evidencefrom blindness

Do we need to reflect on our own perceptual experiences to understand what another person is seeing/hearing? Sighted(n=18) and congenitally blind (n=18) participants listened to scenarios describing sighted or blind observers looking at orhearing another person (target). Participants rated the likelihood that observers would know features of the target (e.g., age,gender, eye/hair color). We manipulated distance of observer from the target (nearby versus far) and duration of perceptualexperience (extended versus brief). Blind and sighted groups agreed on features easiest to discern (e.g. hair easier than eyecolor), although blind participants judgments about vision were more variable. Both groups judged nearby and extendedperception more likely to result in knowing. For seeing experiences, blind participants judgments were more influenced byduration, whereas sighted participants by distance. Linguistic communication is sufficient for discovering basic variablesgoverning perception (i.e., distance, duration), but first-person experience calibrates weighting of the variables.

What is an extreme outcome in risky choice?

Numerous experiments have suggested that extreme outcomesare disproportionately influential when we make decisions in-volving risk, but there is less consensus on what it actuallymeans to be extreme. Existing accounts broadly fall into twocategories: those that suggest that the best and worst outcomesare uniquely influential and those that suggest that outcomesbecome more influential with increasing deviation from thecentre of the distribution. We conducted two experiments thataimed to tease apart these explanations. Although there wassome evidence that the distance from the centre influencesmemory, neither account was able to fully explain the choicesmade by participants. This finding has implications for the vi-ability of these explanations as well as for the generalisabilityof the effect and the interpretation of the method used to assessmemory.

Tell me something I don’t know: How perceived knowledge influences the use ofinformation during decision making

We are often confronted with new causal information aboutthe world, such as what causes a disease. What we think weknow may influence if and how we choose to use this new in-formation. Yet as prior work has shown, we are not alwayssuccessful at evaluating our own knowledge. We explored howhelping people better understand what they know about a do-main can influence their ability to use new causal informationin a decision-making context. Participants self-assessed theirknowledge (Experiment 1) or completed an objective assess-ment of their knowledge (Experiment 2) of diabetes, beforemaking diabetes-related decisions, either with or without newcausal information. Without a knowledge assessment, partic-ipants were less accurate with new causal information com-pared to without such information, replicating previous work.However, reassessing their knowledge increased participants’decision-making accuracy with causal information. We dis-cuss why helping people realize the limits of their causal un-derstanding may make them better supplement it with new in-formation.

Novice conceptions and perception of single and two force interactions

Physics education and psychology research have found novices struggle to accurately predict the trajectory of objects,and perception research has found people cannot perceptually differentiate between plausible and implausible collisionoutcomes. Prior research focused on single force interactions, we explored predictions and perception of both one andtwo force interactions. Participants (N = 111) drew predicted paths of balls acted upon by a single force, two forcesacting simultaneously, and two forces acting sequentially. Paths were categorized into: correct, curved, single forcedominant, inaccurate angle, first force dominant, and recent force dominant. Participants also made perceptual naturalnessand animacy ratings for animations portraying accurate solutions and high frequency alternate conceptions. Preliminaryresults suggest participants were accurate for forces aligned on one dimension, and less accurate for forces not aligned onone dimensionparticipants anticipated curved paths, paths taking an inaccurate angle, and paths aligned with only one ofthe forces.

Cognition at Special Forces Boot Camp: Does High-Intensity Physical ExerciseAffect Memorisation?

There is conflicting evidence regarding the effect of acute physical exercise on peoples ability to memorise declarativeinformation. Some studies have found that exercising before learning improves memorisation, while others have foundan adverse effect. We measured memorisation in 70 recruits for the Special Forces unit of the Dutch army during theirfirst week of training. Recruits used a computer-guided learning system to study the names of locations on a map directlybefore and directly after a high-intensity speed march. In the learning session following the speed march, responses werefaster but less accurate than before, particularly at the start of the session. We fitted a computational cognitive model ofhuman memory to the responses made in each learning session to obtain a continuous index of memorisation. This indexshowed a small improvement after the speed march, suggesting that memory representations formed after high-intensityphysical exercise were slightly more stable.

Children combine information from multiple models in a grid search task

Population size has been proposed to promote cumulative culture in humans. Experimental evidence from adult humanssuggests that this may be due to the potential for combining beneficial information from multiple models. However, it ispossible that such combinatory social learning requires cognitive capacities restricted to adult humans. In our task, childrenaged 5-10 years watched two models consecutively search a 3x3 grid for rewards. Models revealed different correct andincorrect reward locations. This information could be used by the child to maximise their own score on the same task. Wewere interested in childrens ability to select rewarded locations, and avoid unrewarded ones, revealed by both models. Wealso manipulated the spatial and temporal displacement of the information available. Childrens performance on the taskimproved with age. Most children could outperform the mean score of the two models, but outperforming the combinedscore occurred in only limited circumstances.

Encoder-Decoder Neural Architectures for Fast Amortized Inference of CognitiveProcess Models

Computational cognitive modeling offers a principled inter-pretation of the functional demands of cognitive systems andaffords quantitative fits to behavioral/brain data. Typically,cognitive modelers are interested in the fit of a model withparameters estimated using maximum likelihood or Bayesianmethods. However, the set of models with known likeli-hoods is dramatically smaller than the set of plausible gen-erative models. For all but some standard models (e.g., thedrift-diffusion model), lack of closed-form likelihoods typi-cally prevents using traditional Bayesian inference methods.Employing likelihood-free methods is a workaround in prac-tice. However, the computational complexity of these methodsis a bottleneck, since it requires many simulations for each pro-posed parameter set in posterior sampling schemes. Here, wepropose a method that learns an approximate likelihood overthe parameter space of interest by encapsulation into a convo-lutional neural network, affording fast parallel posterior sam-pling downstream after a one-off simulation cost is incurredfor training.

Optimality and Space in Weakly Constrained Everyday Activities

The action order of most everyday activities is only weaklyconstrained: When setting the table, for example, the orderin which the items are placed on the table does not matter ifall required items are on the table eventually. Little is knownabout how humans deal with weakly constrained sequences.Consistent with research on local optimality of human behav-ior and the “law of less work”, we propose that the order ofweakly constrained sequences is not chosen arbitrarily but dueto preferences, with the overall goal to minimize cognitive andphysical effort. We implement and validate a stepwise-optimalmodel for table setting, revealing ordering preferences basedon distance, functional relations between items, and reachabil-ity. The model’s success has implications concerning actionorganization in weakly constrained sequences as well as con-trol of action sequences and provides further evidence on thequestion of global vs. local optimality of human cognition.

Intuitive Signaling Through an ”Imagined We’”

Communication is highly overloaded. Despite this, even young children are good at leveraging context to understandambiguous signals. We propose a computational shared agency account of signaling that we call the Imagined We (IW)framework. We leverage Bayesian Theory of Mind to provide mechanisms for rational action planning and inverse actioninterpretation. In order to expand this framework for communication, we first treat signals as rational actions. We thenincorporate our rich understanding of intuitive utilities to constrain the scope of affordable actions. Finally, we treatcommunication as a cooperative act, subject to constraints of maximizing a shared utility. We implement this modelin two completely different behavioral psychology works to demonstrate the generality of the IW under different typesof uncertainty in cooperative communication. Additionally, we demonstrate that the IW outperforms multiple baselinemodels in a novel task across a series of simulation conditions.

Learning Generalizations and Exceptions: the Good, the Bad and theUnpredictable

How are exceptions to a generalization learned? 20 participants were exposed to a mini-artificial language in whicheach word (prefix + wordstem) was associated with a unique image. One of two prefixes generalized probabilistically:it appeared with 40 stems associated with faces and 8 exceptions, which were associated with scenes. The other prefixoccurred with 8 faces and 8 scenes. The prefixes and the image categories (faces vs. scenes) were counterbalanced acrossparticipants. Participants performed a 2 alternative-forced choice task on all items, with feedback, over 6 repeating blocks.Results show that image-word pairs that included the generalizable prefix were learned better than those which appearedwith the other prefix, despite having 48 items in the first class and 16 in the other (d = 1.28, d = 0.80, p = 0.023). Weinvestigate the neural representation of these words and how they change over the course of learning.

What is Represented in Memory after Statistical Learning?

Statistical learning is a powerful mechanism that allows us torapidly extract structure from the environment. However,nuances of what structure is extracted—for example, whetherreliable groups are stored without knowledge of theirconstituent item order—are not well understood, leaving uswith open questions about how this mechanism supportsbehaviour. Here, we extend prior work on the representation ofstatistical structure by asking what specific aspects of structurematter for memory judgments. We consider three candidatesfor memory representation: transitional probability, order-independent group information, and position tags. Participantswatched a stream of shape triplets and then completed arecognition memory test designed to isolate contributions oftransitional probability, group, and position. We demonstratethat although memory for transitions alone would be sufficientfor knowledge of triplets, participants showed evidence ofrepresenting both transitional probability and group. Our datahighlight statistical learning as a mechanism enablinggeneralization across experiences.

Antarjami: Exploring psychometric evaluation through a computer-based game

A number of questionnaire based psychometric testing frameworks are globally for example OCEAN (Five factor) indi-cator, MBTI (Myers Brigg Type Indicator) etc. However, questionnaire based psychometric tests have some known short-comings. This work explores whether these shortcomings can be mitigated through computer-based gaming platforms forevaluating psychometric parameters. A computer based psychometric game framework called Antarjami has been devel-oped for evaluating OCEAN (Five factor) indicators . It investigates the feasibility of extracting psychometric parametersthrough computer-based games, utilizing underlying improvements in the area of modern artificial intelligence. The can-didates for the test are subjected to a number scenarios as part of the computer based game and their reactions/responsesare used to evaluate their psychometric parameters. As part of the study, the parameters obtained from the game werecompared with those evaluated using paper based tests and scores given by a panel of psychologists. The achieved resultswere very promising.

Constructing Meaning in Small Increments

Humans comprehend natural language sentences in real time, processing the elements of each sentence incrementally withimmediate interpretation, while working within the limitations of general cognitive abilities. While much research hasbeen devoted to human sentence comprehension, a detailed computational theory of how this is done has been lacking.In this work we explore some fundamental principles of human sentence comprehension, propose a novel computationaltheory of knowledge representation and incremental processing to comprehend sentences using general cognitive abilities,and discuss results of an implementation of this theory in a robotic agent. We then explore the theorys implications forfuture work in various areas of cognitive science.

A spiking neuron model of inferential decision making:Urgency, uncertainty, and the speed-accuracy tradeoff

Decision making (DM) requires the coordination of anatom-ically and functionally distinct cortical and subcortical areas.While previous computational models have studied these sub-systems in isolation, few models explore how DM holisticallyarises from their interaction. We propose a spiking neuronmodel that unifies various components of DM, then show thatthe model performs an inferential decision task in a human-likemanner. The model (a) includes populations corresponding todorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, right inferiorfrontal cortex, pre-supplementary motor area, and basal ganglia;(b) is constructed using 8000 leaky-integrate-and-fire neuronswith 7 million connections; and (c) realizes dedicated cognitiveoperations such as weighted valuation of inputs, accumulationof evidence for multiple choice alternatives, competition be-tween potential actions, dynamic thresholding of behavior, andurgency-mediated modulation. We show that the model repro-duces reaction time distributions and speed-accuracy tradeoffsfrom humans performing the task. These results provide be-havioral validation for tasks that involve slow dynamics andperceptual uncertainty; we conclude by discussing how addi-tional tasks, constraints, and metrics may be incorporated intothis initial framework.

Listeners Big Five Personality Traits Predict Changes in Pupil Size During SpokenLanguage Comprehension

We report on findings from a pupillometry study that investigated auditory language comprehension in adults. Specifically,we assessed the participants Big Five traits and correlated them with changes in pupil size in response to socio-culturalclashes violating common gender stereotypes, such as I always buy my bras at Hudsons Bay spoken by a male speaker.Morpho-syntactic errors, such as She usually drive (as opposed to drives) her car slowly, and semantic anomalies, such asPeople often read heads (as opposed to books), were included as controls.Results obtained from 88 native speakers of North American English suggest that the processing of different kinds oflinguistic clashes is correlated with different Big Five traits. The results expand on findings in Hubert and Jrvikivi (2019),and add support to theories of linguistic comprehension in which extra-linguistic variables are considered early in theprocess (see e.g. Van Berkum et al., 2008, 2009).

Modeling Visuospatial Reasoning Across 17 Different Tests on the Leiter Scale of Nonverbal Intelligence

Understanding the computational mechanisms enabling visuospatial reasoning is important for studying human intelli-gence as well as for exploring the possibility of introducing human-like reasoning into artificial intelligence systems. Inour work, we investigate how a collection of primitive image processing operations can be combined into different co-herent strategies for solving a range of visuospatial reasoning tasks. We evaluate our approach on 20 subtests from theLeiter International Performance Scale-Revised (Leiter-R). Through our computational experiments, we show that withonly four primitive operations similarity, containment, rotation, and scaling we can form strategies that solve, to differentdegrees of success, at least portions of 17 of the 20 subtests. These results lay foundations for our future work to study howintelligent agents can learn and generalize strategies from simple task definitions in order to perform complex visuospatialreasoning tasks.

Part of Your World: Trends in the Visual Complexity of Digital Media

Studying the mechanisms and trajectories of child development continues to be of critical importance, especially in thecontext of fast-changing digital environments, ubiquitous screens, and ever-increasing permeations of technology intoour lives. In this work, we study historical changes in the visual complexity of information presented in different mediacategories, including an eighty-year history of Disney movies and a forty-six-year history of NBC news programs andtelevision commercials. Our analyses include metrics of static visual variance in single frames as well as several metricsof visual change over time. By performing similar analyses on a dataset of egocentric videos, we compare trends in digitalmedia with data that more closely resemble real-life visual experiences. Understanding the visual characteristics of themedia we consume is an important step towards further investigating the effects that these characteristics might have onour perception, attention, and learning, especially in young children.

Ten semantic differential evaluations of written Japanese vowels in a paper-basedsurvey study

Vowels in words have been associated with specific meanings in sound symbolism (Hamano, 1998; Newman, 1933;Sapir, 1929). The purpose of this study was to examine whether each vowel individually involves physical and emotionalmeanings. Six-hundred and thirteen participants (482 females; M 16.97) rated 5-point semantic differential scales (size,distance, thickness, extent, weight, height, depth, preference, arousal, and familiarity) to presented Japanese vowels (a, i, u,e, and o). Results showed that the size, extent, and thickness of a, u, and o were significantly higher than i and e, whereasthe preference and familiarity of a was higher than the others. These results were consistent with previous findings towhich vowels in sound-symbolic words were associated with physical (i.e., size, extent, and thickness) and emotional (i.e.,preference) evaluations. Our findings suggest that each vowel itself could individually contribute to specifically physicaland emotional evaluations.

Great Expectations: Evaluating the Role of Object-Color Expectations on Visual Memory

Previous research has shown that category expectations can improve recall, by reducing absolute average error (e.g. Hut-tenlocher, et.al., 1991; Hemmer & Steyvers, 2009), particularly when expectations are consistent with studied information.However, studied information that is expectation-inconsistent may also boost memory (e.g. Sakamoto & Love, 2004).Using a cued-recall task, we manipulated the degree to which studied object-color pairs aligned with peoples (N=29)expectations to explore the role of expectations in delayed recall. Our preliminary results show greater recall accuracyfor expectation-consistent items (e.g. yellow bananas) compared to expectation-inconsistent (purple bananas), and no-expectation items (yellow toothbrushes). However, there was no difference in accuracy between expectation-inconsistentand no-expectation items, nor was there a difference between weak and strong expectation-inconsistent items (orangish-yellow and purple bananas, respectively). This preliminary work shows that in delayed recall, the benefit of categoryexpectations might not extend to instances when studied information is misaligned with those expectations.

Change of Consciousness and Attitude through Learning Experience inUniversity: An Exploratory Learning Model of Japanese University Students

Changes in students learning approaches/attitudes when transitioning from high schools to universities is an importanttopic in Japanese higher education researches. In previous Japanese research, case studies have discussed students learningexperiences and attitudes in high schools and universities. However, most of them only discussed the difference and thesimilarity between high school and university and did not suggest the ways of connecting two different or similar learningsystems. The present study conducted surveys using two questionnaires that examined first-year undergraduate studentslearning experience in high school, learning attitude at the start of the semester, and learning experience and attitude at theend of the semester. The analysis of the startend of the semester suggests there were two different learning attitudes: onethat is continued from high school and difficult to be affected by the learning experience in university and the other that ischangeable through active and communicative learning experiences in university.

Potential for cumulative culture in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) in asimulated transmission chain study.

We investigated whether capuchin monkeys could use information about rewarded and unrewarded stimuli such that chain-ing of their response patterns would in principle generate increasingly successful performances, indicative of potential forcumulative culture. Two populations of tufted capuchin monkeys were tested using a touchscreen stimulus-selection taskrequiring subjects to learn the strategy of repeating rewarded, and avoiding unrewarded selections following demonstra-tions of varying success. Although capuchins outperformed demonstrations of chance-level performance (simulating per-formance of a nave individual), they did not consistently outperform demonstrations of above-chance-level success. Thissuggests that, in a social transmission scenario, the accumulation of beneficial information over successive transmissionevents would be relatively limited. Despite mastering the task contingencies, the capuchins did not use the informationoptimally, limiting the potential for cumulative culture. Our data may provide insights into factors constraining cumulativeculture in the natural behaviour of non-humans.

Learning Hidden Causal Structure from Temporal Data

Past research indicates that humans can infer hidden causesfrom covariational evidence, and readily use temporal informa-tion to infer relationships among events. Here we explore a set-ting in which people can attribute events to a common hiddencause or causal relationships among observed events, includingcausal cycles, purely on the basis of timing information. Wepresent data from three behavioral experiments and extend pre-viously proposed Bayesian models that makes use of order anddelay information for causal structure learning. Our findingssupport the idea that people rely on the delays between eventsrather than order information alone. Meanwhile, deviationsfrom our model predictions suggest that people have an induc-tive bias against common hidden causes and rely on heuristicsto distinguish between causal structures, such as event over-laps, at least with the cover story considered in these experi-ments. Further, our data suggest that people have particularlyflexible representations of cyclic relationships.

Quantifying sound-graphic systematicity and application on multiple phonographs

Do letter-shapes predict in any way the canonical sounds they represent? Does the letter a in any sense visually predictits canonical pronunciation //? We extended existing quantitative approaches to measuring systematicity between phonol-ogy and semantics. We quantified all pairwise visual distances between letters, using Hausdorff distance. We took thecorresponding canonical pronunciations of the letters and quantified all pairwise distances between their feature-level rep-resentations, using edit distance and Euclidean distance. We defined letter-sound systematicity as a correlation betweenthese two lists of distances. We confirmed Korean as the gold standard for letter-sound systematicity; it was designed in the15C to have exactly this characteristic. We found small but significant correlations in Arabic, Cyrillic, English, Finnish,Greek and Hebrew orthographies, with Courier New giving the most consistent correlations. Pitmans English shorthandand the Shavian shorthand alphabet also showed robust systematicity, and baseline fictitious orthographies showed nosystematicity, validating our approach.

Using K-means Clustering for Out-of-Sample Predictions of Memory Retention

In applied settings, computational models of memory haveproven useful in making principled performance predictions.Specifically, historical data are used to derive modelparameters in order to enable out-of-sample predictions.Parameters are typically fit to meaningful subsets of data.However, labels that demarcate what constitutes a“meaningful” subset are not always available. Here, we utilizea data-driven method to cluster past performance into subsetspossessing statistical similarities. We contrast predictions fromcluster-specific model parameters with predictions based onsubsets that are artifacts of the experimental design. We showthat cluster-based predictions are at least as accurate as thechosen baselines and highlight additional advantages of thedata-driven approach.

Preschoolers’ responses to unknown words: Questions and evaluation of definitionquality

Asking questions about unknown things involves recognizing knowledge gaps, identifying information sources, and for-mulating appropriate questions. This active involvement propels development by individualizing the learning environment.To characterize active engagement in word learning, we investigated whether preschoolers ask questions about novel vo-cabulary and evaluate definition quality. In Study 1, preschoolers were asked to perform actions following instructionswith novel (transpose) or familiar (switch) verbs. They asked more questions about novel (M = 3.31 out of 9, SD = 3.34)than familiar verbs (M = .17, SD = .44), t(35) = -5.68, p ¡ .001. In Study 2, informative or uninformative definitions wereprovided. Preliminary data suggest that preschoolers only asked questions when faced with uninformative definitions (M= .65 out of 3). When faced with novel words, preschoolers not only elicit questions, but also determine whether theirinformation needs have been met.

Adapting Educational Technologies Across Learner Populations:A Usability Study with Adolescents on the Autism Spectrum

This paper reports initial results from a usability study con-ducted in the formative and user-centered design phase of alarger project to translate an existing, science-focused edu-cational technology for neurotypical middle school studentsinto a new, social-reasoning-focused educational technologyfor students on the autism spectrum. Participants in our studyincluded both adolescents on the autism spectrum and typi-cally developing adolescents, who were asked to complete theBetty’s Brain educational-technology-based science activity aswell as a social-reasoning movie question-answering activity.Results include qualitative observations of general student en-gagement and challenges as well as quantitative measures ofperformance and eye gaze, including key differences observedacross our two sample groups, with the goal of informingthe design and adaptation of future technology-based inter-ventions. Our findings suggest specific considerations for de-signing educational technologies for adolescents on the autismspectrum, including 1) finding ways to help students followinstructional/tutorial portions of new technologies, especiallywhen lengthy instructions and/or complex interfaces are in-volved; 2) proactively anticipating and finding ways to mit-igate potential student episodes of frustration / dysregulationwhile using the technology; and 3) capitalizing on features ofthe technology found to be engaging/motivating for students.

Item Distinctiveness is More Critical than Item Context in a Cross-SituationalWord Learning Paradigm

Tests of cross-situational word learning use a range of stimuli. How does the distinctiveness of a stimulus affect participantsability to learn its label? In two experiments, participants were presented with pairs of unfamiliar images accompanied bytwo pseudoword labels. The images were either two visually similar robots or two visually dissimilar novel objects. Bydesign, the mapping of label to image was purposefully unclear, and we further manipulated which images were displayedwith one another across trials (i.e., their presentation context). In one condition, pairs of images were randomly determined,while in the other, sets of images consistently appeared with one another throughout training. At test, participants weregiven one label and instructed to match it to one of four possible images. Participants who had been exposed to the visuallydissimilar objects outperformed those who had been exposed to the visually similar robots, regardless of presentationcontext.

Not as Bad as Painted? Legal Expertise, Intentionality Ascription, and Outcome Effects Revisited

Previous research by Kneer and Bourgeois-Gironde (2017) suggests that legal experts are susceptible to the “severity effect” – they ascribe a higher level of intentionality for actions if they lead to very bad side-effects than when they have somewhat bad side-effects. These results are potentially problematic for the legal system because ascriptions of intentionality in the law explicitly depend on the evaluation of mental states of the agent (mens rea), not on the badness of the outcomes she caused. In this paper, we provide and test an alternative explanation of the “severity effect” that has no troubling implications for the law. We suggest that it may be a subtype of a more general “side-effect effect” (Knobe, 2003), which is compatible with certain legal criteria of ascribing intentionality.

What Do Computers Know About Semantics Anyway? Testing DistributionalSemantics Models Against a Broad Range of Relatedness Ratings

Distributional Semantics Models (DSMs) are a primary method for distilling semantic information from corpora. However,a key question remains: What types of semantic relations do DSMs detect? Prior work has addressed this question using alimited set of ratings that typically are either amorphous (association norms) or restricted to semantic similarity (SimLex,SimVerb). We tested four DSMs (SkipGram, CBOW, GloVe, PPMI) using multiple hyperparameters on a theoretically-motivated, rich set of relations involving words from multiple syntactic classes spanning the abstract-concrete continuum(21 sets of ratings). Results show wide variation in the DSMs’ ability to account for the ratings, and that hyperparametertuning buys comparatively little for improving correlations. For CBOW and SkipGram, we included word and contextembeddings. For SkipGram, there was a marked improvement in simulating the human data by averaging them. Ourresults yield important insights into the types of semantic relations that are captured by DSMs.

Graphical vs. Spatial Models of Distributional Semantics

Semantic space models based on distributional information and semantic network (graphical) models are two of the mostpopular models of semantic representation. Both types of models succeed at modeling or explaining various tasks. Bothtypes of models also have limitations. Spatial models have difficulties representing indirect semantic relations, whilegraphical models have lacked a theoretical account for the construction of their semantic network. In this article, wedevelop the Distributional Graph Model. The new model resembles semantic space models in the way that it is a repre-sentation of semantic memory obtained from statistical learning on a linguistic corpus. But like other graphical models,it is able to capture indirect semantic relatedness as well. Using an artificial language specifically designed to test differ-ent types of syntagmatic and paradigmatic relationships, we show that the Distributional Graph Model demonstrates thebenefits of both graphical and spatial distributional models.

Exploring Lexical Relations in BERT using Semantic Priming

BERT is a language processing model trained for word prediction in context, which has shown impressive performancein natural language processing tasks. However, the principles underlying BERT’s use of linguistic cues present in contextare yet to be fully understood. In this work, we develop tests informed by the semantic priming paradigm to investigateBERTs handling of lexical relations to complete a cloze task (Taylor, 1953). We define priming to be an increase in BERTsexpectation for a target word (pilot), in a context (e.g., I want to be a ), when the context is prepended with a relatedword (airplane) as opposed to an unrelated one (table). We explore BERTs priming behavior under various predictiveconstraints placed on the blank, and find that BERT is sensitive to lexical priming effects only under minimal constraintfrom the input context. This pattern was found to be consistent across diverse lexical relations.

Is time travel possible? Childrens intuitive theories about the nature of time

Humans form and revise theories about the world throughout the lifespan. While intuitive theories of the physical andbiological world have been explored, the domain of time is understudied. We explored childrens theories about time byasking 4- to 6-year-olds (n = 38) and adults about the reality or possibility of temporal phenomena. We also asked them torate their confidence in their answers. All children agreed that clocks and aging are real. However, judgements about timetravel, getting younger, seeing the future, and the past and future themselves, changed. While 6-year-olds gave adult-likeresponses to most questions, 5-year-olds were less sure. Unlike older children, 4-year-olds said seeing the future and timetravel are possible, but the past is not real. These results suggest that children converge on adultlike theories about timebetween 4 and 6 years of age. Future work will explore factors driving the formation of these theories.

Choice Strategies in a Changing Social Learning Environment

One challenge that children face when learning from others isthat social agents can behave in unpredictable ways. Socialagents may acquire—or fail to acquire—new information thatinfluences how they interact with the learner. Little is knownabout children’s sensitivity to these changes or howeffectively children update their own behavior in response.Participants (N = 129) searched for rewards while receivingsuggestions from a social agent. The suggestions changed inlevel of reliability over time. All children updated howheavily they weighted the cues after the change. However,younger children were more influenced by their initialexperience with the suggestions, indicating that youngerchildren may have more difficulty disengaging from socialinformation in uncertain learning environments.

Constructing complex social categories from distinct group membershipinformation

Conceptual combination is the act of building complex concepts from simpler ones. Although previous research has ex-amined how inferences about compound objects (e.g., fuzzy chair) are produced from their constituent concepts, littleis known about the combinatorial processes that produce inferences about compound social categories (e.g., Irish Musi-cian). Using a computational approach, we investigated the relationship between trait ratings of 25 nationality-occupationcombinations and ratings of their constituent concepts. 25 non-human animal combinations (e.g., circus snake) serve as acomparison. We find that constituent concepts are weighted unequally when combined: head concepts (Musician/Snake)are prioritized over modifier concepts (Irish/Circus) for both combination types. Additionally, ratings of more familiar so-cial combinations diverge increasingly from ratings of their constituent concepts, whereas ratings of more familiar animalcombinations instead converge with ratings of their constituents. This raises the possibility that existing knowledge playsdifferent roles in peoples inferences about human versus animal categories.

Relations between the Home and Cognitive Development in Nicaraguan Children

Early childhood home environments are well understood to be foundational for cognitive development, yet their relation-ship to specific cognitive skills is challenging to understand empirically in low resourced nations, leading to lack of clarityabout the roles of socialization versus maturation. We examine the contributions of environmental context on culturallyadapted versions of executive functioning (EF; inhibitory control), expressive language, and reasoning tasks (spatial andrelational reasoning) in a representative sample of 1,834 children (24-59 month-olds) in Nicaragua. Multivariate regres-sions revealed children with highly structured homes and enrollment in early education in this context exhibited higherEF, expressive language and reasoning skills, explaining cognitive skills better than socioeconomic status. These resultssuggest these cognitive skills are malleable and impacted by the home context. Language and reasoning skills were alsorelated to more social partners, suggesting language and reasoning are more tied to social interaction than EF.

Decision-Making Under Uncertain Circumstances in Borderline PersonalityDisorder (BPD) Patients

Existing research has developed a working understanding of borderline personality disorder (BPD) patient traits and behav-ior in everyday life, but the subtleties of their cognitive processes during decision-making remains unclear. To understandhow reliance on previous experiences (priors) versus current sensory information (likelihoods) in the decision-makingprocess may differ for those with BPD in comparison to those within neuro-typical population, we implemented a coin-catching behavioral task with varying levels of prior and likelihood uncertainty. We hypothesized that, in accordance totypical BPD characteristics, BPD patients will rely significantly more on likelihood information even when likelihoodinformation is more unreliable than prior information. Analyzing the results using Bayesian statistics, we found evidencesuggesting that both the BPD patient group and the neuro-typical control group utilized prior and likelihood informa-tion similarly in decision-making. We theorize that BPD characteristics that are prominent in social interactions may notexactly replicate in non-social settings.

Executive Function affects Resilience with Different Cognitive Mechanismsbetween Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood

Executive function is a cognitive control system contributes uniquely to resilience (Greenberg, 2006; Obradovic, 2016).This study looked into resilience development during its controversial age period in cognitive perspective, aims to explorehow its components (i.e., cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory) affect resilience in different agegroups. Data were collected in middle schools and universities (N=197). Participants were asked to join a series of labexperiments and questionnaires in a psychological lab. Results showed resilience as well as executive function in algorith-mic mind level develop from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Cognitive flexibility plays central role in functioningresilience with various cognitive mechanisms for different populations. With the identification of cognitive mechanismsunderlying the relation between cognitive flexibility subsets (i.e., reactive flexibility and spontaneous flexibility) and re-silience, this study contributes a cognitive perspective for better understanding of resilience before challenging eventshappen.

Predicting Social Exclusion: A Computational Linguistic Approach to theDetection of Ostracism

Ostracism is a social phenomenon, shared by most social animals, including humans. Its detection plays a crucial role forthe individual, with possible evolutionary consequences for the species.Considering (1) its relation with communication and therefore language and (2) its social nature, we hypothesised that thecombination of linguistic and community-level social features would have a positive impact on the automatic recognitionof ostracism in human online communities.We modelled a linguistic community through Reddit data and we analysed the performance of simple classification al-gorithms (Nave Bayes and SVM), particularly focusing on the feature selection. Comparing the accuracy scores of thealgorithms fed with a) linguistic features, b) extralinguistic features, and c) linguistic + extralinguistic features, we testedour hypothesis, showing how models based on c) generally outperform.To our knowledge, this is the first attempt to automatise the identification of such a complex phenomenon through NLPtechniques.

Text Matters but Speech Influences:A Computational Analysis of Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution

Analyzing how human beings resolve syntactic ambiguity haslong been an issue of interest in the field of linguistics. It is, atthe same time, one of the most challenging issues for spokenlanguage understanding (SLU) systems as well. As syntacticambiguity is intertwined with issues regarding prosody and se-mantics, the computational approach toward speech intentionidentification is expected to benefit from the observations ofthe human language processing mechanism. In this regard, weaddress the task with attentive recurrent neural networks thatexploit acoustic and textual features simultaneously and revealhow the modalities interact with each other to derive sentencemeaning. Utilizing a speech corpus recorded on Korean scriptsof syntactically ambiguous utterances, we revealed that co-attention frameworks, namely multi-hop attention and cross-attention, show significantly superior performance in disam-biguating speech intention. With further analysis, we demon-strate that the computational models reflect the internal rela-tionship between auditory and linguistic processes.

Simulations and theory of generalization in recurrent networks

Despite the tremendous advances of Artificial Intelligence, a general theory of intelligent systems, connecting the psycho-logical, neuroscientific and computational levels is lacking. Artificial Neural Networks are good starting points to buildthe theory. We propose to analyze generalization of learning in simple but challenging problems. We have previouslyproposed to concentrate on learning sameness, as we have shown that this is difficult for a SRN. Here we present theresults of trying to use a Long-Short Term Memory Network to learn sameness. We show that the LSTM although muchmore efficient to learn partial examples of sameness fails to generalize to a proportion of the examples. This suggests thatLSTM and SRN share a core set of features that make generalization of sameness problematic. By analyzing where thetwo models fail, we arrive at a proposal of what makes sameness hard to learn and generalize in recurrent neural networks.

Automatic and Controlled Sentence Production: A Computational Model

We present a computational model of sentence production thatemulates variation of the output of lexicalization andgrammatical encoding of the abstract pre-lexical message, interms of complexity and accuracy of the generated sentence aswell as fluency and cognitive costs of the sentence production.The model integrates approaches from routine action selectionmodels built on Dual Systems Theory (Norman & Shallice,1986) with ‘A Blueprint for the Speaker’ developed by Levelt(1989). The paper describes and justifies the modelarchitecture, explores factors affecting language variation inproduction, and applies the model for testing relationshipbetween complexity, accuracy, and fluency (CAF) of languageproduction as debated within Second Language Acquisition(SLA) research. A simulation that generated 78,750 sentencesprovides evidence of the trade-off relationship between CAFparameters as speakers have to sacrifice performance on one ofthe CAF factors in order to improve the remaining two.

Science & engineering goals: Learning about the control-of-variable strategy from

Children struggle to conduct controlled tests, even with explicit instruction (Chen & Klahr, 1999). How learners approachmultivariable tasks can be affected by task goals; a scientist uncovers causal regularities whereas an engineer produceseffects (Klahr, et al., 2011). This study investigated whether science vs. engineering goals presented in a narrative picturebook influenced childrens ability to conduct a controlled test.Six-to-8-year-olds (N=72) were first pre-tested on their ability to design a controlled test of a variable predicting how fara ball travels down to a ramp. Children were then read a picture book that contained a science (conduct controlled test) orengineering (create faster ramp) goal. Next, they completed an identical post-test and transfer-test with two new variables.Childrens ability to design a controlled test improved significantly from pre- to post-test (p=.008) and marginally frompre-test to transfer (p=.067) in both conditions, suggesting that children learned from both goals.

4- and 5-Year-Olds’ Comprehension of Functional Metaphors

Previous work suggests that children’s ability to understand metaphors emerges late in development. Researchers argue that children’s initial failure to understand metaphors is due to an inability to reason about shared relational structures between concepts. However, recent work demonstrates that causal framing facilitates preschoolers’ relational reasoning. Might causal framing also facilitate preschoolers’ metaphor comprehension? In Experiment 1, we presented 128 4- to 5-year-olds with a novel metaphor comprehension task, following a causal warm-up task, control warm-up task, or no warm-up task. In the novel comprehension task, preschoolers rated functional metaphors and nonsense statements as smart or silly, and provided explanations. Preschoolers ranked metaphors as “smarter” than nonsense statements, and a quarter of preschoolers provided functional explanations. There was no effect of warm-up tasks. In Experiment 2, we validated the metaphor comprehension task with adults. Overall, the current work presents a new paradigm that demonstrates preschoolers’ capacity to understand functional metaphors.

Positive Effects of a Developmental Period Without Control

Executive control processes allow task-appropriate behaviour across cognitive domains, yet, children have a long devel-opmental period with little or no control. Traditionally, this is viewed as a negative but necessary consequence of the timetaken for prefrontal development and learning control processes. Here we examine a recent model of controlled semanticcognition (https://biorxiv.org/cgi/content/short/860528v1) as a test case to present evidence for an alternative (yet perhaps,complementary) view; that a developmental period without control has a positive functional role in learning. Varying thelength of a developmental period without control, we identify an optimal period (around one third of the learning time)which allows conceptual learning to happen much faster, without loss of conceptual abstraction ability. This speeding ismediated by the way control interacts with representation regions (deeper multimodal ¿ shallower input areas). This hasimplications for our understanding of controlled semantic cognition and the development of control more generally.

Visual Statistical Learning Is Facilitated in Zipfian Distributions

Humans can extract co-occurrence regularities from their environment, and use them for learning. This statistical learningability (SL) has been studied extensively. However, almost all SL studies present the regularities to be learned in uniformfrequency distributions (each unit appears equally often). In contrast, real-world learning environments, including thewords children hear and the objects they see, are not uniform, and consequently more predictable than lab-based ones.Recent research shows that word segmentation in children and adults is facilitated after exposure to a Zipfian distribution.Here, we ask if this effect is domain-general by testing children and adults on a visual SL task. Both children and adultsperformed better in the Zipfian distribution compared to the uniform one, overall, and for low-frequency triplets. Theseresults illustrate the impact of distribution predictability on learning across modality and age, and point to the possiblelearnability advantage of skewed distributions in the real-world.

The Signature of All Things: Children Infer Knowledge States from Static Images

From minimal observable action, humans automatically make fast, intuitive judgments about what other people think,want, and feel (Heider & Simmel, 1944). Even when no agent is visible, children can infer the presence of intentionalagents based on the environmental traces that only agents could leave behind (Saxe et al., 2005; Newman et al., 2010).Here we show that four- to six-year-olds can go beyond inferring the presence of an agent to matching an agents mentalstate with the trace they left behind. Participants (N = 35, M: 5.6 years, range:4.0 6.8 years) saw pairs of dresser drawerswith different numbers and orientations of open drawers, and were asked to match one of the static scenes to an agentsknowledge state (whether the agent wasnt searching at all but was just playing, knew exactly where an object was hidden,knew the approximate location, had no idea where it was hidden, or at first didnt know and then remembered). We comparechildrens performance to a formal model, in which we build upon classical models of Bayesian Theory of Mind that treatmental state inferences as a form of inverse planning; here we extend those models to consider cases where the behavioris not observed but must be inferred from the structure of the environment.

Domestic dogs’ understanding of spatial temporal priority

Dogs are recognized for their social reasoning and skillful interactions with humans, but their understanding of causal rela-tionships and the underlying principles (e.g., temporal priority) are under-explored. To address this gap, we adapted a taskused with children to investigate how pet dogs use temporal sequences of events. Dogs (N=22) watched an experimenterperform a sequence of two actions on a puzzle box: i) one action before a treat was dispensed from the box (causal action)and ii) the other action after the treat appeared (non-causal action). Each action was temporally equidistant from the treat.After observing the sequence, dogs interacted with the box. Preliminary results indicate that over the course of five trialsdogs preferred interacting with the causal action and were more likely to investigate it first, compared to the non-causalaction on trial one. Results will be discussed in a comparative context of observational and experiential learning.

How nouns surface as verbs: Inference and generation in word class conversion

Word class conversion refers to the extended use of a wordfrom one grammatical class to another without overt morpho-logical marking. Noun-to-verb conversion, or denominaliza-tion, is one form of word class conversion studied extensivelyin the literature. Previous work has suggested that novel de-nominal verb usages are comprehensible if the listener cancompute the intended meaning based on shared knowledgewith the speaker. However, no existing work has explored thecomputational mechanism under this proposal. We proposea frame-semantic generative model, Noun2Verb, that supportsthe inference and generation of novel denominal verb usagesvia semi-supervised learning. We evaluate this framework ina dataset of denominal verbs drawn from adults and childrenagainst a state-of-the-art model from natural language process-ing. Our results show that Noun2Verb aligns better with humaninterpretation and bridges the gap between machines and hu-mans in lexical innovation.

Costly Exceptions: Deviant Exemplars Reduce Category Compression

We investigated whether the presence of exception items can impede effects of category compression (within-category items appearing more similar) in classification learning. We hypothesized that the distinct representations afforded to exceptions may cause the target category to appear less cohesive, thereby reducing the likelihood of compression occurring. Across two experiments, participants engaged in classification learning without exceptions, with an easy exception, or with a difficult exception. Pairwise similarity ratings for all items were collected before and after learning to index compression. Results from Experiment 1 suggest that difficult exceptions can impede compression for the contrast category when situated within its cluster, while results from Experiment 2 suggest that both kinds of exceptions can impair compression of standard items in a target category relative to the No Exception control. We also observed surprising evidence of a novel between-category compression effect that was observed with the category structure developed for these experiments.

Learning to cooperate: Emergent communication in multi-agent navigation

Emergent communication in artificial agents has been studiedto understand language evolution, as well as to develop artifi-cial systems that learn to communicate with humans. We showthat agents performing a cooperative navigation task in variousgridworld environments learn an interpretable communicationprotocol that enables them to efficiently, and in many cases,optimally, solve the task. An analysis of the agents’ policiesreveals that emergent signals spatially cluster the state space,with signals referring to specific locations and spatial direc-tions such as left, up, or upper left room. Using populationsof agents, we show that the emergent protocol has basic com-positional structure, thus exhibiting a core property of naturallanguage.

Acoustic Features of Infant Directed Speech in Female and Male Speakers

Infant directed speech (IDS) is characterized by exaggerated pitch and vowel lengthening. The current study recorded ev-eryday interactions with fifty 12-month-old infants and their families to examine whether there are significant differencesin the acoustic features of IDS (such as frequency, pause duration, and vowel length) between male and female speak-ers, and whether any differences are related to childrens vocabulary development at 12 months and 15 months. Femalespeakers, compared with male speakers, exhibited significantly longer pauses in phrase final positions, thereby poten-tially signaling syntactic structures more clearly. Controlling for family income and maternal education, female speakersfrequency variation at non-final vowel positions accounted for an additional unique variance for infants productive vocab-ulary at 12 months and receptive vocabulary at 15 months while none of the acoustic features of male speakers related tovocabulary size. These results suggest that female speakers IDS may be more influential in language development.

Lexicalization of quantificational forces in adverbial and determiner domains

Which quantificational forces do languages encode lexically?When a language features multiple quantificational scales(e.g. determiner and adverbial quantification), does the pat-tern of lexicalization of quantificational forces we discoverfor one scale correlate with those of other scales? We useEnglish as a first test case for examining these questions,adapting the basic ideas of Lewis (1975) into the hypothesisthat English lexical quantifiers unrelated to cardinal numbersor definite descriptions, determiner and adverbial alike, haveone of six quantificational forces. To begin to test this claimempirically, we elicited speaker interpretations of a range ofquantifiers in a web-based study. Dividing participants into anadverbial condition and a determiner condition, we gave acontext specifying a 100-day period and asked participants tojudge the quantificational force of quantified sentences denot-ing an individual’s daily activities during this period. Wefound evidence of cross-scale correspondences but fewerquantificational forces than expected. These results providepreliminary evidence for parts of our hypothesis but suggest aneed for future research that covers more lexical items, lan-guages, and quantificational scales.

Causal Learning with Two Causes over Weeks

When making causal inferences, prior research shows that peopleare capable of controlling for alternative causes. These studies,however, utilize artificial inter-trial intervals on the order ofseconds; in real-life situations people often experience data overdays and weeks (e.g., learning the effectiveness of two newmedications over multiple weeks). In the current study, participantslearned about two possible causes from data presented in atraditional trial-by-trial paradigm (rapid series of trials) versus amore naturalistic paradigm (one trial per day for multiple weeks viasmartphone). Our results suggest that while people are capable ofdetecting simple cause-effect relations that do not requirecontrolling for another cause when learning over weeks, they havedifficulty learning cause-effect relations that require controlling foralternative causes.

Multimodal Learning: An Investigation Into Memory Integration AcrossRepresentational Formats

Learning occurs across distributed multimodal experiences. To accumulate knowledge one must integrate related infor-mation across different representational formats i.e. across text and photographs. We extended an established memoryintegration paradigm to test acquisition and integration of knowledge across different representational formats based onart history museum exhibits. Participants received integrable passage pairs in either text-text or text-text+photographformats. Even though the processing demands were higher with photographs, preliminary results indicate no significantdifferences between conditions. Future work will examine potential differences in integration across both context (class-room to museum) and representational formats (text and museum artifacts). We hypothesize that integration across contextand representational format will create higher cognitive demand than integration across representational formats; will thisbe offset by the higher information-value of museum exhibits? This research will provide key insights into multimodallearning and inform best practices for maximizing comprehension in informal learning settings such as museums.

Abstraction and Generalization: Comparing Adaptive Models of Categorization

Between prototype and exemplar models of categorization lie adaptive models, which represent categories using a varyingnumber of reference points. They regulate the amount of abstraction they make depending on the category structure. Moti-vated by ecological considerations, we investigate whether adopting such adaptive strategies could improve generalizationin realistic environments. We compare performance of four adaptive models: RMC, SUSTAIN, REX, VAM with that ofprototype and exemplar models on three artificial and three natural category structures. Both the exemplar model withadapted sensitivity parameter and VAM perform well on category structures requiring different amount of abstraction. Ourresults confirm the importance of the link between abstraction and generalization.

The Relation between Gist and Item Memory Over a Month

Memory requires both individuation of specific episodes aswell as extraction of gist across related experiences. Thisstudy developed a spatial memory paradigm to track changesin item memory (memory for specific locations) and gistmemory (estimate of the center of the locations) across aperiod of a month, and to measure the relation between thesetwo forms of memory. We found that item memories decayedcompared to gist memory after a month, yet there was apositive relationship between the two forms of memory thatpersisted. Moreover, item memories were biased towards gistmemory only after a month. These findings together indicatethat gist memory, initially extracted from item memories,gradually develops into a stable representation that can guideitem memory retrieval over longer durations.

Investigating Simple Object Representations in Model-Free Deep ReinforcementLearning

We explore the benefits of augmenting state-of-the-art model-free deep reinforcement learning with simple object representa-tions. Following the Frostbite challenge posited by Lake et al.(2017), we identify object representations as a critical cognitivecapacity lacking from current reinforcement learning agents.We discover that providing the Rainbow model (Hessel et al.,2018) with simple, feature-engineered object representationssubstantially boosts its performance on the Frostbite game fromAtari 2600. We then analyze the relative contributions of therepresentations of different types of objects, identify environ-ment states where these representations are most impactful, andexamine how these representations aid in generalizing to novelsituations.

Diagnosing pervasive issues with parameter estimation

We explore structural issues with parameter estimation fornon-linear cognitive models: Some parameter values are eas-ier to recover than others, and the recoverability of differentparameters interacts in systematic ways. We propose methodsfor researchers to anticipate and visualize and these issues, andthe systematic ways they differ across experimental designs.Our approach consists of assessing how changes in parame-ter values translate into changes in behavioral predictions, anddevelop measurements of the relative responsiveness of predic-tions to parameter values. We demonstrate application of ourapproach to cumulative prospect theory (CPT), a widely-usedmodel of risky decision-making.

Requisite Variety, Cognition, and Scientific Change

Multiple theories of scientific change have been prominently promulgated since Kuhn. A quasi-discipline “Scientonomy” has even been proposed to formalize these theories. The cybernetics principle known as “The Law of Requisite Variety (LRV)” when combined with cognitive science insights regarding categorization and its ilk can be used to chart one such formalism. LRV holds that control/prediction can only be assured when the internal complexity of a system matches the external complexity it confronts The key indicator of an activity directed at scientific change comes from examinations of the models which scientists deploy in attempting to link pre-existing explanations with new problems to be explained. Normal science is a reductive activity – limiting the variety encountered. Innovative science is the process of expanding such variety, and scientific change is what happens when the innovative crosses the threshold for normal.

Contextual Interference Effect in Motor Skill Learning: An Empirical andComputational Investigation

To efficiently learn and retain motor skills, we can introducecontextual interference through interleaved practice.Interleaving tasks or stimuli initially hinders performance butleads to superior long-term retention. It is not yet clear ifimplicitly learned information also benefits from interleavingand how interleaved practice changes the representation ofskills. The present study used a serial reaction time task whereparticipants practiced three 8-item sequences that were eitherinterleaved or blocked on Day 1 (training) and Day 2 (testing).An explicit recall test allowed us to post-hoc sort participantsinto two groups of learners: implicit learners recalled less itemsthan did explicit learners. Significant decreasing monotonictrends, indicating successful learning, were observed in bothtraining groups and both groups of learners. We found supportfor the benefit of interleaved practice on retention of implicitsequence learning, indicating that the benefit of interleavedpractice does not depend on explicit memory retrieval. ABayesian Sequential Learning model was adopted to modelhuman performance. Both empirical and computational resultssuggest that explicit knowledge of the sequence wasdetrimental to retention when the sequences were blocked, butnot when they were interleaved, suggesting that contextualinterference may be a protective factor of interference ofexplicit knowledge. Slower learning in the interleavedcondition may result in better retention and reducedinterference of explicit knowledge on performance.

An Investigation of the Multilingual and Bi-dialectal Advantage in Executive Control

We examined the effect of speaking more than one language (multilingualism) or two dialects of the same language (bi- dialectalism) on executive control (EC) by administering seven EC tasks to 46 multilingual, 72 bi-dialectal and 47 monolingual young adults. We used the EC model of Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager (2000) according to which EC comprises three components: working memory, task-switching and inhibition. We also tested two theoretical views regarding the locus of the bilingual advantage: first, that bilingualism affects specific EC components and, second, that bilingualism has a more general effect on the whole EC network. Miyake et al.’s (2000) model was a good fit to our EC data. We also found that both multilinguals and bi-dialectals had significantly higher EC scores than monolinguals. Moreover, both the multilingual and the bi-dialectal advantage was found in overall EC ability and could not be attributed to a specific EC component.

Generalizing Outside the Training Set:When Can Neural Networks Learn Identity Effects?

Often in language and other areas of cognition, whether twocomponents of an object are identical or not determine whetherit is well formed. We call such constraints identity effects.When developing a system to learn well-formedness from ex-amples, it is easy enough to build in an identify effect. But canidentity effects be learned from the data without explicit guid-ance? We provide a simple framework in which we can rig-orously prove that algorithms satisfying simple criteria cannotmake the correct inference. We then show that a broad classof algorithms including deep neural networks with standardarchitecture and training with backpropagation satisfy our cri-teria, dependent on the encoding of inputs. Finally, we demon-strate our theory with computational experiments in which weexplore the effect of different input encodings on the ability ofalgorithms to generalize to novel inputs.

Rhythmic abilities in prereaders predict future reading skills

Rhythmic abilities have been related to language processing skills such as phonological awareness, rise time discriminationand verbal memory. Following this reasoning, they have also been linked to reading acquisition. In particular, in prereaders,tapping to a beat, a task that entails rhythmic processing through auditory-motor synchronization (AMS), has shown todiscriminate children with poor and good phonological skills. However, evidence regarding how the AMS-reading linkdevelops through time, starting before reading instruction, is scarce. In the present study, we followed a large sample of600 children from kindergarten to second grade, through a digital assessment of literacy and literacy-related skills, as wellas rhythmic abilities. We found that AMS in K5 uniquely contributes to future reading performance, above and beyondphonological skills. These findings underscore the role of rhythmic abilities in reading acquisition, and its relation tophonological processing.

Can a Composite Metacognitive Judgment Accuracy Score Successfully CapturePerformance Variance during Multimedia Learning?

Theoretical models of self-regulated learning highlight theimportance and dynamic nature of metacognitive monitoringand regulation. However, traditional research typically has notexamined how different judgments, or the relative timing ofthose judgments, influence each other, especially in complexlearning environments. We compared six statistical modelsof performance of undergraduates (n = 55) learning inMetaTutor-IVH, a multimedia learning environment. Threetypes of prompted metacognitive judgments (ease of learning[EOL] judgments, content evaluations [CEs], and retrospectiveconfidence judgments [RCJs]) were used as individualpredictors, and combined in a uniformly-weighted compositescore and empirically based weighted composite score acrossthe learning session. The uniformly weighted composite scorebetter captured performance than the models using only anEOL judgment or RCJ judgment. However, the empiricallyweighted composite model outperformed all other models.Our results suggest that metacognitive judgments should notbe considered as independent phenomenon but as an intricateand interconnected process.

Inferring physical cause from statistical anomalies

People have an intuitive sense of probability beginning early in life, where they appreciate that samples should reflectpopulations in their statistical properties (e.g., Denison & Xu, 2019). We examined whether adult participants in twoexperiments (N=132; N=141, respectively) can use this intuitive sense to infer unseen properties that might be affectingthe sampling process. In both experiments, adults saw boxes with different sized balls in varying proportions. They thensaw sampling events, in which small numbers of balls were shaken from a hidden exit on the top of the box, that wereeither probable or improbable, based on box proportions. In general, adults appropriately inferred constraints on the sizeof the hidden exit by integrating information from the sizes of the balls that were sampled and the overall distribution ofballs in the box. Ongoing work examines whether toddlers can make similar inferences.

Modeling pupillary surprise response in elementary school children withtheory-based Bayesian models

Affective components are frequently overlooked in computational modelling, despite the notable role of emotions in learn-ing. Towards the goal of measuring affect in learning, we developed a theory-based Bayesian model that predicts surprisebased on a learners prior beliefs and the evidence observed, and then compared the model to a physiological measure com-monly suggested to capture surprise: pupil dilation. Critically, we also investigate whether this correlation is strong whenparticipants predict the events. Comparing our model predictions to the first four test trial responses from 93 participants(mean age: 8.00 years) revealed a significant, positive correlation when making predictions (r(9)=.55, p=0.04), a negativecorrelation when only evaluating (r(9)=-.50, p=0.07), and significant difference between groups (z=2.34, p¡0.01). Nextsteps will allow us to build on this result by developing a modified Bayesian model, that takes physiological surprise as acomponent in predicting the participants learning.

Accessing Distant Analogs Over Superficial Matches: ¿How Efficient is theArchitecture of our Retrieval Systems?

Traditional results using a cued-recall paradigm have allegedlydemonstrated that distant analogs tend to be retrieved less oftenthan disanalogous matches maintaining only surface similarity.Recent results, however, suggest that said advantage may be due tothe inadvertent inclusion of structural similarity in surface matches.In two experiments we had distant analogs compete in LTMwith two types of surface matches lacking any degree ofstructural overlap, but equated with the target in terms ofelement similarities. Distant analogs were less retrieved thatstories maintaining similar first-order relations and objects withthe target, but no overlapping structure. This differencedisappeared when surface similarity involved only similar objects.Results show that the surface superiority effect relies on thetype of surface matches that compete with distant analogs, thussuggesting a more complex picture of the forces that governaccess to similar items in memory.

FrameNet for Modeling Extraction from Coordinate Structures

A non-probabilistic model of speakers competence regarding extraction from a coordinate structure, which was argued tobe sensitive to how the conjuncts are connected to each other in discourse (e.g., Lakoff, 1986; Kehler 2002) is presented.The model makes use of Lakoffs (1986) account of the acceptability of extraction from coordinate structures by adoptingthe Frame Semantics framework. Lakoff argues that acceptability of extraction is affected by the belonging of the conjunctsto certain scenarios (e.g. a natural sequence of events), something that is measurable in this framework. An algorithm thatmeasures the degree of relatedness between two conjuncts by consulting FrameNet (the framework implementation) andquantifying the common frames they belong to is proposed and tested on sentences used in an acceptability judgementsurvey on extraction from coordinate structures (Harris, 2009). The models outcomes interact with the experimentalconditions in predicting human judgements, providing initial support for the proposal.

Grammatical marking and the tradeoff between code length and informativeness

Functionalist accounts of language suggest that formsare paired with meanings in ways that support efficientcommunication. Previous work on grammatical markingsuggests that word forms have lengths that enable efficientproduction, and previous work on the semantic typologyof the lexicon suggests that word meanings representefficient partitions of semantic space. Here we consider anintegrated information-theoretic framework that captures howcommunicative pressures influence both form and meaning.We take tense systems as a case study, and show how theframework explains both which tense systems are attestedacross languages and the length asymmetries of the forms inthose systems.

Deep daxes: Mutual exclusivity arises through both learning biases and pragmaticstrategies in neural networks

Children’s tendency to associate novel words with novel refer-ents has been taken to reflect a bias toward mutual exclusivity.This tendency may be advantageous both as (1) an ad-hoc ref-erent selection heuristic to single out referents lacking a labeland as (2) an organizing principle of lexical acquisition. Thispaper investigates under which circumstances cross-situationalneural models can come to exhibit analogous behavior to chil-dren, focusing on these two possibilities and their interaction.To this end, we evaluate neural networks’ on both symbolicdata and, as a first, on large-scale image data. We find thatconstraints in both learning and selection can foster mutual ex-clusivity, as long as they put words in competition for lexi-cal meaning. For computational models, these findings clarifythe role of available options for better performance in taskswhere mutual exclusivity is advantageous. For cognitive re-search, they highlight latent interactions between word learn-ing, referent selection mechanisms, and the structure of stimuliof varying complexity: symbolic and visual.

The Effect of State Representations in Sequential Sensory Prediction: Introducingthe Shape Sequence Task

How do humans learn models supporting decision making?Reinforcement learning (RL) is a success story both in ar-tificial intelligence and neuroscience. Essential to these RLmodels are state representations. Based on what current statean animal or artificial agent is in, they learn optimal actionsby maximizing future expected reward. But how are humansable to learn and create representations of states? We introducea novel sequence prediction task with hidden structure whereparticipants have to combine learning and memory to find theproper state representation, without the task explicitly indicat-ing such structure. We show that humans are able to find thispattern, while a sensory prediction error version of RL cannot,unless equipped with appropriate state representations. Fur-thermore, in slight variations of the task, making it more diffi-cult for humans, the RL-derived model with simple state rep-resentations sufficiently describes behaviour and suggests thathumans fall back on simple state representations when a moreoptimal task representation cannot be found. We argue thistask allows to investigate previously proposed models of stateand task representations as well as supporting recent resultsindicating that RL describes a more general sensory predictionerror function for dopamine, rather than predictions focussedsolely on reward.

Group- and Individual-Level Information Affects Children’s Playmate Choice

Social relationships such as playmates and friendships are im-portant for children’s development. But relatively little isknown about how such relationships are formed. In two stud-ies, 5- to 6-year-old children chose their playmates in a hypo-thetical scenario that resembled a real-world social situation.The findings suggested that children used both the base-rateinformation about the social group and the adaptive samplingstrategy in playmate choice – they approached or avoided in-dividuals based on the group that the individuals belonged to,as well as their past experiences with the individuals.

Chance-Discovery and Chance-Curation in Online Communities

In this paper, we consider chance-curation (the task of eas-ing chance-discovery activities for agents) as far as it concernsinformation sharing in online communities, understood as Vir-tual Cognitive Niches. We claim that Virtual Cognitive Nichesare digitally-encoded collaborative distributions of informa-tion and pieces of knowledge into the environment. The par-ticularity of Virtual Cognitive Niches, as socially biased net-works, is that they provide more ways for agents to interactthan to control the quality of the information they share and re-ceive. We contend that this social bias enables chance-curationstrategies that agents cannot foster in real-life communities. Inparticular, the chance curation strategies that we discuss are:redirecting the attention of agents to the virtual domain, foster-ing an only-docility-based relation with truth, and increasingthe social virtues of fallacies.

Cooperation, Response Time, and Social Value Orientation: A Meta-Analysis

Recent research at the cross between cognitive and social sci-ences is investigating the cognitive mechanisms behind coop-erative decisions. One debated question is whether cooperativedecisions are made faster than non-cooperative ones. Yet em-pirical evidence is still mixed. In this paper we explore theimplications of individual heterogeneity in social value orien-tation for the effect of response time on cooperation. We con-duct a meta-analysis of available experimental studies (n=8;treatments=16; 5,232 subjects). We report two main results:(i) the relation between response time and cooperation is mod-erated by social value orientation, such that it is positive forindividualist subjects and negative for prosocial subjects; (ii)the relation between response time and cooperation is partlymediated by extremity of choice. These results suggest thathighly prosocial subjects are fast to cooperate, highly individ-ualist subjects are fast to defect, and subjects with weaker pref-erences make slower and less extreme decisions. We explainthese results in terms of decision-conflict theory.

Understanding scalar implicature without scale markers SOME and ALL inJapanese preschoolers and adults

Understanding Some girls have bags is difficult for preschoolers, because they may not properly calculate scalar implica-ture. In this study, we examined whether preschoolers and adults guess scalar information without scale markers (some,all) using negative/positive Japanese sentences in picture selection task. This task consisted of three cards, e.g., SOMEcard illustrated two persons with a bag and four persons without a bag. The results were that the positive expression,Baggu wo motte-iru hito ga imasu (there are persons with a bag), was guessed as meaning of ALL (all persons have a bag)by children, while guessed as SOME by adults. Interestingly, the negative expression, Baggu wo motte-inai hito ga imasu(there are persons without a bag), was guessed as NONE by children, but guessed as SOME by adults. The results suggestchildren may not utilize the combination of existence and agents state information to guess scalar implicature.

Retrieving a Distant Analog From Memory in Daily Life is Very Unlikely, Evenin Optimal Conditions of Encoding

Against the typical results from laboratory studies, it has beensuggested that retrieving distant analogs might be easy in real-life, where we tend to encode familiar situations with expert-like schemas. In each of two experiments, we formed twogroups of participants who, as determined by a questionnairepresented during a first session, had reported that they haveexperienced an event corresponding to a schema-governedcategory (Experiment 1) or to a system of schema-governedcategories (Experiment 2). While the episodes reported by oneof the groups belonged to the same domain as the target analogto be presented during the second session, those of the othergroup belonged to a different thematic domain. During atemporally and contextually separated session, the experimenterspresented both groups with a target analog belonging to theschema-governed category for which participants had reporteda base analog. Participants had to retrieve an autobiographicalepisode that they considered analogous to the situationpresented by the experimenter. In line with traditional studies,we found that retrieving distant instances of relationalcategories is much more difficult than retrieving closeinstances.

Modelling the Emergence of Positional Compositional Structure

In a compositional language the meaning of a sentence is a function of the meaning of its parts and the way they arecombined. Recent computational models of the emergence of compositionality have focused on the emergence of wordswhich encode sub-units of meaning in sub-units of form. Decidedly less attention has been paid to the emergence of rulesgoverning the combination of these words. Our work uses LSTM networks in an iterated learning set-up to provide anaccount of how some aspects of compositional structure may emerge through cumulative cultural evolution. We presenta novel metric for assessing the degree of positional structure present in an emergent model and use it to illustrate howcanonical word order may emerge naturally in LSTM models. This supports the notion that some elements of linguisticstructure result more from the dynamics of language transmission and use than domain-specific cognitive biases.

Immediate action-effects facilitate response speed via stimulus-responseassociation

Eitam et al. (2013) reported that immediate feedback to response could motivate the same response in the followingtrials. They suggested action-effects could have a value as information on control over the environment, resulting inthe response facilitation. However, the underlying mechanism of such faciliatory effects, what particular processes action-effects reinforce, remains unclear. Therefore, we investigated whether the response facilitation depends on actions, stimuli,or stimulus-response relationship. Participants were required to select adequate responses in accordance with the stimulias response cue. The action-effects depended on the combination of stimuli and responses; immediate and lagged effectscould be predicted by the stimulus, but shared the same response button. Results showed that the response was executedfaster when driven by stimuli associated with immediate effects than those associated with lagged effects. This indicatesthat immediate action-effects might facilitate response via stimuli-response association, but not via independent processesof actions or stimuli.

Motor Chunking During Sequence Learning in Grid-Navigation Tasks

Several canonical experimental paradigms (serial reaction task, mxn task, etc.) have been proposed to study the typi-cal behavioural phenomena in a sequential motor key-press task. The repeated execution of visuomotor sequences insuch paradigms lead to overall performance improvement such that the inter-response intervals in between certain sub-sequences decreases as compared to that across other sub-sequences. This efficient and hierarchical cluster organisation iscalled motor chunking. We provide empirical evidence for motor chunking in grid-navigation sequencing tasks. The par-ticipants performed Grid-Sailing Task (GST) [Fermin et. al., 2010] that required navigating a 10x10 grid from start to goalposition while using a particular key-mapping between the 3 cursor movement directions and the 3 keyboard buttons. Thisstudy confirms the emergence of subject-specific, unique temporal patterns related to chunking after substantial practice.

End-to-End Models for the Analysis of System 1 and System 2 Interactions basedon Eye-Tracking Data

The Stroop test evaluates the ability to inhibit cognitive interference. This interference occurs when the processing of onestimulus characteristic affects the simultaneous processing of another attribute of the same stimulus. Eye movements are anindicator of the individual attention load required for inhibiting cognitive interference. We used an eye tracker to collecteye movements data from more than 60 subjects each performing four different but similar tasks (some with cognitiveinterference and some without). After the extraction of features related to fixations, saccades and gaze trajectory, wetrained different Machine Learning models to recognize tasks performed in the different conditions (i.e. with interference,without interference). The models achieved good classification performances when distinguishing between similar tasksperformed with or without cognitive interference. This suggests the presence of characterizing patterns common amongsubjects, despite of the individual variability of visual behavior. The results open up interesting investigations.

Configurative Weighting as a Two-Plane Approximation of Bayesian Estimates

Configurative weighting and adding can be a surprisingly effective approximation of multiplicative functions. In thecontext of joint probability judgment, Nilsson et al. (2009) has shown that, when marginal probabilities are only approxi-mately known, the configurative weighted average (CWA) of two probabilities not only predicts a high level of conjunctionfallacies, as observed in data, but also correlates higher with the true joint probability than if the two probabilities are mul-tiplied. Here we show that [1] the surface representing the optimal Bayesian estimate of a joint probability can be closelyapproximated by two planes, [2] configurative weighting and adding, such as the CWA model, constitutes such a two-planeapproximation, and [3] a bias-variance tradeoff is not sufficient to explain the accuracy of the CWA. More generally, thissuggests that the efficiency of heuristics might be due to suitable weighting operations rather than less-is-more effects.

Are Mental Representations of Object Shape Always Quickly Updateable duringLanguage Comprehension?

Research demonstrates that when participants read a sentence about an agent in a certain location and then are showna pictured object, verification time is shorter whenever the pictured object matches the final object state implied by thesentence. Using a sentence-picture verification paradigm, we set out to investigate if the same pattern of results holdstrue when proprioceptive and kinesthetic experiences are considered. In three experiments participants read sentencesthat implied object state-changes as a function of the impact caused by differently weighted items (You drop a bowlingball/balloon on a tomato) followed by a pictured object in either a canonical (e.g., a round tomato) or a non-canonical (e.g.,a squashed tomato) state. The results showed that depictions of non-canonical objects showed the effect, but depictionsof canonical objects did not. Thus, representations of object states compete when non-visual features of the situation areimplied by the sentential context.

Are modal representations automatic ingrained when processing the meaning ofmotor concrete Spanish verbs?

For modal approaches to conceptualization, concepts are couched by a corpus of cognitive processes such as perception,language, and action. Motor verbs offer an opportunity to evaluate the automatic onset of a clear spatial and modalcomponent in the mental representations of linguistic items. This study aimed to test the automaticity of these spatialcomponents when processing the meaning of concrete motor verbs. In one eye-tracking experiment, 31 participants viewed144 Spanish rebug sentences (i.e. escurrir ) with four schematic pictures (left, right, up, down) by a visual word paradigm.The study registered more and larger visits on the schematic pictures related to the motor and perceptive experience whendoing the action refereed by the verbs. Mainly, these findings add evidence on the automaticity of the modal componentof mental representations and help to understand how this component is ingrained in language for action meaning.

Malleability of Working Memory Through Chess in Schoolchildren— A Two-Year Intervention Study

Working memory is the ability to actively maintain information in conscious awareness, carry out cognitive operations on it, and produce an outcome. Working memory holds a small amount of information in the mind and is used in the execution of cognitive tasks, in contrast to long-term memory, which is extensive. Many important cognitive behaviors, such as reading, reasoning, and problem-solving, require working memory because for each of these activities, some information must be maintained in an accessible state while new information is processed and potentially distracting information is ignored. While the effect of chess training on intelligence and academic performance has been examined, its impact on working memory needs to be studied. This study, funded by the Cognitive Science Research Initiative, Department of Science and Technology, Government of India, analyzed the effect of 2-year chess training on the working memory of children. A pretest–posttest with control group design was used. The randomly selected sample consisted of 88 children in the experimental group and 90 children in the control group for the baseline and first-year assessments. Children of both genders studying in school (grades 3 to 9) comprised the sample. At the second-year assessment, there were 80 children in the experimental group and 77 in the control group. The experimental group underwent weekly chess training for 2 years, while the control group was actively involved in sports and extracurricular activities offered by the school. Working memory was measured by two subtests of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) INDIA. The children were trained using Winning Moves curriculum, audiovisual learning method, hands-on chess training and recording the games using score sheets, and analyzing their mistakes. They were also trained in Opening theory, Checkmating techniques, End-game theory, and Tactical principles. Analysis of covariance revealed that the experimental group had significant gains in working memory compared to the control group. The present study supports a link between chess training and working memory. The transfer of skills acquired in chess training to the improvement of working memory could be attributed to the fact that while playing chess, children evaluate positions, visualize new positions in their mind, evaluate the pros and cons of each move, and choose moves based on the information stored in their mind. If working memory’s capacity could be expanded or made to function more efficiently, it could result in the improvement of executive functions as well as the scholastic performance of the child.

Childrens Understanding of Relational Vocabulary for Ordinal and MagnitudeRelations

Although substantial work investigates childrens understanding of ordinal and magnitude-based relations, little work hasinvestigated childrens understanding of the vocabulary used for these relations and how relational language knowledgemay be constrained by symbolic number knowledge. In the current study, children were asked which of two numbers wasbigger/smaller than or before/after five. On close trials, the correct answer was 4 or 6 (one away from 5) and on far trials,the correct answer was 3 or 7 (two away from 5). We hypothesized that 4- to 6-year-olds understanding of ordinal relations(before/after) are initially constrained to refer to numbers immediately before/after (i.e., close values), but that this is notthe case for bigger/smaller comparisons. Preliminary results suggest this to be the case, with children performing betteron close trials than far trials for ordinal relations, but not magnitude relations.

Vicious Loop? Longitudinal Relations Between Math Anxiety and MathPerformance for Grade 2 and 3 Students

Math anxiety is a common correlate of math performance. However, the causal direction of this relation is unclear.Research with young students is limited but critical for determining patterns of development. Students (N = 147) completedmath measures (i.e., number comparison, arithmetic fluency, and math problem solving) and math anxiety assessmentstwice, first in grade 2 (Mage = 7 years:10 months) and then a year later in grade 3. Correlational analysis revealed thatmath anxiety is related only to arithmetic fluency, related to other types of math performance. Cross-lagged analyses wereconducted to evaluate causal relations between math anxiety and arithmetic fluency. These analyses showed that arithmeticfluency in grade 2 predicted change in math anxiety from grade 2 to grade 3, however, math anxiety in grade 2 did notpredict the change in arithmetic fluency from grade 2 to grade 3. These results suggest that math anxiety may be the resultof poor math performance.

Does Prior Knowledge influence Learners Cognitive and Metacognitive Strategiesover Time during Game-based Learning?

Learners ability to effectively monitor and apply cognitive (e.g., reading) and metacognitive (e.g., content evaluations)strategies in game-based learning environments (GBLEs) are influenced by internal factors such as prior knowledge. Thisstudy examined whether there were differences in learners strategy usage over time during learning with Crystal Island,a GBLE for microbiology, between high and low prior knowledge groups. Results indicated that learners with high priorknowledge had greater posttest scores, but spent less time reading. This is further influenced by relative time in gamewhere learners with high prior knowledge have greater reading durations at the start of the game, but smaller durationstowards the end compared to low prior knowledge learners. Learners’ metacognitive strategy usage did not differ betweenprior knowledge groups, but the use of this strategy increased over time. Implications for designing adaptive GBLEs fromlearners cognitive and metacognitive strategy use are discussed.

Using Natural Language Processing Models to Evaluate STEM Book Coherence

Learning in the STEM disciplines depends on high-quality STEM books, but choosing a textbook can be difficult inthe absence of objective measures of text quality. Here we compared two natural language processing approaches forevaluating text cohesion. In Coh-Metrix (Graesser et al. 2004), text cohesion is indicated by the mean cosine value of theall possible pairs of sentence vectors, with sentence vectors based on LSA. We introduce a new method for measuring textcoherence based on the deep learning language model RoBERTa (Liu et al., 2019). In this new approach, coherence ismeasured by determining the average predictability of all of the words in the text, with word predictability a function ofeach words linguistic context. Coherence as measured by RoBERTa more closely matched the coherence ratings of humanjudges than did Coh-Metrix. Implications for the assessment and categorization of STEM books are discussed.

Children’s attribution of disfluency to different sources

Disfluency in speech leads listeners, even two-year-oldchildren, to expect the speaker to refer to novel and discourse-new objects. Previous evidence suggests this link betweendisfluency and discourse novelty is not driven simply bytracking of co-occurrence statistics connecting disfluency withreference to a new object, but also by integrating extra-linguistic information about the speaker’s perspective. Weasked whether children can attribute a speaker’s disfluency todifferent sources – language planning difficulty vs. distractionfrom the conversation. We tested children’s processing ofdisfluency when interacting with an engaged versus adistracted speaker. When the engaged speaker was disfluent,children looked more at a novel and discourse-new image thanat a familiar and just-named image, consistent with the existingliterature. This disfluency effect was attenuated when thespeaker was distracted, suggesting that four-year-old childrencan flexibly attribute a speaker’s disfluency to different sourcesin online interpretation of disfluent speech.

Relational reasoning and generalization using non-symbolic neural networks

Humans have a remarkable capacity to reason about abstract relational structures, an ability that may support some ofthe most impressive, human-unique cognitive feats. Equality (or identity) reasoning has been a key case study of abstractrelational reasoning. This paper revisits the question of whether equality can be learned in non-symbolic neural networks.We find that simple neural networks are able to learn basic equality with relatively little training data. In a second casestudy, we show that sequential equality problems (learning ABA sequences) can be solved with only positive traininginstances. Finally, we consider a more complex, hierarchical equality problem, and find that this task can be solved witheither avast amount of training data or pre-training on basic equality. Overall, these findings indicate that neural modelsare able to solve equality-based reasoning tasks, suggesting that essential aspects of symbolic reasoning can emerge fromdata-driven,non-symbolic learning processes.

Can Two 1/2- and 3 1/2 -year-old Children Learn Verbs Even when IrrelevantEvents are Present?

Children learning verbs benefit from seeing multiple events. Study 1 asks whether children can learn verbs when irrelevantevents are present, as is common in everyday contexts. Two- and 3-year-olds saw events in one of three experimentalconditions or one of two control conditions. They successfully extended the verbs only in the experimental conditions.Three-year-olds were more successful than were 2-year-olds, though the younger children could extend verbs. In Study 2,children saw similar events while an eye tracker tracked visual attention to events. Over trials, children looked longer atrelevant than irrelevant events, and maintained their looking to relevant events while increasing their looking to distractorevents. Two-year-olds performed at chance, but 3-year-olds extended the verbs. Together, these results show children canignore irrelevant events and extend new verbs by 3 years. Results reveal mechanisms for learning in everyday contextswhen verbs are heard in varied situations over time.

Investigating the Behavior of Malicious Actors Through the Game of Mafia

In deception games, deceivers must find ways to draw in unknowing bystanders, and bystanders must develop strategiesfor detecting falsehoods. What are the strategies that people use in these roles, and can computer systems also detect thesebehaviors? We address this question through text-based games of Mafia, wherein players are assigned to deceptive roles(mafia) or roles incentivizing detecting deception (bystanders). We find that participants adopt sophisticated role-basedstrategies, wherein the mafia, who are outnumbered but know the identities of all players, act carefully to secure the votesof the bystanders by speaking more even as verbose speakers tended to be eliminated. These role-based behaviors weredistinct enough that a computational classifier could distinguish between mafia and bystanders with 70.3% accuracy andoutperform human players. Understanding the systematic features defining honest and deceptive players advances ourability to automatically detect online deceit and grasp group dynamics in real-world collaboration.

Active Vision in the Perception of Actions: An Eye Tracking Study in Naturalistic Contexts

Infants’ ability to attend actively and selectively to naturalistic stimuli is critical to early learning. Most studies on infant visual attention use screen-based paradigms wherein infants view stimuli on computer screens. Little is known about how infants observe others’ activities in everyday contexts. Using head- mounted eye-tracking, this study examined how infants distributed attention when observing their parents perform an everyday task – making peanut-butter and jelly sandwiches – in a home-like environment. Infant observers attended to parents’ activities less than adult observers in the same situation. However, when infants were engaged in action observation, their gaze patterns were distributed on task- relevant objects similarly to adult observers, suggesting they actively obtained rich visual input in this free-viewing situation. Moreover, infant-parent dyads coordinated visual attention during the food preparation task in similar ways as observed in other everyday tasks, such as toy play, suggesting sensorimotor processes play a critical role in coordinated attention.

Human-like Learning Framework forFrequency-Skewed Multi-level Classification

Contemporary deep neural network based classification sys-tems are typically designed to learn information at a singlelevel of granularity from datasets in which all items occur withequal frequency. Humans, on the other hand, acquire informa-tion at several different levels of granularity from experiencesthat contain some items more frequently than others. This al-lows us to learn and differentiate frequent items better fromother items. We investigate the consequence of learning froma natural frequency/multi-level dataset in a deep neural net-work designed to model the human neocortex, complementedin some simulations with a replay buffer, playing the role ofthe human hippocampus. The NC network, when trained onits own, is able to learn more frequent items relatively quicklyand differentiate them better from other items, as human learn-ers do. However, the network’s performance on infrequentand unseen examples pays a price in generalization perfor-mance compared to a standard training regime. The replaybuffer serves to ameliorate these deficiencies, and we intro-duce a computationally and psychologically motivated replayweighting scheme that performs better than two alternatives.

Modeling Human Cognitive Flexibility with Extemporaneous Networks

Research in cognitive science and machine learning suggests that learning systems can use small subsets of valuabletraining items in order to quickly learn to achieve good task performance. We hypothesize that people often use smallsubsets of stored exemplars to quickly train new neural networks, called extemporaneous networks, when faced with tasksfor which they do not currently have dedicated networks. We explore this hypothesis using participants’ responses in abehavioral experiment to identify easy versus difficult training items. We find that a network confidence measure indicatesa network trained with a small set of good items provides the best account of participants’ reaction times. Furthermore,computer simulations demonstrate that learning systems can achieve good performance when trained with small sets ofeasy exemplars. Our results indicate that humans may complete tasks using extemporaneously-created networks trainedinternally on small datasets.

Examining Developmental Change in Children’s Information Use

Adults tend to make biased inferences when they are givenbase-rates that conflict with individuating information (i.e., apersonality description). More recent work has shown thatchildren rely on individuating information by the age of 6,though 4-year-olds rely more on numerical information,arguably providing the more normative response (Gualtieri &Denison, 2018). In two experiments (N = 80 per experiment),we explored age differences in 4- and 6-year-old children’sability to integrate base-rate and individuating information bymanipulating the strength of the information provided. Four-year-olds’ responses reflected more base-rate use, regardless ofthe strength of the individuating information. Six-year-oldsweighed the information at hand, showing a general preferencefor the individuating information but relying more on the base-rates when the individuating information was less informative.Though younger preschoolers may overuse base-rateinformation, with development there is an increased sensitivitytoward individuating information and weighing information.

The Semantic Network: Uncovering The Mechanisms that Build Organized WordKnowledge in Development

Language is rich in statistical regularities that capture meaningful, semantic links between words crucial for languagefluency. Words that can be combined to express meaningful ideas (e.g., drink-soda) reliably directly co-occur together,and words similar in meaning share patterns of co-occurrence (e.g. soda and milk share co-occurrence with drink). Here,we investigate whether children (4-year-olds) and adults can capitalize on these regularities to form new semantic linksbetween new and familiar words. Participants hear sentences in which new words directly co-occur or share co-occurrencewith familiar words. We then assess the formation of corresponding semantic links using an implicit, gaze-based measureand an explicit labeling measure. Results suggest that new semantic links form only from direct co-occurrence in chil-dren, and from both direct and shared co-occurrence in adults. This research is therefore uncovering the development ofmechanisms for building organized word knowledge from mere exposure to language.

Extracting low-dimensional psychologicalrepresentations from convolutional neural networks

Deep neural networks are increasingly being used in cognitivemodeling as a means of deriving representations for complexstimuli such as images. While the predictive power of thesenetworks is high, it is often not clear whether they also offeruseful explanations of the task at hand. Convolutional neuralnetwork representations have been shown to be predictive ofhuman similarity judgments for images after appropriate adap-tation. However, these high-dimensional representations aredifficult to interpret. Here we present a method for reducingthese representations to a low-dimensional space which is stillpredictive of similarity judgments. We show that these low-dimensional representations also provide insightful explana-tions of factors underlying human similarity judgments.

A Novel Quantum Approach to the Dynamics of Decision Making

We present a new quantum-markovian model of two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) decision-making. We treatthe decision-making process as an accumulation of evidencebetween two competing alternatives, analogous to the drift dif-fusion model (DDM), in which the stimulus acts as a gener-ative process, emitting bits of information that are treated asquantum particles. The particles are acted on by a landscapedetermined by the agent’s experience with the task or stimu-lus, signal strength, and allocated cognitive control. We de-rive closed form expressions for success rates under both theinterrogation and free response paradigms. Under the free re-sponse paradigm, we show that this model reduces to a Markovprocess with closed form response time (RT) distributions thattake the form of inverse gaussians (IGs) with periodic noisecharacteristic to the task set. In the limit of long RT, the RTdistributions become smooth, recovering true IG distributionsanalogous to the standard DDM.

A memory-augmented neural network model of abstract sequential reasoning

A key aspect of human reasoning is the ability to recognize abstract patterns in sequential data and then use those patternsto make novel inferences. Capturing this capacity for abstract reasoning is a major challenge for neural network modelsof human cognition. We present a recurrent neural network model of abstract sequential reasoning that is augmented witha form of episodic memory. This memory system enables the network to accomplish a form of variable-binding that haslong been considered an important component of abstract reasoning. We evaluate the model using visually grounded,abstract sequential reasoning and pattern completion tasks, including a task based on relations commonly found in RavensProgressive Matrices.

The effects of mindfulness meditation and relaxation on brain activity

Meditation aims to improve ones psychological capacities by encouraging a calm and focused mind. Studies have ob-served positive benefits of meditation on health and cognition, such as reduced anxiety and enhanced executive control.Meditation has even been shown to alter brain structure and function. These benefits are mainly observed in long-termmeditators, with few studies examining the effects of short-term meditation. The current study investigated whether thereare immediate benefits of meditation. Electroencephalography was recorded while cognitive tasks were completed, wealso collected subjective well-being measures before and after exposure to either a brief meditation or a relaxation story.Post-intervention reaction time was shorter in meditators compared to the relaxation story. Both groups exhibited increasedwell-being, smaller N2s, and larger P3bs post-intervention. These results suggest that while mindfulness meditation mayimprove conflict monitoring, both interventions appear to improve well-being. Overall, there may be immediate benefitsof meditation for even novice meditators.

Chaining and historical adjective extension

A hallmark of natural language is the innovative reuse of ex-isting words. We examine how adjectives extend over timeto describe nouns and form previously unattested adjective-noun pairings. Our approach is based on the idea of chainingthat postulates word meaning to extend by linking novel ref-erents to existing ones that are close in semantic space. Wetest this proposal by exploring a set of models that learn toinfer adjective-noun pairings from historical text corpora fora period of 150 years. Our findings across three diverse setsof adjectives support a chaining mechanism that is sensitiveto semantic neighbourhood density, best captured by an exem-plar model of category extension. This work sheds light on thegenerative cognitive mechanisms of word usage extension.

Phonemic learning based on articulatory-acoustic speech representations

Infants learn to imitate and recognize words at an early age,but phonemic awareness develops at a later age, guided byacquisition of literacy for example. We investigate ahypothesis that speech representations in the brain are formedpartly due to articulatory-acoustic learning, and theserepresentations may be used as a basis when learning anadditional mapping to phonemes. We train a convolutionalrecurrent neural network, having an articulatory branch and aphonemic branch for multitask learning. When trained withreal conversational speech and aligned synthesizedarticulation, it is shown that the use of the articulatoryrepresentation boosts phoneme recognition accuracy, whenthe first convolutional layers are shared between the twobranches. It is hypothesized that representations involved inspeech perception formed in the brain during childhood maybe partly based on articulatory learning, and an additionalmapping from these low-level speech representations tophonemes has to be learned.

Quality Engineering in the Development of an Intelligent Agent

Our laboratory is involved in the development of an intelligent agent that operates a remotely piloted aircraft with twohuman teammates that communicate using text chat. The task is well-defined, but there are potentially numerous andunpredictable inputs during varied 40 minute missions. To assure reliability of agent behavior, we must run a largenumber of missions and analyze the behavior of the agent at milliseconds resolution. To support this requirement, we havedeveloped 1) a scripting language and control system that drives a mission with simulated teammates and environmentalevents, 2) scripted missions using actual chat input from a previous study, 3) output files for each mission that trace agentactions, situation state, and program events, and 4) scripts that analyze the output files based on performance heuristicsand differences from known-good output. This framework allows us to verify complex agent behavior as developmentprogresses.

Spatial alignment supports comparison of life science visuals for 7th graders

Visual comparisons are ubiquitous in STEM education. We suggest that visual comparisons are carried out by a structuralalignment process that draws correspondences between analogs based on relational structure (Sagi, Gentner, & Lovett,2012). The spatial arrangement of images can influence visual comparisons by increasing or decreasing competitionfrom incorrect correspondences (Matlen, Gentner, & Franconeri, 2020). The present study tested whether this could beleveraged to help children compare complex STEM-related images. Seventh graders were shown drawings of skeletonscontaining an anomalous bone, either solo or paired with a correct standard. Children were more accurate at finding theanomaly when given a correct standard to compare to. On especially difficult trials in which skeletons were shown innon-canonical orientations (e.g., a cow oriented vertically), performance was enhanced when the spatial placement of thetwo skeletons was direct, minimizing competing correspondences. Thus, direct placement may help students comparecomplex unfamiliar images.

Can audio-visual integration, adaptive learning, and explicit feedback improve theperception of noisy speech?

The perception of degraded speech input is essential in everyday life and is a major challenge in a variety of clinicalsettings, including for cochlear implant users. We investigated English speakers perception of noisy speech via an audio-visual lexical decision paradigm that modulated cross-modal integration, adaptive modulation of task difficulty, and ex-plicit feedback on response accuracy. We then tested whether proficiency with this task transferred to the perception ofnoisy audio stimuli in a post test. Although we observed a processing advantage for bimodal stimuli during training,particularly in the adaptive training condition, we did not observe any benefit from these conditions in the post test, nor abenefit associated with providing explicit feedback. These results are discussed in relation to other studies of audio-visualintegration and learning to perceive noisy speech, which may have observed different results due to more extensive trainingand different baseline proficiency levels.

Distributional Statistical Learning: How and How Well Can It Be Measured?

Individuals are readily able to extract and encode statistical information from their environment (or statistical learning). However, the bulk of the literature has primarily focused on conditional statistical learning (i.e. the ability to learn joint and conditional relationships between stimuli), and has largely neglected distributional statistical learning (i.e. the ability to learn the frequency and variability of distributions). In this paper, we investigate how and how well distributional learning can be measured by exploring the relationship between and psychometric properties of two measures: discrimination judgements and frequency estimates. Reliable performance was observed in both measures across two different distributional learning tasks (natural and artificial). Discrimination judgements and frequency estimates also significantly correlated with one another in both tasks, and performance on all tasks accounted for the majority of variance across tasks (55%). These results suggest that distributional learning can be measured reliably, and may tap into both the ability to discriminate between relative frequencies and to explicitly estimate them.

Individual differences in metacognitive ability of grandiose and vulnerablenarcissists

Understanding individual differences in metacognitive ability may provide novel insights into how we think about ourown thinking. Past research has revealed individual differences in the extent to which grandiose and vulnerable narcissistsare metacognitively miscalibrated with respect to cognitive ability (Littrell, Fugelsang, & Risko, 2019). Building off ofthis work, we present a study examining the relations between trait narcissism across different cognitive tasks (e.g., verbalability, memory) and measures of metacognitive ability (e.g., bias, relative accuracy). Results indicate that while grandioseand vulnerable narcissists did not differ with respect to performance on cognitive tasks, they did significantly differ in theirperformance on certain metacognitive metrics. These results contribute to both our understanding of narcissism, individualdifferences in metacognitive ability, and the relation between different measures of metacognitive ability.

The influence of mismatched network topologies on learning across levels of thelanguage hierarchy

We test here the two-way influence of word and sentence level network topologies on learning. Participants viewed a self-paced stream of ”letters” in the form of novel glyphs. Glyphs were shown individually with words separated by spacesand sentences denoted with a prompt. In one condition, streams were generated via a walk along a scale-free graph at bothlevels, with nodes corresponding to either single glyphs (word level) or single words (sentence level). In a mismatchedcondition, sentences were generated from a graph with a scale-free degree distribution and words were instead generatedfrom a random graph. After exposure to the streams, participants completed familiarity judgments on words and sentences.Interestingly, performance on the word test was enhanced for participants exposed to mismatched topologies. Future workwill tease apart whether: (1) contrasting topologies boost learning; or (2) words that do not display scale-free degreedistribution are inherently easier to learn.

A FIRST: Arabic-English biliterates demonstrate the SNARC effect

The SNARC effect is demonstrated in number judgment tasks when subjects are faster to respond to higher values withresponses made on the right and to lower values with responses made on the left. This effect has been found to beimpervious to handedness but works best for single digit values. Researchers speculate the reason for this robust effectto be a Mental Number Line (MNL) from which numbers, proceeding from 0-9 are oriented in a horizontal fashion fromleft to right. This follows when people consistently use text that proceeds from left to right, but for 1 or 2 billion ofpeople worldwide, text orientation proceeds right to left or top to bottom. The current experiments investigated whetherthe SNARC effect would be found among Egyptian Arabic-English biliterates who are highly proficient in both languagesand for whom reading and writing proceed from right-to-left, except for the numbering system. To our knowledge we havefound the first ever demonstration of the typical SNARC effect among this population.

Leveraging Unstructured Statistical Knowledge in aProbabilistic Language of Thought

One hallmark of human reasoning is that we can bring to beara diverse web of common-sense knowledge in any situation.The vastness of our knowledge poses a challenge for the prac-tical implementation of reasoning systems as well as for ourcognitive theories – how do people represent their common-sense knowledge? On the one hand, our best models of so-phisticated reasoning are top-down, making use primarily ofsymbolically-encoded knowledge. On the other, much of ourunderstanding of the statistical properties of our environmentmay arise in a bottom-up fashion, for example through asso-ciationist learning mechanisms. Indeed, recent advances in AIhave enabled the development of billion-parameter languagemodels that can scour for patterns in gigabytes of text from theweb, picking up a surprising amount of common-sense knowl-edge along the way—but they fail to learn the structure of co-herent reasoning. We propose combining these approaches, byem- bedding language-model-backed primitives into a state-of-the-art probabilistic programming language (PPL). On twoopen-ended reasoning tasks, we show that our PPL modelswith neural knowledge components characterize the distribu-tion of human responses more accurately than the neural lan-guage models alone, raising interesting questions about howpeople might use language as an interface to common-senseknowledge, and suggesting that building probabilistic modelswith neural language-model components may be a promisingapproach for more human-like AI.

Extending the Rogers and McClelland Model of Semantic Cognition (2003) towork with Raw Pixel Information

Understanding how we acquire semantic knowledge is a central topic in cognitive science. In a now classic paper, Rogersand McClelland (2003) explored how a parallel distributed processing (PDF) model could recreate several important phe-nomena in semantic memory including how concepts are acquired, lost due to semantic dementia, and become organizedhierarchically. One well known limitation of this model, which was acknowledge by the original authors, is that thefeatures used in the model were largely hand coded. In this project we revisit this classic PDP account in light of mod-ern advances in neural network techniques. In particular, we show that we can recreate several of the predictions of theRogers and McClelland (2003) model in a network trained directly on raw pixel information from category exemplars.These results add realism to the original model while also showing how the principles of the model generalize to higherdimensional input spaces.

Coloring Outside the Lines:Error Patterns in Children’s Acquisition of Color Terms

A key challenge for children in language acquisition is to learnthe mapping of words to mental categories, since this mappingvaries greatly from language to language. The errors childrenmake in this process are very informative regarding the devel-opment of lexical semantic categories; in particular, how chil-dren overextend a word to an inappropriate exemplar providesa window onto the mechanisms that underlie their categoriza-tion processes. We perform a large-scale quantitative analysisof the detailed patterns of children’s errors in the domain ofcolor, finding evidence that these error patterns are driven byan interaction between domain general principles of catego-rization, and children’s developing knowledge of the seman-tics of color. Our results suggest that, while domain generalprocesses play a role throughout development, their influencevaries across ages according to their use of domain specific(conceptual) knowledge, which gradually increases over time.

Human-Generated Explanations of Inferences in Bayesian Networks: A CaseStudy

As AI systems come to permeate human society, there is an increasing need for such systems to explain their actions,conclusions, or decisions. This is presently fuelling a surge in interest in machine-generated explanation. However,there are not only technical challenges to be met here; there is also considerable uncertainty about what suitable targetexplanations should look like. In this paper, we describe a case study which makes a start at bridging between machinereasoning, and the philosophical and psychological literatures on what counts as good reasoning by eliciting explanationsfrom human experts. The work illustrates how concrete cases rapidly move discussion beyond abstract considerationsof explanatory virtues toward specific targets more suitable for emulation by machines. On the one hand, it highlightsthe limitations of present algorithms for generating explanations from Bayesian networks. At the same time, however, itprovides concrete direction for future algorithm construction.

Attentional Competition in Genuine Classrooms: Analysis of the Classroom VisualEnvironment

Prior research in laboratory settings suggests highly decoratedlearning environments reduce attention to instructional taskshampering learning. However, systematic research examininghow the visual environment relates to children’s on-taskbehavior in genuine learning environments is more rare. Thus,it is unknown whether prior laboratory findings can beextended to genuine classrooms and what specific aspects ofthe visual environment might pose a challenge for children’sattention regulation and learning. This study aims to (1)provide a nuanced examination of specific elements of theclassroom visual environment (e.g., visual noise, quantity ofposters, color darkness, color variability, adherence to generaldesign principles) by analyzing panoramic photographs of 58classrooms, and (2) investigate whether specific elements ofthe visual environment are related to rates of on-taskbehavior. Results indicate on-task behavior declined inclassrooms containing greater visual noise.

The Development of Creative Search Strategies

What is creativity and how does it develop? Intuitively, it seems that children are often especially creative, but it isdifficult to find measures that are precise and comparable across development. In this study we use a creative foragingtask that involves the exploration of a high-dimensional space. This task precisely measures elements of creativity, whichwe compare between 4- to 8-year-olds and adults. We find that children show exploration-exploitation behavior in theircreative search resembling adults search. However, children are more exploratory in nature - compared to adults, theyspend a higher percentage of their search in exploration mode, and their exploitation phases are less optimal comparedto adults. Moreover, the products of childrens creative search are more often unique, compared to those of adults; andyounger children create more unique shapes than older children. Together, these results support the hypothesis that creativesearch may change across development, both in how the space of possibilities is navigated and what ideas are ultimatelygenerated. These findings inform not only our understanding of why childrens learning may sometimes be superior thanthat of adults, but also may inform our understanding of creativity and the creative process across development.

Simulating Infant Visual Learning by Comparison: An Initial Model

Researchers have recently found that 3-month-old infants are capable of using analogical abstraction to learn the same or different relation, given the right conditions (Anderson et al. 2018). Surprisingly, seeing fewer distinct examples led to more successful learning than seeing more distinct examples. This runs contrary to the prediction of standard learning theories, which hold that a wider range of examples leads to better generalization and transfer, but is compatible with other findings in infant research (Casasola 2005; Maguire et al. 2008). Anderson et al. (2018) propose that this is due to interactions between encoding and analogical learning. This paper explores that proposal through the lens of cognitive simulation, using automatically encoded visual stimuli and a cognitive model of analogical learning. The simulation results are compatible with the original findings, thereby providing evidence for this explanation. The assumptions underlying the simulation are delineated and some alternatives are discussed.

The Rainbow Mnemonic Improves Recall in Preschool Children

Mnemonic devices aid recall. However, little research has explored their use with preschool-aged children. The present studies examined whether a new peg-type mnemonic technique (rainbow mnemonic) could be used to improve memory in preschool children. Item cards, which displayed a picture and its label, were studied alongside colored cards, and this condition was compared to a control condition in which children were left to their own devices to study the item cards. In Experiment 2, the rainbow mnemonic was also compared to a condition in which the children did not have access to the color cues during study or recall. The experiments revealed that the rainbow mnemonic could improve recall for preschool children as compared to control. This study demonstrates the effectiveness of a novel peg-type technique with preschool- aged children.

Pronoun interpretation in the context of dynamic actions: a test of thereinstatement hypothesis

Pronouns (she, they) are semantically underspecified and depend on context for interpretation. One proposal is thatinterpretation occurs by reactivating a pronouns antecedent, consistent with memory reinstatement models. We evaluatethis account using a novel task where the semantics of the antecedent are no longer appropriate after an instruction iscompleted (e.g., Move the house on the left to area 3, where the result is that ANOTHER house is now the leftmost one).If antecedent semantics are activated when subsequently hearing a pronoun (”Now put it”), listeners should experienceconfusion regarding the intended referent. However, measures of (i) the object selected, (ii) mouse-click reaction times,and (iii) eye-movements all demonstrate the pronoun is effortlessly linked to the previously-mentioned object, regardlessof whether antecedent semantics are still relevant. This demonstrates that pronouns have indexical meaning, denoting afocused referent directly, and are not mediated by activating linguistic antecedents in discourse memory.

Radical Embodiment and the Relation Between Individual and Joint Action: ALevel-Neutral Approach

A common assumption in the philosophical literature on joint action is that individual-level action is both ontologicallyand explanatorily prior to collective action: in this view, joint action emerges fromand is therefore best explained in termsofindividual-level mental (intentional, propositional) states. This leads to the awkward position of attributing individual-like minds to groups. But assigning priority to the collective level is equally unsatisfactory. Here I draw from radicalembodied cognitive science to offer a level-neutral alternative. Whether individual or joint, successful action is properlyunderstood as the soft-assembly of a synergistic system, i.e., a higher-order control system exhibiting dimensional com-pression and reciprocal compensation. This level-neutral lens of synergistic dynamics helps elucidate the circular relationbetween individual and collective action: joint action recruits individual-level motor/cognitive mechanisms, yet individual-level mechanisms only emerge through development in social settingsresulting in a nested, self-reinforcing coordinativestructure for action, both individual and collective.

Selective Numeracy: Effects of Numeracy, Popular-Science Reportsand Personal Experience on Data-Based Decision Making

In the current research, we investigated whether numeracy,scientific reports in the popular press, and personal experiencewere associated with people’s data-based decision making.We collected data from English-speaking adult participants(N = 187), residing in the United States and Canada, whowere recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk andcompleted the online study. Results showed that participantswith higher numeracy were more likely to make the correctdata-based decision. However, participants used theirnumeracy selectively. They seemed to use their numeracyskills to confirm their own desire rather than to objectivelyevaluate the data or confirm reported scientific findings. Nosignificant association was found between personalexperience and data-based decision making. Future researchmay examine decision making across other, general-lifedomains to examine the replicability of the current results.

Children’s Expressive and Receptive Knowledge of the English Regular Plural

We investigate the development of children’s early grammat-ical knowledge using the test case of the English regular plu-ral. Previous research points to early generalization, with chil-dren applying an abstract morphological rule to produce novelplurals well before 24 months. At the same time, childrenuse the plural inconsistently with familiar object words, anddemonstrate limited receptive knowledge of the plural in theabsence of supporting linguistic features. In the first studyto test knowledge of the plural within participants using aparadigm matched across comprehension and production, weconduct two experiments with n = 52 24-36-month-olds: aneyetracking task to evaluate what they understand, and a sto-rybook task to test how they use the plural. We manipulateboth novelty (novel vs. familiar object words) and phonolog-ical form (/s/ vs. /z/ plurals). We find strong, age-related ev-idence of productive knowledge of the plural in an expressivetask, but do not find evidence of receptive knowledge in thesesame children.

Generalizations about the functions of agents

Studies show that people begin to associate objects with functions early in development (Atran, 1995; Carey, 1985; Csibra& Gergely, 1998; Keil, 1992). They can describe generalizations about the functions of objects by producing teleologicalgeneric language, i.e., statements that express generalities about the purposes of objects. A recent study shows thatpeople accept teleological generics about body parts such as eyes are for seeing but reject statements such as eyes are forblinking. Nevertheless, little is known about whether people associate living, volitional agents with functions. In a seriesof experiments, we show that they do: they accept statements of the form ”horses are for riding” but not ”horses are forneighing”. The studies show further that people appear to have normative expectations about the functions of agents, e.g.,they accept statements such as ”all normal horses are for riding” and ”horses are supposed to be for riding”. The resultcorroborates Korman and Khemlani’s (2018, 2020) proposal that people mentally represent principled connections, i.e.,privileged conceptual links, between kinds and their functional properties.

An investigation of the origin of logical quantification: infants and adultsrepresentations of collective and distributive actions in complex visual scenes

The human mind can compress visual experiences via universal quantification, expressed with the words All and Each.We tested adults and infants representations underlying the tracking of collectively-exhaustive actions or distributively-exhaustive actions. In Experiment 1, adults spontaneously used the word All to describe movies where agents all pursueda single ball together and Each for those where each agent chased its own ball. Crucially, the use of Each, but not of All,significantly decreased when there were more than 3 chasers, suggesting that Each piggybacked on the representation ofdiscrete individuals, while All on the representation of a single collective event. In Experiment 2, infants habituated to theAll movies successfully dishabituated to the Each movies and vice versa, when the chasers were 3. These findings begin tosuggest that the representations of collectively-exhaustive and distributively-exhaustive actions that connect with naturallanguage quantifiers are in place early in life.

Disentangling Generativity in Visual Cognition

Human knowledge is generative: from everyday learning people extract latent features that can recombine to producenew imagined forms. This ability is critical to cognition, but its computational bases remain elusive. Recent researchwith -regularized Variational Autoencoders (-VAE) suggests that generativity in visual cognition may depend on learningdisentangled (localist) feature representations. We tested this proposal by training -VAEs and standard autoencoders toreconstruct bitmaps showing a single object varying in shape, size, location, and color, and manipulating hyperparame-ters to produce differentially-entangled feature representations. These models showed variable generativity, with somestandard autoencoders capable of near-perfect reconstruction of 43 trillion images after training on just 2000. However,constrained -VAEs were unable to reconstruct images reflecting feature combinations which were systematically withheldduring training (e.g. all blue circles). Thus, deep auto-encoders may provide a promising tool for understanding visualgenerativity and potentially other aspects of visual cognition.

Humans measure algorithmic complexity to guide engagement with eventsequences

The criteria for guiding endogenous attention are largely unknown. A prominent view is that humans preferentiallyengage with information of intermediate complexity, and minimize engagement with too simple or too complex events.Here, we operationalize the notions of engagement and complexity to test this hypothesis. We asked participants toengage with differentially complex sequences of symbols shown one-by-one and disengage when they 1) could predictthe next element of the sequence, or 2) felt the sequence was unpredictable. We define sequence complexity as a functionof the probability of obtaining that sequence from a particular Hidden Markov Model. This extends previous measuresof complexity to respect sequential structure and closely relates to the algorithmic complexity of sequence-generatingprograms. We construct different measures using this operationalization of sequence complexity to predict the probabilityof disengagement at each event. We assess under which definitions intermediate complexity is preferred.

Learners sacrifice robust communication as a result of a social bias

Languages are subject to many competing pressures, whichoriginate in individual-level learning and communication bi-ases and in social biases reflecting community-level dynamics.Recent work suggests that certain aspects of language struc-ture, such as the cross-linguistic trade-off between case andconstituent-order flexibility, originate in learners’ biases forefficient communication: Learners drop redundant case but re-tain informative case in production. Social biases can lead toretention of redundant case, resulting in systems that requiremore effort to produce. It is not clear, however, whether socialbiases can influence the use of informative cues. We tested thisby exposing participants to a language with uninformative con-stituent order and two dialects, only one of which employedcase. We manipulated the presence of social biases for andagainst the case dialect. Learners biased towards the no-casedialect dropped informative case without compensating for theresulting message uncertainty. Case was retained in all otherconditions.

Generalization of Novel Object Names in Comparison Contexts in a yes-no paradigm by young children. When the rate of stimulus presentation matters.

A common result in novel word generalization tasks is that comparison settings (i.e., several stimuli introduced simultaneously) favor conceptualization and generalization. We hypothesized that typical comparison forced-choice designs between a lure and a target conceptual dimension might have constrained children’s choices. Here we used a “yes-no” free choice design with 3- and 4-year-old children, and manipulated the presentation mode of the stimuli, either simultaneous or sequential. We manipulated the semantic distance between training and transfer items. Results showed that simultaneous, rather than sequential, presentations in the transfer phase led to more taxonomic generalizations in four- year olds. Results are discussed in terms of the constraints that both types of presentation bring into the task.

Why blueberries are blue: intuitions about color labels among congenitally blindand sighted adults

Why do we describe blueberries as blue as opposed to white (their inside color)? People might label object colors entirelyaccording to what they see most frequently. We hypothesized instead that labeling takes into account typical viewingconditions (outside/daytime) and object causal history (colors relationship to function; Cohen, 2004). We further predictedthat these intuitions develop independently of visual experience. Sighted (n=15) and congenitally blind (n=20) participantschose one of two color labels for novel objects, described as having different colors (or textures) on the inside/outsideor during daylight/nighttime. On some day/night trials, objects had nighttime-intended functions. Sighted and blindindividuals alike chose observer-centric outside and day colors by default, but switched to nighttime colors when objectshad nighttime functions. First-person visual experience is not required for color-labeling to take into account observercharacteristics and object causal history.

Learning word-referent mappings and concepts from raw inputs

How do children learn correspondences between the language and the world from noisy, ambiguous, naturalistic input?One hypothesis is via cross-situational learning: tracking words and their possible referents across multiple situationsallows learners to disambiguate correct word-referent mappings (Yu and Smith, 2007). While previous models of cross-situational word learning operate on highly simplified representations, recent advances in multimodal learning have shownpromise as richer models of cross-situational word learning to enable learning the meanings of words from raw inputs.Here, we present a neural network model of cross-situational word learning that leverages some of these ideas and examineits ability to account for a variety of empirical phenomena from the word learning literature.

Developmental Changes in Children’s Categorization of Facial Cues of Emotion

How do children learn to categorize the facial configurations classically believed to represent basic emotions? Many stud-ies have examined when children are able to perceptually discriminate between emotional facial expressions and whenchildren are able to verbally label these expressions. However, while these studies provide important information aboutthe timeline of emotional development, they give less information about the nature of childrens category representationsfor different facial configurations. For instance, emotion concepts may emerge from childrens perceptions of facial con-figurations along the dimensions of valence and arousal. To evaluate how 3- to 7-year-old children categorize emotionconcepts, we had them sort facial configurations on a grid based on whether the people were feeling the same kind ofthing. We found that while both children and adults consistently sorted faces according to the dimensions of valence andarousal, sorting faces using discrete emotion categories emerged only gradually across development, with children notdemonstrating consistent use of emotion categories until approximately 5- to 6- years of age.

Monolingual and Bilingual Toddlers’ Reliance on the Mutual Exclusivity Principle and Statistics to Learn Colour Labels

Monolingual toddlers reportedly rely more heavily on the Mutual Exclusivity Principle (MEP) than their age-matched bilingual counterparts when learning new words. Here, we re- visit this issue by testing monolingual and bilingual 24-month- olds’ reliance on the MEP to learn novel colour labels across multiple labelling instances, where cross-situational statistics link a particular label to a particular colour – but not a particular object. In addition, we ask whether the presentation of atypically-coloured objects (e.g., turquoise dogs) may have influenced how readily toddlers attached novel labels to colour terms rather than objects. Thus far, our results demonstrate that monolingual and bilingual toddlers are equally successful in learning colour labels when taught with atypically-coloured objects. However, only bilingual children are able to learn colour labels taught with typically-coloured objects. We conclude that researchers need to carefully consider the richness and statistical input in children’s learning environments to better understand development in diverse language settings.

When do labels facilitate category learning in adults? The role of visual categorystructure

Adults category learning is accelerated by redundant verbal labels (Lupyan et al., 2007). However, it is an open questionhow category representations are affected by labeling. Here, we presented subjects with a learning task that involvedseparating sine wave gratings of differing spatial frequency and orientation into two categories. Categories of easy, mediumand difficult separability were constructed. Participants (N=128) either received only feedback sounds during training, orheard verbal labels in addition. Growth curve analysis (Mirman, 2014) was used, fitting 2nd order polynomials to the dataacross the learning phase. In addition to main effects of difficulty on intercepts and the linear time term, the best-fittingmodel showed an effect of labeling on the linear time term, with steeper learning curves in conditions with labeling. Therewas no interaction of labeling and difficulty, indicating that the impact of labeling is similar across the types of categoriesused here.

What matters? The effect of individual political ideology on spoken genderstereotype comprehension

When people hear ’The babysitter/ put on a TV show/ for the kids/ because he/ needed to use/ the washroom’, the maleidentity of the subject clashes with the stereotypical expectation of babysitters as female, rendering the pronoun he moredifficult to process than she. We asked whether participants political views would modulate listening times to pronounscongruent/incongruent with stereotyped role nouns in spoken sentences.74 English speaking participants listened to sentences with female/male stereotypes in segments and pushed the spacebarto proceed; these reaction times were recorded. Correlating the results with scores from a Political Ideology questionnaireusing Generalized Additive Models, we found slower reaction times with incongruent pronouns on the segment followingthe pronoun (p¡.005). More interestingly, we found an interaction between participants political ideology scores andpronoun congruence on this segment: participants who were higher in Conservatism showed longer reaction times toincongruent pronouns (p¡ .0001).

The impact of semantic versus perceptual attention on memory representation

Encoding new information in relation to existing knowledgebenefits learning. However, integration into existingknowledge might also lead to false memories for similar—butnever-studied—information. Here, we asked whether certainattentional encoding states promote the integration of newinformation into prior knowledge, thereby enhancing memoryand elevating false alarms. We manipulated participants’attention to semantic versus perceptual features by cueing themto alternately make a judgment about the story (semantic) orartistic style (perceptual) of a series of pictures. We then usedan old/new recognition test—which included new illustrationsdepicting studied stories or artistic styles (lures)—to assesswhether story attention increased false alarms to story lures,representing integration into story knowledge. We found thatsemantic attention benefited memory. However, whileintegration into prior semantic knowledge was high overall, itwas not impacted by attention. These findings suggest thatwhile semantic attention improves memory, it does not do soby boosting integration of new memories into existingknowledge structures.

Abstract Words as Emotion Buffers: Affect Labeling and Distress Reduction

Putting feelings into words can dampen emotions, reducing the distress elicited by aversive stimuli. Across two experi-ments, we explored whether the effectiveness of such affect labeling depends on the concreteness of the label. Whereasconcrete labels (e.g., blood) may amplify negative emotions via perceptual reactivation, more abstract labels (e.g., danger)may distance the labeler from the source of emotional distress, thus alleviating negative affect. We investigated this pro-posal by having participants passively watch distressing images or label the same images with either concrete or abstractlabels. We found that abstract labels yielded a greater reduction in participants self-reported distress (compared to passivewatching) than concrete labels. These results suggest that not all labels are equally effective as emotion buffers: abstractlabels enable us to better separate ourselves from our negative feelings.

Generating new concepts with hybrid neuro-symbolic models

Human conceptual knowledge supports the ability to generatenovel yet highly structured concepts, and the form of this con-ceptual knowledge is of great interest to cognitive scientists.One tradition has emphasized structured knowledge, view-ing concepts as embedded in intuitive theories or organizedin complex symbolic knowledge structures. A second tradi-tion has emphasized statistical knowledge, viewing conceptualknowledge as an emerging from the rich correlational structurecaptured by training neural networks and other statistical mod-els. In this paper, we explore a synthesis of these two traditionsthrough a novel neuro-symbolic model for generating new con-cepts. Using simple visual concepts as a testbed, we bring to-gether neural networks and symbolic probabilistic programsto learn a generative model of novel handwritten characters.Two alternative models are explored with more generic neuralnetwork architectures. We compare each of these three mod-els for their likelihoods on held-out character classes and forthe quality of their productions, finding that our hybrid modellearns the most convincing representation and generalizes fur-ther from the training observations.

Understanding Childrens Productions: Does Experience Play a Role?

Toddlers are notoriously difficult to understand, yet like accented speakers, their productions tend to systematically differfrom adult productions. Thus, we hypothesize that listeners with routine exposure to toddlers (perhaps even toddlersthemselves) should comprehend toddlers best. Three listener groups were tested on their comprehension of toddlersutterances in an eye-tracking study; forty-eight toddlers (Mage= 33 months), sixteen undergraduates with little experiencearound children (Mage= 18 years), and sixteen mothers of young children (Mage=38 years). All listeners looked longerto targets than distractors (p ¡ 0.05), with mothers and undergraduates target fixations significantly greater than toddlers(p ¡ 0.001). Mothers target fixations (78%) did not differ significantly from undergraduates (74%; p = 0.17). Thesepreliminary findings suggest a complex picture regarding the role of experience in comprehending toddlers utterances.Clearly, however, toddlers do not outperform adults in understanding toddlers.

The iconicity of random words

Mounting evidence suggests that people make use of non-arbitrary relationships between word form and meaning (e.g.,rounded vowels and rounded shapes) when determining the meaning of a novel word. Typically, these studies use carefullyselected materials to maximize iconic relationships between word forms and meanings. Can people make use of form-meaning resemblances for randomly selected word-forms? We gave 21 groups of undergraduates 40 randomly generatednonce words and asked them to draw a creature for each word such that a nave viewer could reliably match the creature-drawing back to the word that motivated it. Despite the words being selected randomly and filtering out any reliance onexisting English words, drawings were routinely matched back by nave participants (n=222) at rates well above chance.We discuss possible explanations for what makes certain words fit an especially good fit for certain drawings.

The Adaptive Glasgow Face-Matching Task

Current face-comparison tests use a fixed set of stimuli, such that task difficulty is not tailored to the participant’s abilityto perform face matching, which varies greatly across people. Here, we create an adaptive version of the Glasgow FaceMatching Test (GFMT). To accomplish this, we make use of recent advances in machine learning that can encode pho-tographs into a learned face space and then generate photorealistic morphs that interpolate between mid-level features ofthe depicted individuals. In particular, we first use the StyleGAN neural-network architecture to generate challenging vari-ants of the GFMT. We then use QUEST+, a Bayesian adaptive psychometric testing procedure, to estimate the observer’ssensitivity to appearance changes during face matching. The resulting test, the adaptive GFMT (aGFMT), aims to moreefficiently estimate a participant’s face-matching ability.

Dissociable influences of reward and punishment on adaptive cognitive control

When deciding how to allocate cognitive control to a giventask, people must consider both positive outcomes (e.g.,praise) and negative outcomes (e.g., admonishment). How-ever, it is unclear how these two forms of incentives differen-tially influence the amount and type of cognitive control a per-son chooses to allocate. To address this question, we had par-ticipants perform a self-paced incentivized cognitive controltask, varying the magnitude of reward for a correct responseand punishment for an incorrect response. Formalizing controlallocation as a process of adjusting parameters of a drift diffu-sion model (DDM), we show that participants engaged in dif-ferent strategies in response to variability in reward (adjustingdrift rate) versus punishment (adjusting response threshold).We demonstrate that this divergent set of strategies is optimalfor maximizing reward rate while minimizing effort costs. Fi-nally, we show that these dissociable patterns of behavior en-able us to estimate the motivational salience of positive versusnegative incentives for a given individual.

Embodiment and immersion in cognition-focused virtual environments

Cognitive science has much to contribute in regard to the development of accurate and valid virtual environments wherehumans act as operators. For example, optimal performance for visual-motor tasks may require a strong sense of immersionwith respect to flow and interactivity. The present research examined the relation of presence/absence of operator handsduring simulated flight simulation to a series of key immersion factors (N=47). Furthermore, the impact of levels ofimmersion (using self-report scales) on operator performance were also investigated. Results show that hand presenceaffected both absorption and interactivity. Importantly, operator performance showed greater precision when absorptionand interactivity were rated higher. These findings suggest that the development of virtual environments requiring humanoperators and complex cognitive functions must consider the impact of embodiment and levels of immersion.

Using the TrackIt Task to Measure the Development of Selective Sustained Attention in Children Ages 2-7

The TrackIt task was developed as a measure of selective sustained attention that is developmentally-sensitive and able to partially separate exogenous and endogenous factors affecting attention regulation. However, these predictions have only been investigated within a limited set of parameters and age range (3-5 years). This preregistered study reports a systematic effort to examine performance on TrackIt in an expanded parameter space and age range. This study largely replicated and extended prior findings: across most implementations of the task, we found a medium-to-large effect of age and a small effect of condition. We also found that distractor errors were more likely given Low Exogenous support and in younger children. Contrary to the preregistered hypothesis, younger children did not benefit more from exogenous support than older children. Overall, these results contribute to the body of evidence that selective sustained attention (1) improves with age and (2) is bolstered by exogenous support.

Supplementing problem solving with erroneous examples does not improvelearning from an online fraction tutor

It is established that examples are beneficial for learning, but are certain types of examples more helpful than others?Erroneous examples include errors that students are asked to correct, something that can be helpful in addressing mis-conceptions. One domain that is vulnerable to misconceptions is fraction arithmetic. In the present study, undergraduatestudents solved fraction problems using a tutoring system we designed. Some participants worked with the Erroneous-Example tutor, which supplemented problems with erroneous examples, while other participants worked with a traditionalProblem-Solving tutor that did not include erroneous examples. To evaluate the impact of tutor type on learning andself-efficacy, we analyzed difference scores from pre-test to post-test. While overall participants significantly improvedtheir fraction knowledge and self-efficacy, there was no significant difference between the two groups. Bayesian analysesprovided evidence for the null model, i.e., that erroneous examples were not more beneficial than traditional problemsolving.

Learners’ bias to balance production effort against message uncertainty is independent of their native language

Miniature language learning is gaining increasing popularity to study biases underlying language universals. However, it is unclear whether learning preferences in these studies are influenced by learners’ native language. We ask whether a previously identified bias to balance production effort against message uncertainty holds across speakers of structurally different languages. We expose English (fixed order language without case) and German (flexible order language with case) speakers to miniature languages with optional case and either fixed or flexible constituent order and study their deviations from the input. We find that English and German speakers restructure the input in the same way: They match the input constituent order proportions and use more case in the flexible order language than in the fixed order language, thus following the bias to balance production effort against message uncertainty. Our findings suggest that this bias and its specific realization are independent of learners’ native language.

Optimal nudging

People’s judgments and decisions often deviate from classicalnotions of rationality, incurring costs to both themselves and tosociety. Previous research has proposed that the cost of thesebiases can be reduced by redesigning decision problems basedon theories of human decision making. These modifications—or nudges—can have dramatic results and have been success-fully applied to variety of domains. However, the formal un-derpinning of nudge theory is limited, and it is not always clearwhat the effect of a nudge will be before it is implemented. Asa result, designing nudges can be time consuming and error-prone. In this paper, we propose an automatic method for de-riving optimal nudges. The method is based on a resource-rational model, which assumes that people make decisions ina way that achieves a near-optimal tradeoff between the costand benefits of deliberation. We then frame nudges as modi-fications to the costs of different cognitive operations, encour-aging the cognitively frugal decision maker to consider someproblem features over others. As a proof of concept, we applythe method to the Mouselab process-tracing paradigm, findingthat optimal nudges lead participants to make better decisionswith less cognitive effort.

Childrens use of linguistic and non-linguistic negation in reasoning by thedisjunctive syllogism

Whether logical inference is available without language is highly debated. One such inference is the disjunctive syllogism(A Or B, Not A, Therefore B). Evidence from a search task that required disjunctive reasoning suggests that that thesyllogism is unavailable before age 3 (Mody & Carey, 2016). However, in a replication of the same task using language(i.e., verbal negation), even 2.5-year-olds succeeded (Grigoroglou, et al., 2019). Here we explore the role of languagein childrens logical reasoning. 2.5- to 4-year-olds performed the non-linguistic task, after a short training in reasoningby exclusion. Half of the children received linguistic training (e.g., heard there is no coin in X cup); half received non-linguistic training (i.e., saw that one location was empty). Results show that 2.5- and 3-year-olds were more successful inreasoning with the disjunctive syllogism after the linguistic training. Thus, offering children the premise Not A verballyfacilitated logical reasoning.

Variation in surface features improves recognition of common magnitude relations

An issue in higher-order reasoning is the influence of irrelevant surface (perceptual) features in tasks involving a deep(relational) structure. Many machine learning models use feature vector representations of objects. However, the extent towhich these representations predict or explain human behavior and learning is unclear. A feature vector model facilitatesabstraction and transfer when weights on irrelevant features are minimized and weights on the diagnostic (relational)features are increased. The current study tested whether a feature vector model applies to human behavior in the contextof magnitude relations (line ratio comparison). We systematically varied the degree of surface feature variation whilemaintaining relational structure. We found that, consistent with a feature vector model, participants were more accurateat recognizing common relational structure when surface features differed (t = 4.22, p ¡.001). This approach may bepreferable to a progressive alignment approach to relational magnitude learning.

Increasing Diversity of Contrast Examples Decreases Generalization from aProbabilistic Target Set

Four experiments explored the effect of diversity of contrasting evidence on inductive inferences drawn from a multi-item target. In Experiments 1 and 2, increasing the diversity of a contrast set led to lower generalization of a novelproperty that was probabilistically associated with the target. Further, this effect was not sensitive to weak vs. strongsampling assumptions (Experiment 3). Critically, when the property was universal (all target items shared the feature),increasing contrast diversity did not affect generalization to novel members of the target category (Experiment 4). Post-testquestioning suggested that people believed that the probabilistic property indicated subordinate categories in the target set(in fact, there werent). Such a change in the default-level representationin this case, from basic to subordinatealters theperceived size of the setswith subordinate, there are more items. Differences in default-level may explain these findings.We discuss implications for accounts of inferential reasoning.

Adding biological constraints to deep neural networks reduces their capacity tolearn unstructured data

Deep neural networks (DNNs) are becoming increasingly pop-ular as a model of the human visual system. However, theyshow behaviours that are uncharacteristic of humans, includingthe ability to learn arbitrary data, such as images with pixel val-ues drawn randomly from a Gaussian distribution. We investi-gated whether this behaviour is due to the learning and memorycapacity of DNNs being too high for the training task. We re-duced the capacity of DNNs by incorporating biologically mo-tivated constraints – an information bottleneck, internal noiseand sigmoid activations – in order to diminish the learning ofarbitrary data, without significantly degrading performance onnatural images. Internal noise reliably produced the desiredbehaviour, while a bottleneck had limited impact. Combiningall three constraints yielded an even greater reduction in learn-ing capacity. Furthermore, we tested whether these constraintscontribute to a network’s ability to generalize by helping it de-velop more robust internal representations. However, none ofthe methods could consistently improve generalization.

Consideration of Alternative Outcomes of Psychological Studies: Some Evidencefor Transfer

Scientific thinking relies on consideration of alternative possible outcomes to research. We considered whether 1. en-gaging with psychological research resultssome of which were surprisingin a learning phase transferred to considerationof alternative outcomes for a different set of research studies in a test phase, and 2. whether transfer was heightened bypredicting results before learning the actual outcomes (foresight), as opposed to indicating what one would have predictedafter learning the actual outcomes (hindsight). One indication of transfer would be decreased confidence in the outcomeone believed to be true, but we did not observe this trend. However, we did see evidence of transfer for a subset ofparticipants: No participants in the learning phase provided reasons for alternative outcomes, but a sizable minority ofparticipants, across both hindsight and foresight groups, did so in the test phase. We will discuss what factors distinguishparticipants who showed transfer.

Visual Attention and Real-World Decision Making: Sharing Photos on SocialMedia

The present study examined the effect of visual attention and personality traits on decision-making in digital environ-ments. Fifty-nine individuals were asked how likely they would be to share 40 distinct memes (photos with superimposedcaptions) on social media while their eye movements were tracked. Results showed that the likelihood of sharing memesincreased as fixation duration to the text of the meme increased; conversely, the likelihood of sharing decreased as visualattention to the image of the meme increased. In addition, agreeableness predicted an increased likelihood of sharingmemes. These results indicate that differences in perceptual processing of digital content and specific personality traitsaffect the likelihood that an individual will share said content on social media platforms.

Accurate representation for spatial cognition using grid cells

Spatial cognition relies on an internal map-like representationof space provided by hippocampal place cells, which in turnare thought to rely on grid cells as a basis. Spatial Seman-tic Pointers (SSP) have been introduced as a way to representcontinuous spaces and positions via the activity of a spikingneural network. In this work, we further develop SSP rep-resentation to replicate the firing patterns of grid cells. Thisadds biological realism to the SSP representation and links bi-ological findings with a larger theoretical framework for rep-resenting concepts. Furthermore, replicating grid cell activitywith SSPs results in greater accuracy when constructing placecells.Improved accuracy is a result of grid cells forming the op-timal basis for decoding positions and place cell output. Ourresults have implications for modelling spatial cognition andmore general cognitive representations over continuous vari-ables.

Implicit Structure in Sensory Metaphors of Personality

Across many cultures, similar sensory metaphors are used for similar kinds of personality traits, including words likesweet and bitter, straight and crooked, warm and cold (Asch, 1958). Although such metaphors seem to make sense,our post hoc intuitions may be tainted by confirmation bias. We measured the strength of alignments between each ofa set of nine sensory pairs (e.g., warm/cold) pictured literally, and a set of eight pairs of literal personality concepts(e.g., friendly/aloof) using dual categorization tasks (IATs), and then extracted principal components from these patternsof alignment between sensory and personality concepts across 72 different pairings. The resulting 2D metaphor spaceseemed to reflect something akin to the stereotype content model (Fiske et al., 2002), with axes representing both warmth(PC1: warm/soft) and competence (PC2: bright/high). When we repeated the experiment, with new images and labels,essentially the same structure captured these nine sensory metaphor pairs.

Which Sentence Embeddings and Which LayersEncode Syntactic Structure?

Recent models of language have eliminated syntactic-semanticdividing lines. We explore the psycholinguistic implicationsof this development by comparing different types of sentenceembeddings in their ability to encode syntactic constructions.Our study uses contrasting sentence structures known to causesyntactic priming effects, that is, the tendency in humans to re-peat sentence structures after recent exposure. We comparehow syntactic alternatives are captured by sentence embed-dings produced by a neural language model (BERT) or by thecomposition of word embeddings (BEAGLE, HHM, GloVe).Dative double object vs. prepositional object and active vs.passive sentences are separable in the high-dimensional spaceof the sentence embeddings and can be classified with a highdegree of accuracy. The results lend empirical support to themodern, computational, integrated accounts of semantics andsyntax, and they shed light on the information stored at differ-ent layers in deep language models such as BERT.

Finding probabilistic context-free grammar in Chinese writing system

Writing systems play a very important role in human languages, but the mathematical nature of writing systems remainsunderstudied. Here, we conduct a case study of an open-class writing system Chinese characters, which consists of aset of expandable basic units, in contrast to most other writing systems whose basic units form closed sets, or closed-class systems. We demonstrate that probabilistic context-free grammars underlie the representation of Chinese writing, byformalizing Chinese characters as a grammar with character shapes, as nonterminal rules, and components. as terminalnodes. Rule probabilities are estimated from a character treebank of the most frequent 3500 characters. Exploratoryanalysis reveals Zipfian distributions of both shapes and components. Our experiments also demonstrate that Chinesewriting system shows generative powers similar to PCFG, with 78% of the noncharacters generated from our grammarjudged acceptable, which suggests fundamental differences between open-class and closed-class writing systems.

Dynamic Control Under Changing Goals

Acting effectively in the world requires a representation that can be leveraged to serve one’s goals. One practical reason thatintelligent agents might learn to represent causal structure is that it enables flexible adaptation to a changing environment.For example, understanding how to play a videogame allows one to pursue other goals such as doing as poorly as possibleor only gathering one type of item. Across two experiments that manipulated the expected utility of learning causalstructure, we find that people did not build causal representations in dynamic environments. This conclusion was supportedby behavioral results as well as by participants being better fit by models describing them as utilizing minimally complex,reactive control policies. The results show how despite being incredibly adaptive, people are in fact computationally frugal,minimizing the complexity of their representations and decision policies even in situations that might warrant richer ones.

Certain to be surprised:A preference for novel causal outcomes develops in early childhood

A large literature on the development of causal reasoningcharacterizes early childhood as a period of curiosity,exploration, and experimentation. This suggests that a noveltypreference may be a universal hallmark of early causallearning. Functionally, such a bias might serve to directattention towards new opportunities for knowledge gain. Analternative possibility is that a preference for exploring noveloutcomes develops over time. In three experiments with 2- to5-year-olds, we investigate the developmental trajectory ofchildren’s preference for causal processes that producereliable versus novel outcomes. We find evidence for adevelopmental shift between ages 2 and 3: while two-year-olds trend toward a preference for reliable over noveloutcomes, older children clearly prefer novel ones. Wediscuss possible adaptive reasons for this developmental shift.

Mental Imagery – Eyes Open and Shut

Studies of mental imagery often ask participants to attend to a visual scene at the same time as their mental imagery. Despite the common intuition that imagery and perception interfere (known as the Perky effect), results in such experiments are not typically distinguished from those found when participants engage with mental imagery with their eyes closed. Nevertheless, studies which demonstrate the analog nature of mental images by recording the time taken for participants to scan across images have consistently found quicker scanning speeds when participants have eyes open paying attention to a visual scene as compared to with eyes closed. We show here that these results are due to the external scanning of attention across a visual scene and argue for a reevaluation of the results of such paradigms.

Algebra decoded: individual differences in strategy selection when solving for ’x’

Understanding variables and solving algebraic equations are essential to advanced mathematical thinking. Missing-operand problems (e.g., x + 3 = 5) are solvable via two strategies: 1) pattern-matching, or direct arithmetic fact retrieval(e.g., 2 + 3 = 5), and 2) algebraic symbol-manipulation, or performing the inverse operation (e.g., 5 3 = 2). U.S. undergrad-uates made speeded verifications of arithmetic sentences like 2 + 3 = 5 and 5 3 = 2. They then solved missing-operandproblems like x + 3 = 5. We decoded individual differences in strategy choice by whether speed on missing-operandproblems was better predicted by speed on verifying direct- or inverse-matched arithmetic facts. We found individualdifferences in strategy choice, although these were not significantly associated with mathematical achievement.

Jargon Jinx: An Early Bias Toward Opaque Explanations

As adults we understand that effective teachers cannot rely solely on expertise (content knowledge) in a domain, but thatteachers must also be able to efficiently communicate that knowledge to students (pedagogical skill). In three studies,we demonstrate how children fail to appropriately integrate their intuitions of expertise (Study 1) with those of under-standability (Study 2) to make coherent judgments of teacher quality (Study 3). In the context of repairing an unfamiliarmechanism, adults and children recognize that teachers should provide relevant causal information. However, children (6-and 7-year-olds and 8- and 9-year-olds) fail to acknowledge that, while jargon may indicate expertise, it is inaccessible toa student with no prior knowledge. Our data suggests that children as old as 9 years have immature conceptions of whatconstitutes great teaching. Childrens misconceptions of what characterizes good pedagogy raise questions about studentsattentional allocation in educational contexts and subsequent learning gains.

Hierarchical Inferences Support Systematicity in the Lexicon

Language exhibits striking systematicity in its form-meaningmappings: Similar meanings are assigned similar forms. Herewe study how systematicity relates to another well-studiedphenomenon, linguistic regularization, the removal of unpre-dictable variation in linguistic variants. Systematicity is ulti-mately a property of classes of form-meaning mappings, eachmember of which can be acted upon independently by linguis-tic regularization. Both are supported by a cognitive bias forsimplicity, but this leaves open the question of how they inter-act to structure the lexicon. Using data from a recent artificialgesture learning experiment by Verhoef, Padden, and Kirby(2016), we formalize cognitive biases at the item level and thelanguage level as inductive biases in a hierarchical Bayesianmodel. Simulated data from models that lack either one ofthose biases show how both are necessary to capture subjects’systematicity preferences. Our results bring conceptual clar-ity about the relationship between regularization and system-aticity and promote a multi-level approach to cognitive biasesin artificial language learning and language evolution.

Leveraging Machine Learning to Automatically Derive Robust Planning Strategiesfrom Biased Models of the Environment

Teaching clever heuristics is a promising approach to improvedecision-making. We can leverage machine learning to dis-cover clever strategies automatically. Current methods requirean accurate model of the decision problems people face inreal life. But most models are misspecified because of lim-ited information and cognitive biases. To address this prob-lem we develop strategy discovery methods that are robustto model misspecification. Robustness is achieved by model-ing model-misspecification and handling uncertainty about thereal-world according to Bayesian inference. We translate ourmethods into an intelligent tutor that automatically discoversand teaches robust planning strategies. Our robust cognitivetutor significantly improved human decision-making when themodel was so biased that conventional cognitive tutors were nolonger effective. These findings highlight that our robust strat-egy discovery methods are a significant step towards leverag-ing artificial intelligence to improve human decision-makingin the real world.

Pictorial Depth Cues in Young Children’s Drawings of Layouts and Objects

Humans have been faced with the challenges of pictorialproduction since at least the Paleolithic. Curiously, while thecapacity to navigate layouts and recognize objects in everydaylife comes almost effortlessly, inherited from our evolutionarypast, the capacity to draw layouts and objects is more effortful,often needing time to improve over the course of anindividual’s development and with the technologicalinnovations acquired through culture. The present studyexamines whether young children might nevertheless rely onphylogenetically ancient spatial capacities for navigation andobject recognition when creating uniquely human pictorial art.We apply a novel digital coding technique to a publiclyavailable dataset of young children’s drawings of layouts andobjects to explore children’s use of classic pictorial depth cuesincluding size, position, and overlap. To convey pictorialdepth, children appear to adopt several cues, without apreference among them, younger than had been suggested byprevious studies that used other, less rich, analytic techniques.Moreover, children use more cues to pictorial depth indrawings of layouts versus objects. Children’s creation ofuniquely human pictorial symbols may thus reflect theirheightened use of depth for navigating layouts compared torecognizing objects, both cognitive capacities that humansshare with other animals.

Exploring Category Structure in Children and Adults

Understanding how statistical regularities result in category learning requires access to the underlying psychological spacesin which these categories are represented. However, uncovering these spaces, especially in developmental settings, posessignificant experimental and methodological challenges what are relevant dimensions on which these spaces are organizedand how can we uncover them without prohibitively long or straining experiments?Here, we propose a novel way of uncovering these spaces. We learn participants implicit similarity functions, instantiatedas a neuronal network, by training on simple groupings of stimuli. In simulations, we show that our method can recovergroup-specific categorical structures. Furthermore, we show that young children quickly understand the grouping task, andspaces can be obtained in short, engaging experiments. Finally, we apply our method to uncover age-related differencesin category representations. In an experiment contrasting 4-5, 6-7 year-olds, and adults, we find that the learned spacesexhibit age-specific feature biases.

Childrens Mathematical Strategy Choices are not Influenced by NumberMagnitude

When solving mathematical equivalence problems (e.g. 5 + 3 + 6 = + 6), children use a variety of problem-solvingstrategies (Perry, Church, & Goldin-Meadow, 1988). We investigated factors potentially influencing how children choosestrategies and solve problems, including the size of the numbers, the problem structure, and the structure of childrensstrategy repertoires. We predicted that childrens strategy choices would be influenced by both the size of the numbersand the problem structure. We found that, contrary to our expectations, childrens strategy choices and their accuracywere not influenced by the size of the numbers in the problem. We also predicted that there would childrens strategyrepertoires would reveal conceptual structure. Children were highly consistent in their strategy choices across problems,and individual strategies showed evidence of varying affinity with one another. Childrens repertoires appear to reflectchildrens emerging understanding of equivalence, providing a potential target for personalizing instruction in mathematicalequivalence.

Differential Effect of Blocked and Interleaved Study on Category Learningby Classification and Inference

Previous research has indicated that the way of learning and thesequence of study influence how we learn and representcategories. However, most studies have focused onclassification learning and it has been rarely studied howlearning sequence influences inference learning. The currentstudy attempted to address this issue. Participants learned fourcategories by classification or inference in both blocked andinterleaved sequence. Then participants completed a transfertask and a feature prediction task. Results showed thatclassification learners encoded characteristic features andformed similarity-based representations in the blocked study,whereas in the interleaved study, they encoded deterministicfeatures and formed rule-based representations. In contrast, forinference learners, the blocked and interleaved study changedtheir learning and representation in the same direction. In bothsequences, inference learners encoded deterministic featuresand formed rule-based representations. These results suggestthat different mechanisms are likely to be involved forinference and classification learning.

A Grounded Framework of Cognition for Teaching, Learning, and Assessment inHigher Education

Models of cognition and learning structure and inform the thinking and action of educational practitioners and researchersalike. They serve as a communication device both within and between research and practice. There is a need for a holis-tic framework of cognition that appropriately reflects and synthesizes the current state of the field of the cognitive andlearning sciences with its rich diversity of research agendas. I propose such a model, which conceptualizes learning asunfolding from three interlinked basic domains: Conscious thought in the form of percepts and symbolic representationsin a symbolic-conceptual domain; foundational preconscious processing in a domain of cognitive metaphor; as well assituated, embodied interaction in a tangible enculturated agent-environment domain. The fundamental theoretical com-mitment of this Holistic Framework of Cognition and Learning is to dynamical systems theory. Emergence serves as thefunctional binder that ties the frameworks seemingly disparate elements together into a coherent whole.

A Task and Motion Approach to the Development of Planning

Developmental psychology presents us with a puzzle: though children are remarkably apt at planning their actions, theysuffer from surprising yet consistent shortcomings. We argue that these patterns of triumph and failure can be broadlycaptured by the framework of task and motion planning, where plans are hybrid entities consisting of both a structured,symbolic skeleton and a continuous, low-level trajectory. As a proof of concept, we model two case studies from the tooluse literature and show how their results can be understood by the interaction of symbolic and continuous plans.

Discovering Conceptual Hierarchy Through Explicit and Implicit Cues inChild-Directed Speech

n order for children to understand and reason about the worldin a mature fashion, they need to learn that conceptual cate-gories are organized in a hierarchical fashion (e.g., a dog isalso an animal). The caregiver linguistic input can play an im-portant role in this learning, and previous studies have doc-umented several cues in parental talk that can help childrenlearn a conceptual hierarchy. However, these previous studiesused different datasets and methods which made difficult thesystematic comparison of these cues and the study of their rel-ative contribution. Here, we use a large-scale corpus of child-directed speech and a classification-based evaluation methodwhich allowed us to investigate, within the same framework,various cues that varied in their degree of explicitness. Wefound the most explicit cues to be too sparse or too noisy tosupport robust learning (though part of the noise may be dueto imperfect operationalization). In contrast, the implicit cuesoffered, overall, a reliable source of information. Our workconfirms the utility of caregiver talk for conveying conceptualinformation. It provides a stepping stone towards a cognitivemodel that would use this information in a principled way,leading to testable predictions about children’s conceptual de-velopment.

Forms of Distributed Curiosity in the Collaborative Exploration of UnknownEnvironments by Artificial Agents

We propose a multi-agent approach to the problem of exploring unknown environments. We use a master-slave architec-ture. Mapping and exploration are coordinated by two separate agents: the mapper and the broker. The slave agents, theexplorers, are endowed with forms of curiosity, measured in terms of the decrease in uncertainty and novelty. The mapperis in charge of merging everyones maps and sending the global map back to each explorer, while the broker assigns nextmoves to every explorer, based on the interesting locations they spotted. The explorers analyse the environment they in-habit, send their local map to the mapper, pick points of interest based on their current knowledge of the area, send them tothe broker, and finally move to the location assigned by the broker. The advantages of these forms of distributed curiosity,together with those of the collaborative multi-agent exploration strategy, are tested in several scenarios.

How Hong Kong Preschoolers Perceive Chinese Characters: Are There AnyRelationships between the Effect of HP and Literacy Ability?

The present study examined the relationship between Holistic processing (HP), a well-established perceptual-expertisephenomenon for visual-object recognition, and Chinese literacy ability in Hong Kong preschoolers. The literacy abilityof participants was assessed by The Hong Kong Reading Ability Screening Test for Preschool Children (RAST-K); whileHP of Chinese characters was measured by adopting the complete composite paradigm from Hsiao & Cottrell (2009). Inline with the previous findings, preschoolers also showed HP in Chinese character perception, with a negative correlationbetween HP and writing ability when other measurements were controlled. This study provides a theoretical contributionon the study of Chinese writing difficulties among preschoolers. Educational implications will also be discussed.

How many observations is one generic worth?

Generic language (e.g., “Birds fly”) conveys generalizationsabout categories and is essential for learning beyond our directexperience. The meaning of generic language is notoriouslyhard to specify, however (e.g., penguins don’t fly). Tessler andGoodman (2019b) proposed a model for generics that is math-ematically equivalent to Bayesian belief-updating based on asingle pedagogical example, suggesting a deep connection be-tween learning from experience and learning from language.Relatedly, Csibra and Shamsudheen (2015) argue that genericsare inherently pedagogical, understood by infants as referringto a member of a kind. In two experiments with adults, wequantify the exchange-rate between generics and observationsby relating their belief-updating capacity, varying both thenumber of observations and whether they are presented ped-agogically or incidentally. We find generics convey strongergeneralizations than single pedagogical observations (Expt. 1),even when the property is explicitly demarcated (Expt. 2). Wesuggest revisions to the vague quantifier model of generics thatwould allow it to accommodate this intriguing exchange-rate.

Whom will Granny thank?Thinking about what could have been informs children’s inferences about relative helpfulness

To evaluate others’ actions, we consider action outcomes (e.g.,positive or negative) and the actors’ underlying intentions (e.g.,intentional or accidental). However, we often encounter situ-ations where neither actual outcomes nor intentions provideuseful evidence for evaluation but representations of unreal-ized (counterfactual) outcomes matter. Here we ask whetherpreschool-aged children consider counterfactual outcomes toevaluate whose action was more helpful. When two agentseach caught one of two falling apples (one caught it above atrash can and the other above a fruit basket), children chosethe former as the one who should be thanked (because oth-erwise the apple would’ve fallen into the trash). When theagents caught crushed cans, however, children made the op-posite choice, choosing the agent who caught the can over thefruit basket. Even though preschoolers typically struggle withcounterfactuals, children in our task readily engaged in suchreasoning in the context of social evaluation.

Bootstrapping an Imagined We for Cooperation

Remaining committed to a joint goal in the face of many entic- ing alternatives is challenging. Doing so while cooperating with others under uncertainty is even more so. Despite this, agents can successfully and robustly use bootstrapping to con- verge on a joint intention from randomness under the Imagined We framework. We demonstrate the power of this model in a real-time cooperative hunting task. Additionally, we run a suite of model experiments to answer some of the potential chal- lenges to converging that this model could face under imperfect conditions. Specifically, we ask what happens when (1) there are increasingly many equivalent choices? (2) I only have an approximate model of you? and (3) my perception is noisy? We show through a set of model experiments that this framework is robust to all three of these manipulations.

Poster Session 3

“Conscious” Multi-Modal Perceptual Learning forGrounded Simulation-Based Cognition

Barsalou (1999) presented a simulation-based theory ofgrounded cognition called Perceptual Symbol Systems.According to this theory, a fully functional conceptual systemcan be implemented using only modal representations (akaperceptual symbols) and simulations. While the theory hasgained considerable neuroscientific and experimental support,there is an urgent need for computational accounts that fleshout the theory. The current paper explores one approach forimplementing these computational foundations. We present animplementation of perceptual symbols, simulators, simulation-based perception, and “conscious” multi-modal perceptuallearning based on generative neural networks, called

The Plausible Impossible: Graded Notions of Impossibility Across Cultures

Events that violate the laws of nature are, by definition, impossible, but recent research suggests that people view some violations as “more impossible” than others (Shtulman & Morgan, 2017). When evaluating the difficulty of magic spells, American adults are influenced by seemingly irrelevant considerations, judging, for instance, that it would be more difficult to levitate a bowling ball than a basketball even though weight should no longer be a consideration if contact is no longer necessary for support. Here, we explore these effects in a non-Western context—China—where magical events are represented differently in fiction and reasoning styles are often more holistic than analytic. Across several studies, Chinese adults showed the same tendency as American adults to honor implicit causal constraints when evaluating the plausibility of magical events. These findings suggest that graded notions of impossibility are shared across cultures, possibly because they are a byproduct of causal knowledge.

When Me Is Mine: An Embodied Origin of Psychological Ownership?

Neurological evidence has shown that brain damages canselectively impair the ability to discriminate between objectsbelonging to others and those that we feel are our own. Despitethe ubiquity and relevance of this sense of object ownership forour life, the underlying cognitive mechanisms are still poorlyunderstood. Here we ask whether psychological ownership ofan object can be based on its incorporation in one’s body image.To explore this possibility with healthy participants, weemployed a modified version of the rubber hand illusion inwhich both the participant and the rubber hand wore a ring. Weused the self-prioritization effect in a perceptual matching taskas an indirect measure of the sense of (dis)ownership overobjects. Results indicate that undermining the bodily self hascascade effects on the representation of owned objects, at leastfor those associated with the body for a long time.

Gendered Robots Can Change Children’s Gender Stereotyping

Research suggests children readily treat robots as social actorsand sources of information for learning. Here we ask if childrenuse depictions of gender-counterstereotypic robots (e.g., afemale construction worker robot) and gender-stereotypicrobots (e.g., a female secretary robot) as sources of informationabout cultural gender stereotypes. Forty-five 6- to 8-year-oldchildren participated in a short counterstereotyping task.Children in the counterstereotypical condition viewed videosof cartoon female gendered robots with culturally stereotypedmasculine occupations, interests in activities, and traits.Children in the stereotypical condition viewed videos ofcartoon female gendered robots with culturally stereotypedfeminine attributes. Children completed a measure of genderstereotyping before and after viewing the intervention videos.From pretest to posttest, children’s gender stereotypingdecreased in the counterstereotypical condition and increasedin the stereotypical condition. These finding suggest childrenmay learn from robots as models of cultural gender stereotypes.

The limits of learning to learn

Learning to learn is a reduction in the amount of training neededfor task attainment across a series of similar tasks. Transferdifferentiates (adult) humans from other species, portending awindow into unique aspects of human learning. However, itsunclear whether such differences are quantitative, or qualitativeand what it means for the evolution/development of cognition.In this paper, learning is regarded as a (categorical) limit. Alimit is a universal construction, and so transfer follows froma (generalized) optimization process. This result provides aformal basis for comparison/contrast of learning transfer in hu-mans and other species—another step to bringing the empiricalquestion into sharper relief.

Calibrating Trust in Autonomous Systems in a Dynamic Environment

Appropriately calibrating trust in autonomous systems is es-sential for successful collaboration between humans and thesystems. Over-trust and under-trust often happen in dynami-cally changing environments, and they can be major causes ofserious issues with safety and efficiency. Many studies haveexamined the role of continuous system transparency in keep-ing proper trust calibration; however, not many studies havefocused on how to find poor trust calibration nor how to miti-gate it. In our proposed method of trust calibration, a behavior-based approach is used to detect improper trust calibration, andcognitive cues called “trust calibration cues” are presented tousers as triggers for trust calibration. We conducted an on-line experiment with a drone simulator. Seventy participantsperformed pothole inspection tasks manually or relied on thedrone’s automatic inspection. The results demonstrated thatadaptively presenting a simple cue could significantly promotetrust calibration in both over-trust and under-trust cases.

Infants Relax in Response to Unfamiliar Foreign Lullabies

Music is a human universal characterized by acoustical forms that are predictive of its behavioral functions. For example,listeners accurately distinguish between unfamiliar lullabies and other songs on the basis of their features alone. Thiscould be attributable to adults extensive musical experience, however. Here we show that infants (N = 144) relax inresponse to foreign lullabies, relative to matched foreign non-lullabies, as measured by heart rate, electrodermal activity,and pupillometry. These results were unrelated to age, suggesting the relaxation response is not a function of infantsrich musical experiences. Infants showed no visual preferences for the animated characters producing the songs, but theyattended more to the lullabies, blinking less during the singing. Moreover, the infants parents chose lullabies as the songsthat they themselves would use to calm their fussy infant. These findings raise the possibility that links between form andfunction in music are innately specified.

The spatial arrangement method of measuring similarity can capture high-dimensional, semantic structures

Despite its centrality to cognition, similarity is expensive tomeasure, spurring development of techniques like the SpatialArrangement Method (SpAM), wherein participants placeitems on a 2-dimensional plane such that proximity reflectssimilarity. While SpAM hastens similarity measurement, itssuitability for higher-dimensional stimuli is unknown. InStudy 1, we collected SpAM data for eight differentcategories composed of 20-30 words each. Participant-aggregated SpAM distances correlated strongly (r=.71) withpairwise similarity judgments, although below SpAM andpairwise judgment split-half reliabilities (r’s>.9), and cross-validation with multidimensional scaling fits at increasingdimensionalities suggested that aggregated SpAM datafavored higher dimensional solutions for 7 of the 8 categories.In study 2, we showed that SpAM can recover the Big Fivefactor space of personality traits, and that cross-validationfavors a four- or five-dimension solution on this dataset. Weconclude that SpAM is an accurate and reliable method ofmeasuring similarity for high-dimensional items.

Developmental Differences in Information Sampling Effort

Adolescence is marked by increased risky decisions. Making better decisions typically requires obtaining more informa-tion relevant to that decision. Adolescents may be especially tolerant of uncertainty when making decisions or averseto the effort needed to obtain more information. We had adolescents and adults complete an effort-based informationsampling task, in which participants could sample information until deciding that the evidence obtained was sufficientfor responding. Effort was manipulated by varying the number of mouse clicks required to sample information acrosstrials. Surprisingly, adolescents sampled more than adults prior to responding at low effort and continued to sample moreeven as effort requirements increased. Computational modeling indicated that adolescents and adults used simple heuris-tics to decide between sampling more or responding but that adolescents sought a higher evidence threshold than adults.Adolescents may seek more information and be less averse to effort costs in information sampling compared with adults.

The impact of speech disfluencies on the believability and recall of sentences

It is well-established that when people process sentences fluently, they are more likely to believe the sentences are true.It has also been shown that sentences which include disfluencies improve peoples memory for the sentences content.We sought to test whether both of these effects were present simultaneously. In Experiment 1, we found that speechdisfluencies do not appear to always aid memory, but they do impact participants truth judgments. In Experiment 2 wefound that this impact on truth judgments may not be due to processing fluency, but rather due to reasoning about thespeakers certainty. We found a similar effect on truth judgments when participants were presented with sentences thatwere fluent but had rising (i.e. uncertain) intonation in comparison with falling intonation. In both cases, the effect waslocalized to only the sentences that had the cue, rather than to all sentences that the speaker produced.

The Effect of Knowledge about a Group on Perceived Group Variability and Certainty about Stereotype-Based Inferences

People often learn about categories, particularly social categories, based on biased information. Unless people are able to correct for this, they may develop biased beliefs and inferences about these categories. The current research examines if potentially biased information about social groups makes groups appear more homogeneous, and makes people more confident in their inferences about group members. Two sources of biases are considered: due to lacking first-hand experience with a group, or due to having second-hand information from the media or other people. Both sources made groups appear more homogeneous, suggesting that information biases were present and not corrected for. However, only second-hand knowledge led to greater confidence about group members, because, when people lacked first-hand knowledge, their uncertainty about the group average counteracted this effect. This highlights the importance of understanding biases present in people’s information, and corrective processes that may allow people to continue to make unbiased inferences.

A study of hand manipulation and spatial tasks in which preschool girls performwell.

Image manipulation has been reported in mental rotation (Noda, 2010). The purpose of this study is to examine thedevelopment of hand manipulation and gender differences in the placement tasks. Participants included 26 five-year-olds(15 boys, 11 girls), 29 four-year-olds (15 boys, 14 girls), and 29 three-year-olds (14 boys, 15 girls). The task was similarto the WISC picture arrangement. As a procedure, 0 and 180 cards were placed on both sides. Participants were asked theimage of inclining in the intermediate states. Then, 45, 90, and 135 cards were placed. The convex and the bird-like picturewere used. The results showed that girls performed better than boys. And the method of manipulation has changed withage. Boys manipulated cards more frequently than girls. As performance increased, manipulation frequency decreasedin boys while it increased in girls. This may be due to developmental changes in cognitive processing between boys andgirls.

Characteristics of Visualizations and Texts in Elementary School Biology Books

A breath of research has investigated how characteristics of visualizations and characteristics of texts influence learningand generalization. Given that students integrate information from visualizations and text, we investigate how the char-acteristics of the text depends on characteristics of the visualization. We focus on two characteristics of visualizations(perceptual richness, and whether they display variability), and one characteristic of the text (use of generic language). Wefound that the majority of visualization were detailed photographs and do not display variability. Most of the text usedgeneric language, but we found that some visualizations qualified these generic statements with more specific phrases.The use of generic was more common for visualizations that display variability and photographs. Our study highlightsthe importance of investigating what students are normally exposed to and suggest that future research on multi-medialearning should place close attention to the characteristics of the text that accompany the visualization.

Are all Framing Effects Created Equal? Relationships between Risky ChoiceFraming, Metaphor Framing, and Language

Behavior in classic framing tasks is unrelated to other cognitive bias measures, but little is known about the relation-ship among different types of framing effects. Across two experiments, participants in the US and India completed aclassic risky choice framing task, a metaphor framing task, and measures of cognitive style, linguistic proficiency, andmetaphor usage. We found no relationship between performance on the framing tasks for either sample, suggesting theytap into different underlying processes. Interestingly, language proficiency predicted risky choice framing behavior innative speakers and metaphor framing in non-native speakers. While there was a positive relationship between metaphorusage and metaphor framing for US participants, the sample from India showed a negative relationship, suggesting thatcurrent measures of metaphor usage may assess different behaviors for native versus non-native speakers. Overall, theresults suggest a heterogenous account of the mechanisms underlying framing effects even as they highlight the importantrole of language.

Decision-Making Under Uncertainty in Major Depression Patients

Substantial evidence has suggested that major depression is associated with a dysregulated dopamine system, which playsa pivotal role in decision-making under uncertainty. Previous research has proposed that dopamine enhances the weightgiven to current sensory information (sensory weight) versus prior beliefs, yet how much this relationship holds true indepression remains a topic under debate. To examine whether depression patients have decreased sensory weight dueto disturbed dopaminergic neurotransmissions, we used a visual coin-catching task in which uncertainty in both priorand sensory information varied. Decision-making strategies during the task were modeled by Bayesian statistics. Ourresults supported that depression patients preserved the ability to learn both prior and sensory information uncertainty,comparable to healthy controls. In contrast to our prediction, depression patients did not show decreased reliance onsensory information compared to controls, suggesting that depression does not induce a universal alteration in decision-making strategies under uncertainty. Our study provides empirical evidence that depression does not always show deficitsin uncertainty processing regardless of its correlation with dopamine dysregulations.

QuLBIT: Quantum-Like Bayesian Inference Technologies forCognition and Decision

This paper provides the foundations of a uni-fied cognitive decision-making framework (QulBIT)which is derived from quantum theory. The mainadvantage of this framework is that it can caterfor paradoxical and irrational human decision mak-ing. Although quantum approaches for cognitionhave demonstrated advantages over classical prob-abilistic approaches and bounded rationality mod-els, they still lack explanatory power. To addressthis, we introduce a novel explanatory analysis ofthe decision-maker’s belief space. This is achievedby exploiting quantum interference effects as a wayof both quantifying and explaining the decision-maker’s uncertainty. We detail the main modulesof the unified framework, the explanatory analy-sis method, and illustrate their application in situ-ations violating the Sure Thing Principle.

Modulating the coherence effect in causal-based processing

Causal-based cognition is thought to be relevant for human beings because it allows inferring the unfolding of events. Theories of causal-based cognition offer researchers a way to understand inter-feature relations, above and beyond the purely associative relations posited by similarity theories. In the causal-model theory (a.k.a. the Generative Model), people are thought to categorize an exemplar depending on how likely its particular feature combination is, given the category’s causal model. This mechanism predicts the coherence effect (i.e., when people categorize, features interact). This effect has been widely reported in the literature. In the current experiment, we sought to specify conditions that modulate the coherence effect. To that end, we implemented a between-subjects manipulation where participants had to judge either category membership or category consistency. Our results show that subjects exhibit a larger coherence effect in consistency condition. We discuss our results’ relevance for causal-model theory and for the possibility of distinguishing causal-based from similarity-based processing.

How Can We Access Children Basic Academic Skills? The Possibility of CorrectedAcademic Skills via an Alternative Approach

The skills of reading, writing, and calculating manually are fundamental parts of subject learning. However, information-communication technology is expected to serve as an alternative to these basic academic skills. We conducted a study of158 Japanese elementary students (2nd to 6th grade) comparing students basic academic skills to their corrected academicskills, as measured with accommodations. Students were asked to perform independent reading and reading with a lis-tening comprehension task (Experiment 1), a manual Kanji-writing word task (Japanese characters) and a multiple-choicetask to measure Kanji knowledge (Experiment 2), and a manual calculation task and one using a calculator (Experiment3). Comparing the scores on the tasks performed by themselves and with accommodation, we found that 5 to 13% of thestudents were supported in their basic academic skills by the accommodation. The cognitive processes involved in learningthe basic academic skills and the corrected one are discussed.

Intentional information sharing promotes cumulative culture relative toinadvertent behavioural cues: an experimental demonstration

Using an experimental transmission design, we investigated the extent to which intentional information-sending creates anaccumulation of beneficial information, relative to transmission via inadvertent information.A small subset of an information providers search was transmitted to an information receiver, either selected by theinformation provider themselves (Intentional), or randomly sampled from their full search history (Inadvertent). A thirdcondition where information receivers were shown all of the information providers search attempts was included as acontrol.Intentional information-sending led to cumulative improvements that were comparable to receiving full information froma previous participants search, demonstrating that intentional information-sending had promoted cumulative cultural evo-lution. A follow-up study manipulated whether the sender also received feedback from the receiver which provided infor-mation about locations which had not been searched. No difference was found between these conditions, indicating thatfor this task, bidirectional communication did not further boost the effects of unidirectional intentional communication.

Promoting relational responding by varying presentation conditions

The relational match-to-sample (RMS) task assesses whether people are sensitive to matching relational content and con-sider such matches more compelling than an object-based alternative. On each trial, participants see a triad of shapesequences: target item (XYX), object match (VZY), and relational match (TST). In prior research, participants show arelational preference supporting the structural alignment account of similarity-based processing. We address two goals:1) assessing generality across variation in stimulus materials and task wording; and 2) investigating the hypothesis thatrelational responding can be promoted via presentation conditions for the RMS task. Specifically, along with the standardsimultaneous presentation of target plus options, we tested two sequential variations: presenting each possible match inisolation before showing the full triad and presenting only the target item for evaluation before showing the full triad.Results are discussed in the context of structural alignment theory and the role of relational encoding.

May I Have Your Attention? Testing a Subjective Attention Scale

The concept of ‘attention’ – our ability to focus on particularparts of the world - is a seemingly simple one. Research,however, often driven by clinicians need to diagnoseattentional deficits after brain injuries, has demonstrated itscomplexity. This has resulted in significant testing beingrequired to assess the full range of attentional abilities.Herein, we designed a Subjective Attention Scale, consistingof 15 Likert-scale questions based on five types of attentionidentified by Sohlberg and Mateer (1989). Preliminary datasuggested the scale had good psychometric properties(Cronback’s α > 0.8) and an interpretable factor structure (4factors; 49% of variance). However, it showed almost nosignificant correlations with measures from six laboratorytests of attention. Instead, analyses suggest peoples’subjective beliefs regarding their attentional abilities mapmore closely onto the Conscientiousness personality trait thanthose traits identified from clinical work.

Modeling Gestalt Visual Reasoning on Ravens Progressive Matrices UsingGenerative Image Inpainting Techniques

Psychologists recognize Raven’s Progressive Matrices as an effective test of general intelligence. While many computa-tional models investigate top-down, deliberative reasoning on the test, there has been less research on bottom-up perceptualprocesses, like Gestalt image completion, that are also critical in human test performance. We investigate how Gestalt vi-sual reasoning on the Raven’s test can be modeled using generative image inpainting techniques from computer vision.We demonstrate that a reasoning agent using an off-the-shelf inpainting model trained on object photographs achieves ascore of 27/36 on the Colored Progressive Matrices, which corresponds to average performance for nine-year-old chil-dren. When our agent uses inpainting models trained on other datasets (faces, places, and textures), performance is lower.Our results illustrate how learning visual regularities in real-world images can translate into successful reasoning aboutartificial test stimuli, and also how different learning inputs translate into different levels of performance.

Beyond rationality: We infer other people’s goals by learning agent-variableexpectations of efficient action

Our ability to make sense of goal-directed behavior is central to social reasoning. From infancy, this capacity is structuredaround an assumption that agents act efficiently. But agents are often inefficient and how we move is affected by ouremotional states and personal idiosyncrasies. How, then, does an assumption of efficiency allow us to accurately interpretpeople’s actions? We hypothesized that people expect agents to move efficiently relative to an agent-specific baselinerather than to an objective notion of efficiency. Consistent with this, we found that people can quickly learn and subtractagent-idiosyncratic movements when interpreting goal-directed action (Experiment 1). Moreover, in a free-response task,people’s propensity to explain superfluous movement in terms of goals depended on the agent’s relative efficiency ratherthan on the path’s objective efficiency (Experiment 2). Our results show that people flexibly adjust their expectations ofefficiency by attending to how agents typically move.

Learning the internal structure of novel categories

How do we learn the internal feature co-occurrence structure of a new category? We constructed novel animal categoriesusing a network science framework in order to examine category structure learning. Two categories were defined bydistinct graph structures in which nodes corresponded to features (e.g., bushy tail, black fur) and edges captured within-category feature co-occurrences. The graphs contained isomorphic core structures, in which certain features occurred inall category exemplars. In a high-modularity graph, additional features formed clusters of co-occurring features, whereasin the low-modularity graph additional features were randomly distributed. Participants learned about these categories ina missing-feature task which probed different kinds of category structure knowledge. Though core structure was identicalacross categories, core structure was better learned in the high- relative to low-modularity category. This suggests thatlearning features of a new category is influenced by the global structure of the concept.

Exploring demographic differences in a large-scale study of Spanish wordassociation norms: The role of age, gender, and nationality

Free association techniques, which involve listing the first word that comes to mind after a probe word (e.g., probe wordDOG eliciting response BONE) are powerful tools in the cognitive sciences. However, their validity and generalizabilitydepend on the total sample size and the diversity of the participant pool. We report a large-scale free association normingstudy conducted in Spanish, the most widely spoken and geographically diverse romance language, using the methodologylaid out by De Deyne and colleagues (2019, BRM). Our results include 1 million responses to 5,000 cues from 20,000participants. Using our norms, we explored how the demographic factors of age, gender, and nationality shaped responses.We observed that between 12-18% of cue-response pairs varied systematically based on these demographic factors. Ourresults illustrate how free associations can reveal broad similarities and systematic demographic differences in lexico-semantic structure.

Enhancing Cognitive Assessment through Multimodal Sensing:A Case Study Using the Block Design Test

Many cognitive assessments are limited by their reliance onrelatively sparse measures of performance, like per-item ac-curacy or reaction time. Capturing more detailed behavioralmeasurements from cognitive assessments will enhance theirutility in many settings, from individual clinical evaluationsto large-scale research studies. We demonstrate the feasibilityof combining scene and gaze cameras with supervised learn-ing algorithms to automatically measure key behaviors on theblock design test, a widely used test of visuospatial cognitiveability. We also discuss how this block-design measurementsystem could enhance the assessment of many critical cogni-tive and meta-cognitive functions such as attention, planning,progress monitoring, and strategy selection.

Biasing Moral Decisions Using Eye Movements: Replication and Simulation

A current debate concerns the degree to which moral rea-soning is susceptible to bias from low-level perceptual cues.P ̈arnamets et al. (2015) reported that moral decisions couldbe biased by manipulating the timing of a prompt to respondvia measurement of eye gaze, but these results were critiquedby Newell and Le Pelley (2018) as a potential design artifact.To reconcile these findings, we first replicate the previous ex-periments with an adjusted stimulus set. Then, we present theresults of a drift-diffusion model that simulates our findings,offering an account of the mechanism by which the gaze-basedtiming manipulation can bias moral decision-making.

Cognitive Machine Theory of Mind

A major challenge for research in Artificial Intelligence (AI)is to develop systems that can infer humans’ goals and beliefswhen observing their behavior alone (i.e., systems that haveTheory of Mind, ToM). In this research we use a theoretically-grounded, pre-existent cognitive model to demonstrate the de-velopment of ToM from observation of other agents’ behavior.The cognitive model relies on Instance-Based Learning The-ory (IBLT) of experiential decision making, that distinguishesit from previous models that are hand-crafted for particular set-tings, complex, or unable to explain a cognitive developmentof ToM. An IBL model was designed to be an observer ofagents’ navigation in gridworld environments and was queriedafterwards to predict the actions of new agents in new (notexperienced before) gridworlds. The IBL observer can inferand predict potential behaviors from just a few samples ofagents’ past behavior of random and goal-directed reinforce-ment learning agents. Furthermore the IBL observer is able toinfer the agent’s false belief and pass a classic ToM test com-monly used in humans. We discuss the advantages of usingIBLT to develop models of ToM, and the potential to predicthuman ToM.

Effects of Battle and Journey Metaphors on CharitableDonations for Cancer Patients

Having cancer is often described metaphorically as a battle(“my fight against cancer”) or as a journey (“my path throughcancer treatment”). Previous experimental work has demon-strated that these metaphors can influence people’s reason-ing and emotional inferences about experiences with cancer(Hendricks, Demj ́en, Semino, & Boroditsky, 2018; Hauser &Schwarz, 2019). However, it is currently unknown how theuse of these metaphorical frames translates into behavioralchanges, such as the likelihood and magnitude of charitablegiving. Using hand-labeled data from more than 5,000 Go-FundMe cancer-related campaigns, we ask how a campaign’suse of metaphor predicts several measures of donation behav-ior beyond what other control variables predict (e.g. shares onFacebook). We find that the presence of either metaphor fam-ily (battle or journey) has a positive effect on campaign successand donation behavior.

Euphemism and Gender: A Computational Inquiry

Euphemisms are a part of language which enable the discussion of taboo topics, without directly naming those taboos.Previous work suggests that women use euphemisms more than men do. However, there has been no quantitative attemptto test this proposal. We develop a simple computational method to investigate whether men and women use euphemismdifferently in the Canadian Hansard and US Congressional datasets. For a set of taboo-euphemism pairs (e.g. died-passedaway), we computed the relative frequency of the euphemism in speech from female and male speakers. Preliminaryevidence from these two political datasets show that women do use the euphemistic expressions more than men do, butthey also use the taboo expressions more. Future work should investigate whether the same pattern holds in data fromdifferent domains.

Predicting Difficulty with Learning in the Mathematics Classroom: TheUsefulness of Heart Rate Variability

Mathematical thinking and learning are negatively affected by adverse childhood experiences (ACEs), which have beenshown to impact school attendance, behavioral issues, and achievement of grade-level standards of a variety of academicsubjects (Blodgett & Lanigan, 2018). ACEs are often linked to permanent physiological changes to the nervous system ina dose-response relationship (Dube, Felitti, Dong, Giles, & Anda, 2003). Laboratory studies have identified physiologicalindicators–such as heart rate variability–which can point to students who may have unique learning needs, but this has notyet been tested in a classroom setting, where students learning needs may be amplified (Smith, Thayer, Khalsa, & Lane,2017). In this study we use sport watches to explore the value of measuring heart rate variability of students while theyare in the classroom to predict those who may need support to optimize learning in math class.

Dynamics of spatio-temporal scope of attention: Temporal Correlations inreaction time data

Recent studies have emphasized on the idea that attention is a multi-faceted phenomenon that emerges from interactionbetween a number of different selection-based processes, and is influenced both by the expectations from the environmentas well as the constrains of the underlying cognitive system. Dynamical system approach enables us to look at temporalstructure of behavior and talk about the underlying system. With help of three experiments, the study looks at how thetemporal structure of reaction time is influenced by predictability of the environment as well as the task , manipulating bothspatial scope of attention as well as temporal scope of attention. Reaction time of participant is treated as a time-series andHurst component is estimated to measure nature of long-range temporal correlations. Results show an interaction betweentask-demands and predictability of the environment on LRTC, suggesting that task-related constraints and environmentalconstraints are handled by interdependent processes.

Evidence for representation of symbolic associations and Negation logical operatorin 4 mo old infants

In experiment 1 in an EEG-ERP design we showed 4 mo-old infants who are trained on 2 associations strictly in labelto object direction and 2 other associations in the opposite direction can retain these representations bi-directionally, asopposed to several other species failing on this task (Urcuioli, 2015), suggesting that the label-object associations areacquired symbolically in early infancy. In Experiment 2 infants were home trained on four label-object associations asin Exp 1 and then received a brief familiarization that when the labels precede a pseudo-word, the upcoming object canbe any except the one originally matched with that label. Results suggest that infants discriminate between incongruentand congruent applications of this negation pseudoword on a novel label and can furthermore generalize to new objects asevidenced by the patterns of their EEG-ERP responses, providing a first direct evidence for negation in early infancy.

Cognitive offloading increases false recall.

Offloading to-be-remembered information is a ubiquitous memory strategy, yet in relying on external memory stores,our ability to recall from internal memory is often diminished. In the present investigation, we examine how offloadingimpacts true and false recall. Across three preregistered experiments, participants studied and wrote word lists that wereeach strongly associated with an unstudied critical word. We compared recall in the offloading condition (i.e., when theyexpected to have access to their written lists during recall) with a no-offloading condition (i.e., when they did not expectto have access to their written lists during recall). In the absence of the written external stores, offloading decreased truerecall of the presented words while increasing false recall for the unpresented critical words. Results are discussed in termsof offloadings differential effects on the formation of gist and verbatim traces during encoding.

The early cue catches the word: how gesture supports cross-situational wordlearning

Gesture is important for language acquisition, but how gesture and its temporal aspects integrate with other informationis not fully known. We manipulated referential ambiguity, and the availability and timing of a deictic gesture duringtraining on a word-learning task with adults to assess how gestural cues alter learning when tested on those words. Wedemonstrate that the presence of a gestural cue during training in a condition with two potential referents can reducereferential ambiguity sufficiently to produce performance at test similar to a condition with only one referent. We furthershow that learners demonstrate better performance at test with gestures that occur prior to, rather than after, the verballabel in training. Gesture during learning thus appears better at predicting, rather than confirming the referent. Theseresults offer insight into how cues can facilitate the disambiguation of meaning during word learning. Pre-registration:https://osf.io/exq7d/?view only=8b28001e56404ff79c2258f3b66d7474 Keywords: word learning; language acquisition;multiple cues; gestures; temporal; word-referent mapping

Hemispheric asymmetries in “expert” processingof semantic relationships during reading

How does individual-level variation in experience andknowledge influence neural mechanisms recruited during real-time language comprehension? We used event-related brainpotentials (ERPs) combined with lateralized visualpresentations of critical sentence-final words to examineasymmetries in hemispheric processing as individuals whovaried in their knowledge of the fictional world of Harry Potter(HP) read sentences about general topics / HP. HP sentenceendings were either contextually supported, unrelatedanomalies, or semantically related anomalies. Amongst HP“experts,” both hemispheres were sensitive to contextualsupport, but only the right hemisphere (RH) was sensitive tothe related anomaly manipulation. The exact pattern of resultsdepended on the relationship (categorical vs event). Ourfindings are in line with accounts on which the left hemisphere(LH) activates narrow/specific semantic contents and the RHactivates a broader range. We tentatively hypothesize thatcontent experts may exploit these hemispheric differences inscope of activation.

Gender Gaps Correlate with Gender Bias in Social Media Word Embeddings

Gender status, gender roles, and gender values vary widelyacross cultures. Anthropology has provided qualitative ac-counts of economic, cultural, and biological factors that im-pact social groups, and international organizations have gath-ered indices and surveys to help quantify gender inequalitiesin states. Concurrently, machine learning research has recentlycharacterized pervasive gender biases in AI language models,rooting from biases in their textual training data. While thesemachine biases produce sub-optimal inferences, they may helpus characterize and predict statistical gender gaps and gendervalues in the culture(s) that produced the training text, therebyhelping us understand cultural context through big data. Thispaper presents an approach to (1) construct word embeddings(i.e., vector-based lexical semantics) from a region’s social me-dia, (2) quantify gender bias in word embeddings, and (3)correlate biases with survey responses and statistical gendergaps in education, politics, economics, and health. We validatethis approach using 2018 Twitter data spanning 143 countriesand 51 U.S. territories, 23 international and 7 U.S. gender gapstatistics, and seven international survey results from the WorldValue Survey. Integrating these heterogeneous data across cul-tures is an important step toward understanding (1) how biasesin culture might manifest in machine learning models and (2)how to estimate gender inequality from big data.

Processing particularized pragmatic inferences under load

A long-standing question in language understanding iswhether pragmatic inferences are effortful or whether theyhappen seamlessly without measurable cognitive effort. Wehere measure the strength of particularized pragmatic infer-ences in a setting with high vs. low cognitive load. Cognitiveload is induced by a secondary dot tracking task. If this type ofpragmatic inference comes at no cognitive processing cost, in-ferences should be similarly strong in both the high and the lowload condition. If they are effortful, we expect a smaller effectsize in the dual tasking condition. Our results show that partic-ipants who have difficulty in dual tasking (as evidenced by in-correct answers to comprehension questions) exhibit a smallerpragmatic effect when they were distracted with a secondarytask in comparison to the single task condition. This findingsupports the idea that pragmatic inferences are effortful.

A cognitive computational model of mindsets

An individuals intelligence mindset describes their implicit beliefs about whether intelligence is fixed (fixed mindset)or malleable (growth mindset). Here, we introduce a computational framework to unify and build upon findings in themindsets literature. We postulate that individuals maintain a mental model of others skill, in which current skill is thesum of innate skill (1) and skill acquired from experience (growth potential (2) times fraction of potential realised (3)).An observed current skill level is consistent with multiple combinations of (1), (2), and (3). To disambiguate, the modelobserver performs probabilistic inference, which requires priors. In particular, we conceptualise a fixed mindset usinga high-variance prior over innate skill and a low-variance, low-mean prior over growth potential. Through proofs andsimulations, we demonstrate that our model accounts for empirical findings in terms of the latent psychological processes.Our results offer promise for a computational cognitive science of mindsets.

Contrasting RNN-based and simulation-based models of human physicalparameter inference

A number of recent studies have used ideal observer models to capture human physical reasoning as based on approximatemental simulation driven through a realistic inner physics engine. While these approaches can match human competencein specific tasks, they are still relatively far from cognitive plausibility and are limited in their ability to capture patternsof human biases and errors. In this work, we train a recurrent neural network (RNN) extensively on a physical reasoningtask – conceptually mimicking the lifetime of experience that human adults have to build physical competence. We thenexamine its behavior alongside that of adults in the same test set of problems. We find that the RNN matches humanpatterns of judgments and errors much better than the idealised simulation account. We highlight specific situations whereboth RNN and humans erred and discuss the ramifications for current debates about the prevalence of physical simulationin cognition.

Identifying the Bounds of Peripersonal Space with Phase Transition Methods

The shape of the transition in multisensory integration between the (defensive) peripersonal space (DPPS) and the extrap-ersonal space (EPS) has recently been debated. Contributing to this discussion, we approached the DPPS-EPS transitionfrom a dynamic systems perspective. Specifically, the dynamic complexity of visuotactile reaction times to moving stimuliwas employed to evaluate the presence of phase transitions. Reecting well-established ndings on the DPPS-EPS transi-tion, we hypothesized that a phase transition would be identied for looming stimuli, but not for receding stimuli, andthat the phase transition for looming threatening stimuli would be located further away from the body than for loomingnon-threatening stimuli. Contrary to these hypotheses, we found that phase transitions for receding stimuli were moreprominent and located further away from the body than phase transitions for looming stimuli. Nonetheless, we considerthe identification of phase transitions to be a promising approach for future studies of multisensory integration.

Young Children Do Not Anticipate That Sunk Costs Lead to Irrational Choices

When people invest a lot in completing a project or in obtaining a resource, they often overvalue it. This sunk costbias leads people to persist in pursuing failing projects, and to favor resources they have invested in over alternatives.We investigated whether children (N=135) and adults (N=150) consider this bias when predicting peoples choices. InExperiments 1 and 2, 4-6-year-olds and adults saw stories where an agent collected two identical objects, one easy toobtain and one difficult.They then predicted which object the agent would keep. Experiment 3 used similar stories toexamine 6-year-olds predictions about how they would act in this situation. Adults were sensitive to sunk costs, butchildren were not. These findings suggest that young children may not show the sunk cost bias, and also may struggle toanticipate how cognitive biases can lead people to depart from making rational choices.

Analogy as Nonparametric Bayesian Inference over Relational Systems

Much of human learning and inference can be framed withinthe computational problem of relational generalization. Inthis project, we propose a Bayesian model that generalizesrelational knowledge to novel environments by analogicallyweighting predictions from previously encountered relationalstructures. First, we show that this learner outperforms anaive, theory-based learner on relational data derived fromrandom- and Wikipedia-based systems when experience withthe environment is small. Next, we show how our formal-ization of analogical similarity translates to the selection andweighting of analogies. Finally, we combine the analogy-and theory-based learners in a single nonparametric Bayesianmodel, and show that optimal relational generalizationtransitions from relying on analogies to building a theory ofthe novel system with increasing experience in it. Beyondpredicting unobserved interactions better than either baseline,this formalization gives a computational-level perspective onthe formation and abstraction of analogies themselves.

Individual Working Memory Capacity Moderates the Power Effect on CognitiveTask Performance

The experience of power is known to help people pursue their goals more effectively. It has been argued that this is becausethe powerful are better at managing working memory processes during goal pursuit. However, past research has oftendisregarded individual differences in working memory capacity. We examined how manipulated power affects peoplescognitive task performance, depending on their working memory capacity. Results showed that high-power participantswith a relatively lower capacity performed significantly better than low-power participants, whereas individuals with ahigher capacity performed equally well in both high- and low-power conditions. Thus, individuals with a relatively highercapacity were less affected by the experience of low power than individuals with a lower capacity, who in turn profittedmore from the experience of high power. Overall, our findings imply that individuals working memory capacity is animportant factor to consider in the power effect on cognitive task performance.

What is a Choice in Reinforcement Learning?

n reinforcement learning (RL) experiments, participantslearn to associate stimuli with rewarding responses. RLmodels capture such learning by estimating stimulus-responsevalues. But what is a response? RL algorithms can model anyresponse type, whether it is a basic motor action (e.g. pressinga key), or a more abstract, non-motor choice (e.g. selectingpizza at the restaurant). Are these different responses learnedthe same way? In this study, we examine differences betweenlearning a rewarding association between (1) a stimulus and amotor action and (2) two stimuli. We show that learningdiffers between these two conditions, contrary to the commonimplicit assumption that response type does not matter.Specifically, participants were slower and less accurate inlearning to select a rewarding stimulus. Using computationalmodeling, we show that the values of motor actions interferedwith the values of stimulus responses, resulting in moreincorrect choices in the latter condition.

A spiking neural architecture for conscious chaining of mental operations

Flexible information routing in the brain is crucial to perform sequential tasks in which an operation takes as input theresult of the preceding operation (e.g. add 2 to a given digit, then compare the result to 5). Experiments suggest thatindividual operations such as addition and comparison can proceed subliminally, while their chaining requires consciousperception. Here we use the semantic pointer architecture to model a global workspace and specialist processors withspiking neurons. Non-conscious information has limited spatio-temporal influence in our model, while information that isselected to enter the global workspace can be maintained over time and selectively routed to the processors whose role isto execute the operations. The model can perform three tasks that consist of different operation chains. Response timesand accuracy are compared to human performance data.

Effects of Voiced Initial Consonants in Japanese Sound-Symbolic Words:Experiments 1 and 2

Theoretical linguists have hypothesized that the vocalization of the initial consonants in Japanese sound-symbolic wordsaffect their psychological evaluations. By using 5-point semantic differential scales associated with 13 psycholinguis-tic features (familiarity, visual imagery, auditory imagery, haptic imagery, arousal, preference, disgust, hardness, soft-ness, heaviness, lightness, fastness, and slowness), we asked 36 Japanese participants to evaluate sound-symbolic wordswith voiceless (SSWVL; e.g., kirakira) or voiced initial consonants (SSWV; e.g., giragira) in experiment 1, whereas weasked them to evaluate sound-symbolic words with semi-voiced consonants (SSWSV; e.g., pochapocha) or SSWV (e.g.,bochabocha) in experiment 2. Results of experiments 1 and 2 showed that the participants had higher levels of disgust,arousal, hardness, heaviness, and slowness for SSWV as opposed to SSWVL and SSWSV (ps ¡ .05). In sum, these find-ings suggest that the presence of vocalization of initial consonants in Japanese sound-symbolic words contribute to theirpsychological evaluations to sound-symbolic words.

What remains of ”belief bias” once we generalise logic to probabilities?

A key phenomenon in the psychology of reasoning is belief bias, a tendency to accept the conclusion of an argument basedon whether it is believable, regardless of logical status. The traditional notion of belief bias assumes a contrast betweenlogic and beliefs: we are either logical, or we are biased away from logic by our beliefs. But this contrast is unnecessaryin probabilistic theories of reasoning that generalise logic to cover uncertain degrees of belief. An experiment examinedwhether reasoners inferences about conditional syllogisms conform to principles of probabilistic coherence and whetherthis was affected by the believability of argument premises. Inferences for a majority of syllogisms showed above-chancecoherence regardless of the believability of argument premises. When deviations from coherence did occur these mostoften reflected underconfidence in arguments with unbelievable premises. These results show that positing two distinctreasoning processes is not necessary to explain belief bias.

fMTP: A Unifying Computational Framework of Temporal Preparation acrossTime Scales

Temporal preparation is influenced by factors across a range of time scales, from effects of the previous trial to learningeffects throughout entire experiments. Theories on temporal preparation thus far have failed to offer a complete account ofthese effects. We present the formal multiple trace theory of temporal preparation (fMTP), a computational framework thatintegrates theories on time perception, motor planning, and associative learning. At fMTP’s core lies Hebbian, associativelearning between a layer of time cells and a motor layer. Its preparatory state is governed by the automatically retrieval oftraces formed in the past. We show that fMTP, with only this single implicit learning mechanism, accounts for behavioralphenomena across a range of time scales that previously have been considered to be the result of distinct processes.Furthermore, for experimental setups where the predictions of existing accounts and fMTP differ, the data aligns with ourmodel.

Audiovisual Information Processing in Emotion Recognition: An Eye Tracking Study

In audiovisual information processing, auditory information may interfere with eye movement planning in visual processing due to competition for attentional resources. Here we hypothesize that this interference may be mitigated in the recognition of emotions involving strong audiovisual coupling. Participants judged the emotion of a talking head video under audiovisual, video-only, and audio-only conditions. While participants generally performed the best in the audiovisual condition, their eye movement pattern did not change significantly across the three conditions except for the recognition of disgust. In disgust recognition, eye movements in the audiovisual condition were less eyes-focused than the video-only condition, and the larger the difference, the less the audiovisual advantage in performance. Disgust recognition develops later in life and may involve weaker audiovisual coupling. Accordingly, our results suggest that whether emotional voice information facilitates emotion recognition without interfering with eye movement planning depends on the strength of audiovisual coupling in emotion processing.

Learning sequential patterns from graphical programs

How do people learn complex rules? We introduce a novel paradigm called ”Track-A-Mole”, in which participants have tolearn about and predict the moves of a cartoon mole, whose movements are generated by graphical programs. Our resultsshow that participants can learn to predict richly structured programs, and often require only few observations to do so,showing rapid learning and early insights about the underlying patterns. Moreover, we found that how learnable a programis can be predicted by features related to its complexity and compressibility. Finally, participants also show interestingpatterns of generalizations, assuming more parsimonious rules first and then gradually adjusting their predictions to morecomplex regularities, as well as matching their predictions to the general direction of movements and producing sensi-ble errors. These results extend our understanding of complex rule learning and open up future opportunities to modelsequential pattern predictions as graphical program induction.

Experienced effort depends on evaluation mode

Our understanding of effort perception is limited. Performance (e.g., response time; accuracy) is typically used as one wayto assess effort in cognitive tasks; however, performance can be readily dissociated from subjective ratings of effort. Onepotential contribution to effort ratings that could lead to such dissociations is the judgment context. We tested this notionusing a recently reported dissociation between performance and subjective effort in combination with a manipulation ofevaluation mode (i.e., joint versus separate evaluation). Participants were asked to silently read a display of words asquickly as possible, then provide the level of effort experienced. Results demonstrate that evaluation mode can have amarked effect on retrospective judgments of effort. Implications are discussed.

The Influence of Negated Causal Information on Pronoun Disambiguation

The disambiguation of pronouns is a complicated process that has been shown to be influenced by both linguistic andcognitive factors. In particular, readers prefer an interpretation that is causally likely. For example, in the sentence pairJohn accused Mark of stealing a car. He called the police, readers judge that the antecedent of he is more likely to beJohn than Mark because of the perceived causal link between the accusation and calling the police. I will describe newresults that explore how the presence of negation affects such interpretations (e.g., He did not call the police). While,as expected, negation disrupts the perceived causal link, this disruption does not affect the choice of antecedent (John isstill the preferred antecedent). This suggests that readers identify the unnegated causal relationship when interpreting thenegated sentence. The implication of this result to models of pronoun disambiguation will be discussed.

The surprising consequences of engaging in contrastive explanation

When we explain a fact or event, we typically contrast it with a specific set of counterfactual alternatives. For example, anexplanation of why Alex (as opposed to somebody else) ate the cake will seek to identify relevant factors that vary acrossagents, rather than across food items. The contrastive nature of explanation has been widely appreciated, but its cognitiveconsequence have not. We report a study with 340 adults examining how commitment to a particular explanatory contrast(agent- or patient-based) affects discovery of noisy patterns. Maximum predictive accuracy could be achieved by detectingpredictive regularities along multiple dimensions. We found that engaging in contrastive explanation (committing to aparticular contrast) impeded the discovery of alternative patterns that predicted the outcome. While explaining is likely tobe beneficial in many contexts, seeking an explanation with a single contrast could interfere with peoples ability to identifyreal structure in the world.

The development of accent-based friendship preferences: Age and languageexposure matter

Previous research has shown that children exhibit strong,language-based social biases, preferring speakers of theirlocally dominant accent over foreign language or foreign-accented speakers. Even when regularly exposed to multiplelanguages or to speakers with non-local accents, elementaryschool-aged children nevertheless display strong languagebiases, preferring to be friends with native speakers over non-native speakers. The present study revisited this issue,examining whether routine exposure to additional languagesand/or non-local accents influences language-based friendpreferences. Three- to 5-year-old children (N = 183) growingup in a large, multicultural, North American City with at least70% English exposure were presented with pairs of children—one speaking native-accented English and the other speakingforeign-accented English—and were asked to choose whomthey wanted to be friends with. While accent exposure was notfound to predict children’s preference, there was a significanteffect of language exposure, such that greater experience withmultiple languages reduced biases for native-accentedspeakers.

A theory of bouletic reasoning

No present theory explains or models the inferences peopledraw about the real world when reasoning about “bouletic”relations, i.e., predicates that express desires, such as want inLee wants to be in love. Linguistic accounts of such bouleticrelations define them in terms of their relation to a desirer’sbeliefs, and how its complement is deemed desirable (cf.Heim, 1992; Villalta, 2008; Rubinstein 2012). In contrast, wedescribe a new model-based theory (cf. Johnson-Laird, 2006;Khemlani, Byrne, & Johnson-Laird, 2018) that posits that suchpredicates are fundamentally counterfactual in nature. Inparticular, X wants P should imply that P is not the case,because you cannot want what is already true. The theorymakes empirical predictions about how people assess theconsistency of bouletic relations as well as how they use suchrelations to eliminate disjunctive possibilities. Twoexperiments tested and validated the theory’s centralpredictions. We assess the theory in light of alternativeaccounts of human reasoning.

A Mechanistic Account of Model-Free / Model-Based Trade-off and its ChangeAcross Development

The joint recruitment of two systems (habitual and goal-directed) for the control of behaviour has provoked wide interestin the last decades. The systems relative contributions have been quantified through a standard two-stage task and byapplying reinforcement learning (model-free/model-based), but less is known about the processes behind their integration.We address this with an interactive activation model of the standard task in which the two systems activate, to varyingdegrees, the potential responses. The model is able to capture the behavioural patterns characterizing the trade-off betweenthe two systems. Additionally, the model is able to simulate response times because activations vary over time within atrial. We explore three mechanistic hypotheses of the trade-off related to developmental data from childhood to adulthood.We argue that process-level models such as ours are needed, conjointly with new empirical tasks, to further understandchanges in the control of action selection across development.

You’re surprised at her success?Inferring competence from emotional responses to performance outcomes

How do we learn about who is good at what? Others’ compe-tence is unobservable and often must be inferred from observ-able evidence, such as failures and successes. However, eventhe same performance can indicate different levels of compe-tence depending on the context, and objective evaluation met-rics are not always available. Building on recent advances onchildren’s use of emotion as information, here we ask whetherexpressions of surprise inform inferences about competence.Participants saw scenarios (sports, academics) where two stu-dents achieved identical outcomes but a teacher showed sur-prise to one student and no surprise to the other. In Exp.1,adults inferred that the successful student who elicited theteacher’s surprise was less competent than the other student,but this pattern reversed when both students failed. Exp.2 (4-9-year-olds) finds initial evidence for such inferences in school-aged children. These findings have implications for promotinghealthy social comparisons and preventing acquisition of neg-ative stereotypes from non-verbal cues.

The Emergence and Propagation of Online Slang

Slang is a common socio-linguistic phenomenon, but how slang emerges and propagates is poorly understood. We explorethis problem by analyzing longitudinal data from 1,000 Reddit communities over the past decade. We consider socialand linguistic factors pertaining to the emergence and propagation of recently emerged online slang. We show that whilelinguistic factors can be relevant, social factors play a more important role in predicting the emergence and propagation ofonline slang. We find community size to be the dominant factor in the emergence of novel slang terms and user mobilityto be the most critical factor in the widespread propagation of slang.

Machine Learning Optimizes Assessment: New Insights for the Development ofNumerosity Estimation

In a conventional number-line task, a given number that varies every trial is estimated on a line flanked with 0 and anupper-bound number. An upper-bound number is often arbitrarily selected, although this design variable has been shownto affect non-linearity in estimates. Examining estimates of varying given numbers (design variable 1) with varying upper-bound numbers (design variable 2) can be costly because adding another design variable into the task drastically increasesthe number of trials required to examine the numerical representation. In the present study, a novel Bayesian machinelearning algorithm, dubbed Gaussian Process Active Learning (GPAL), was used to make this costly paradigm feasible bypresenting only the most informative combinations of the design variables every trial. We found that children were morelogarithmic than adults across upper bounds, replicating log-to-linear shifts in development. More importantly, childrenand even educated adults became more logarithmic as the upper bound increased, indicating the persistent use of logrepresentation across age groups.

Relationship between Social Support and Posttraumatic Growth for KoreanFirefighters

Firefighters are exposed to elevated levels of potentially traumatizing events through the course of their work. Suchexposure can have lasting negative consequences (e.g., posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)) and/or positive outcomes(e.g., posttraumatic growth (PTG)). Research had implicated trauma, occupational and personal variables that account forvariance in posttrauma outcomes yet at this stage no research has investigated these factors and their relative influenceon both PTSD and PTG in a single study. Based in Calhoun and Tedeschi’s model of PTG and previous research, in thisstudy regression models of PTG and PTSD symptoms among 610 firefighters were tested. Results indicated organisationalfactors predicted symptoms of PTSD, while there was partial support for the hypothesis that coping and social supportwould be predictors of PTG. Increases in PTG were predicted by experiencing trauma from multiple sources and the useof selfcare coping.

Are content effects out of sight? An eye-tracking study of arithmetic problem solving

Evidence suggests that general, non-mathematical knowledge about the entities described in an arithmetic word problem may interfere with its encoding. We used behavioral and eye- tracking measures to investigate how the use of specific quantities may foster a cardinal representation of the numbers mentioned in a problem, whereas other quantities may favor an ordinal representation instead. We asked 50 pre-service teachers to complete a solution validity assessment task. We compared participants’ gaze patterns on isomorphic problems to gather insights into their encoded representations. On problems featuring cardinal quantities, we found that specific sentences describing elements relevant in a cardinal understanding of the problems but irrelevant otherwise were looked at longer and were the focus of a higher number of backward eye movements. Additionally, an increase in pupil dilation on correctly solved cardinal problems supported the idea that participants need to engage in a recoding process when facing semantic incongruence.

Interpersonal physiological linkage is related to excitement during a joint task

Interpersonal physiological linkage has been shown to play important roles in social activities. Studies have shown thatpeople tend to share heart rate (HR) dynamics through a joint collaborative task. In this study, we investigated whethershared HR dynamics (i.e., HR synchrony) would correlate with excitement during a joint task. Two participants played acollaborative block-stacking game (Jenga), alternating their roles as player and adviser, while their HRs being recorded.The participants evaluated their own excitement for each turn. Additional bystanders watched their playing to evaluatethe players excitement. The results showed that the players excitement increased with individual HR but also with HRsynchrony. HR synchrony also affected the evaluation of players excitement by the bystanders. These results suggestthat physiological linkage between cooperating individuals is related to the evaluation of excitement not only by playerthemselves but also by bystanders.

Trust-Related Heuristics and Biases: How Do We Trust Healthcare Systems?

An online questionnaire attempted to reveal the heuristics and biases used when participants reflected on their trust ina healthcare system. Participants answered quantitative questions related to six different heuristics and biases, whichrevealed their propensity for exhibiting each heuristic, before rating healthcare systems on seven trust-related metrics: re-source allocation; access to treatment; honesty, integrity and intention; competence; quality; safety; and equality. Multipleregressions tested whether the predictive power of heuristics and biases on trust ratings was significantly moderated bythe relative proportion of patients receiving service in the public and private sector. Results revealed that heuristics andbiases significantly impacted thought processes when arriving at assessments of participants willingness to trust. As trustin major institutions declines, this presents the scientific and medical communities with relevant data to potentially alterpractices and communication approaches in a way that fosters trust.

Infants inferences about insides reveal parallel causal representations

Work on the origin of causal thought has always proposed that there is one ”original” causal representation, and overdevelopment this causal representation is applied to understanding different events. We propose that there are in factmultiple independent causal primitives, which must be integrated at some later point in development. In three experiments,we provide the first evidence that infants have multiple ways of representing cause and effect, that are fully dissociatedfrom each other in the first year of life. At 10 months, infants represent ”launching” events (Newtonian elastic collisions)as causal, in that they track which of two arbitrary objects is causing the other to move. They make inferences aboutwhether objects have an internal source of motion based on entraining events (in which A collides with B and remains incontact with it as they moves together). Critically, each representation lacks the signatures of the other.

Sensitivity to Ostension is Not Sufficient for Pedagogical Reasoning by Toddlers

To investigate the role of ostensive cues in pedagogical reasoning, we explored whether toddlers, like preschoolers, would copy causally implausible actions following a pedagogical demonstration. Toddlers watched a demonstrator perform a two-action sequence (AB) on a puzzle-box that led to a reward. We manipulated the demonstrator’s intentionality and the causal plausibility of action A and examined how these factors influenced copying behavior. Although toddlers were more likely to copy A when it was causally plausible, they were not influenced by the demonstrator’s intentionality. Importantly, toddlers were no more likely to copy the AB sequence following a pedagogical demonstration vs. a non- communicative demonstration. Comparing behavioural data to computational model predictions for learners differing in their sensitivity to intentionality and causal plausibility supported an absence of pedagogical reasoning. These results suggest that sensitivity to ostension may be a necessary prerequisite—but is not sufficient for—pedagogical reasoning in a causal imitation task.

The representation of recursive center-embedded and cross-serial sequences inchildren and adults

The ability to represent recursive structures is thought to be foundational for language, music, mathematics, complex tooluse, and theory of mind. However, we do not currently know what type of computational machinery is used to representrecursive structures, or when this ability develops. Here we measure the developmental trajectory in young childrenusing a sequence generation task. We also test two proposed mechanisms for representing these structures: a stack-likedata structure a first-in-last-out structure in which only the last item can be accessed, and a queue-like data structurean ordered list that can only be accessed from its beginning. Each of these mechanisms make different predictions forwhat types of sequential structures should be easier to generate and have specific item-by-item response time signatures.We show evidence that both children and adults use a queue-like representational system which iteratively runs forwardssearches through a stored queue.

Stereotypes Decrease Childrens Tendency to Acknowledge Constraints on Choice

Prior research has documented childrens recognition that a choice made when constrained to a single option is a poorindicator of anothers preference. The present study (N = 246; 5 to 10 years) examined childrens tendency to make thisinference in stereotypical contexts (e.g., a girl playing with a doll). Because stereotypes provide powerful explanatoryframeworks (e.g., girls inherently like dolls), children may discount constraints and infer that constrained and uncon-strained stereotypical choices are both evidence of a preference. The majority of children discounted constraints in thisway. However, while younger children (5 to 6 years) tended to discount constraints similarly across both stereotypicaland gender-neutral choices, older children (9 to 10 years) were more likely to discount constraints when reasoning aboutstereotypical choices. We also report evidence that, overall, childrens acknowledgment of environmental constraints maynot be as robust as previously documented.

When in Rome, do as Bayesians do: Statistical learning and parochial norms

It’s a familiar point in anthropology that many norms are parochial, meaning they apply to people in certain groups (e.g., one’s ingroup) and not to others (e.g., one’s outgroup). One explanation for such parochialism is that people are just innately biased against outsiders. But it’s also possible that, given the evidence, people infer the parochiality of norms in statistically appropriate ways. This paper uses a Bayesian learning framework to investigate inferences of normative scope both experimentally and computationally. An experiment in which adult participants (n = 480) viewed sample violations of a novel rule among novel groups reveals that both sensitivity to statistical evidence and prior knowledge of relevant social categories are integral to computations of normative scope. In tandem with the experimental results, computational analysis supports the notion that degree of prior inclusivity bias (i.e., an expectation that a norm will be broad, rather than narrow, in scope) is another key factor. Together, these novel insights raise intriguing possibilities for integrating perspectives on norms research.

Mental inference: Mind perception as Bayesian model selection

Beyond an ability to represent other people’s mental states,people can also represent different types of minds, like those ofnewborn babies, pets, and even wildlife that we rarely interactwith. While past research has shown that people have a nu-anced understanding of how minds vary, little is known abouthow we infer what kind of mind different agents have. Here wepresent a computational model of mind attribution as Bayesianinference over a space of generative models. We tested ourmodel in a simple experiment where participants watched shortvideos in the style of Heider & Simmel, 1944, and had to in-fer the representations in the agent’s mind. We find that, fromjust a few seconds, people can make accurate inferences aboutagents’ mental capacities, suggesting that people can quicklyinfer an agent’s type of mind, based on how they interact withthe world and with others.

Culturally-Constructed Beliefs About Physical and Mental Illness

We explored Asian- and Caucasian-American adults beliefs about illness, investigating whether conceptions of mentaland physical illness reflect the Western biomedical framework and an energy-healing practice grounded in traditionalChinese medicine. For physical illnesses (i.e., cold/flu and cancer), White young adults primarily cited biomedical causes,while Asian young adults and older energy believers often cited alternative causes, X2(4, N=27)=19.06, p¡.01. Whenasked about treatment and prevention, the energy believers continued to endorse alternative approaches, but both whiteand Asian young adults focused on biomedical approaches, X2(4, N=27)¿22.99, ps¡.0001. For mental illnesses (i.e.,depression and anxiety), the energy believers continued to endorse the alternative framework, while White and Asianyoung adults responses were more distributed between biomedical and alternative methods. These results suggest thatmental models of illness are shaped by cultural beliefs, and conflicting beliefs may coexist within young adults who arebeing enculturated in a new framework.

The Scaled Target Learning Model: A Novel Computational Model of the BalloonAnalogue Risk Task

The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) is a sequential decision making paradigm that assesses risk-taking behavior.Several computational models have been proposed for the BART that accurately characterize risk-taking propensity. Anaspect of task performance that has proven challenging to model is the learning that develops from experiencing winsand losses across trials, which has the potential to provide further insight into risky decision making. The Scaled TargetLearning (STL) model was developed for this purpose. STL describes learning as adjustments to the pumping strategyin reaction to previous outcomes, and the size of adjustments reflects an individuals sensitivity to wins and losses. STLis shown to be sensitive to the learning elicited by experimental manipulations. In addition, the model matches or beststhe performance of three competing models in traditional model comparison tests (e.g., parameter recovery performance,predictive accuracy, sensitivity to risk-taking propensity). Findings are discussed in the context of the learning processinvolved in the task. By characterizing the extent to which people are willing to adapt their strategies based on pastexperience, STL provides a more complete depiction of the psychological processes underlying sequential risk-takingbehavior.

a process model of procrastination

Procrastination is prevalent. Empirical studies of procrastination have identified various contributing factors underlyingprocrastination. Models of procrastination, however, have only considered temporal discounting and have ignored otherfactors. Moreover, existing models of procrastination are mostly conceptual, and there is a lack of process models toexplain why people procrastinate. Here, we use reinforcement learning theory to build a process model of procrastination.The model assumes that people maximize expected utility while minimizing the total cost of the effort. Our model makesseveral predictions: 1. Strong temporal discounters will delay working early and rush to work near the deadline; 2. If atlow effort cost, cost is sensitive to increases in effort, people will delay working until the last minute; 3. If time pressure oreffort cost is high, perfectionists will not work at all. We designed a behavioral experiment to study the factors underlyingprocrastination and to test our model predictions.

Inducing preference reversals by manipulating revealed preferences

It is currently difficult to test the validity of existing explana-tions for the emergence of context-dependent preference rever-sals. This is because these explanations are generally placed atthe level of the process of evidence accumulation, and acrossexperimental paradigms, this process is unobservable. In thispaper, we propose a new experimental paradigm for elicitingpreference reversals, wherein the process of evidence accumu-lation is significantly observable. Over a series of experiments,we successfully induce preference reversals for arbitrary stim-uli by showing participants sequences of stimuli comparisonswith pre-determined outcomes. Our findings partially supportthe view that context-sensitive assimilation of a history of ordi-nal comparisons is sufficient to explain classic context effects.

Commonality Search as a Way of Facilitating Creative Thinking: A Comparison with the Alternative Categorization Task

The purpose of this study was to clarify the cognitive processes of commonality search between unrelated objects. Specifically, we investigated the relationship between the performance of the commonality search task and that of the alternative categorization task. We hypothesized that one needs to focus on obscure features of objects to do both tasks well and that there would therefore be a positive correlation between the performances on the two tasks. We also compared the performance of the commonality search task with that of the alternative categorization to investigate exploratorily how each task promotes creative thinking. Thirty-one participants were asked to engage in two tasks: the commonality search task and the alternative categorization task. In the commonality search task, they were asked to list as many commonalities as possible between nine unrelated object pairs within 90 seconds for each pair. In the alternative categorization task, they were asked to list as many categories as possible to which each of the five objects belonged, within 60 seconds for each object. Although There was a significant positive correlation between the numbers of answers on these tasks. The additional results showed that there was no significant difference between the two tasks in terms of average saliency score or the first answer, but the saliency of the commonality search task was significant lower than the alternative categorization task in the second answer. We discussed the similarities and differences between the two tasks and the potential use of the commonality search task as a way to promote creative thinking.

Differences in Implicit vs. Explicit Grammar Processing as Revealed byHierarchical Weibull Modeling of Reaction Times

Artificial language studies using reaction time-based measures have suggested grammar learning even in participants with-out awareness of underlying grammatical rules (Leung & Williams, 2011; Batterink, Reber, & Paller, 2014). However,traditional linear analyses of reaction times might not capture qualitative differences between participants with/withoutconscious rule awareness (Rouder, Lu, Speckman, Sun & Jiang, 2005; Rousselet & Wilcox, in press). In a partial repli-cation of one study (Batterink et al., 2014), participants were exposed to pseudoword articles that were predictive of anaccompanying English noun’s living/non-living status. Linear analyses showed that both rule-aware and rule-unaware par-ticipants exhibited slowdowns to rule-violating trials, indicating grammar learning. Hierarchical Weibull distribution anal-yses suggested that rule-unaware and rule-aware participants differed in the underlying cognitive mechanisms involved:rule-violating trials affected the processing architecture for both groups but only affected processing speed for rule-awareparticipants. These results illustrate the potential of yet-underused distribution-modeling approaches for second languagepsycholinguistics.

Risk preferences in option generation: Do risk-takers generate more risky coursesof action?

Decision making research typically focuses on choices between predetermined sets of options. In many real-world de-cisions, however, individuals must generate potential courses of action themselves. Individual differences in processesinvolved in option generation therefore influence which actions are considered. We examined the role of one such fac-tor: the propensity to take risks. We hypothesized that risk-taking propensity would be related to the generation of morerisky actions associated with uncertain or unfavorable outcomes. Participants generated options in ill-structured situationsand rated the perceived risk associated with each option. As predicted, higher risk-taking propensity was associated withincreased generation of risky options that could lead to unfavorable outcomes. The riskiness of generated options wasalso related to affective state, consistent with prior evidence of emotional influences on risky decision making. The find-ings suggest that both real-life risk-taking and risky option generation arise from common cognitive processes involved inresponding to uncertainty.

Limited Domain Structure for Conjunction Errors

People make conjunction errors, rating a conjunction as morelikely than one of its constituents, across many different typesof problems. They commit the conjunction fallacy in problemsof social judgment, in physical reasoning tasks, and in gam-bles of pure chance. Doctors commit the fallacy when mak-ing judgments about hypothetical patients. Do all these errorsshare an underlying cause? Or does the fallacy arise indepen-dently in different types of reasoning? In a series of studies, welook for structure in conjunction errors across various types ofproblems. We find that error magnitudes are related for someclusters of items, but there does not appear to be a universalrelationship between all cases of this fallacy.

’Eye Can Reason’- How Eye Parameters Marked one’s Performance in a VisualReasoning Task

Eye tracking systems have the potential of providing efficient, non-intrusive solutions towards the study of human be-haviour. This work shows that eye movements may be markers of visual information processing and hence can provideinsights into a persons cognitive problem-solving ability and reasoning behaviour. We studied the relationship betweenperformance and eye parameters of individuals for a visual reasoning based problem-solving task. Inter-group analysesrevealed fixation duration and peak saccadic velocity as differentiating markers of performance and time. Intra-groupstudies indicated that the eye parameters acting as performance markers were not the same for all performance groups. Aseparate marker of ’Visual to Textual Processing Ratio’ was defined. Correlating eye parameters with performance couldhelp us develop eye metrics to better mark the cognitive information processing of a person through tests even whereperformance parameters (like score) are not defined.

Categorical perception as inference under uncertainty: New evidence from color

The category adjustment model of Huttenlocher, Hedges, and colleagues explains category effects on memory or percep-tion in terms of probabilistic inference. This model has been shown to account for category effects in color cognitionacross several languages, suggesting that effects of language on color cognition reflect standard principles of inferenceunder uncertainty. Previously unexamined is whether the same model can illuminate an influential intuition advanced byKay and Kempton: that language is likely to affect cognition primarily when purely perceptual discrimination of stimuli isdifficult because the stimuli are similar. Recent data by Welch et al. support this intuition. Here, we show that the categoryadjustment model accounts for these new data as well, strengthening the case for viewing category effects of language oncognition through the lens of probabilistic inference.

Effects of domain size during reference production in photo-realistic scenes

The current study investigates how speakers are affected by thesize of the visual domain during reference production. Previousresearch found that speech onset times increase along with thenumber of distractors that are visible, at least when speakersrefer to non-salient target objects in simplified visual domains.This suggests that in the case of more distractors, speakers needmore time to perform an object-by-object scan of all distractorsthat are visible. We present the results of a reference productionexperiment, to study if this pattern for speech onset times holdsfor photo-realistic scenes, and to test if the suggested viewingstrategy is reflected directly in speakers’ eye movements. Ourresults show that this is indeed the case: we find (1) that speechonset times increase linearly as more distractors are present; (2)that speakers fixate the target relatively less often in larger do-mains; and (3) that larger domains elicit more fixation switchesback and forth between the target and its distractors.

What Gives a Diagnostic Label Value? Common Use Over Informativeness

A labels entrenchment, its degree of use by members of a community, affects its perceived explanatory value even ifthe label provides no substantive information (Hemmatian & Sloman, 2018). Here we show that entrenched psychiatricand non-psychiatric diagnostic labels are seen by laypersons and mental health professionals as better explanations evenif circular. This preference is not attributable to conversational norms, reflectiveness or attentiveness, and the recipientsunfamiliarity with the label. In Experiment 1, whether a label provided novel symptom information had no impact onlaypersons’ responses, while its entrenchment enhanced ratings of explanation quality. The effect persisted in Experiment2 for incoherent random categories and regardless of provided mechanistic information. The entrenchment manipula-tion induced causal beliefs about the category even when respondents were informed that no causal relation exists. Wereplicate the effect in Experiment 3 with mental health professionals despite a marked tendency to find all uninformativeexplanations unsatisfactory.

Color Categorization and Naming in Normal, Deficient, and Mixed PopulationsUsing Agent Based Modelling

Humans make sense of the world by compressing and classifying perceptual information into discrete linguistic categories.A major consideration in linguistic categorization is that humans being social and cultural creatures have categories thatare not just consistent internally, but across a linguistic community. Color naming represents an exemplary problem incognitive science because of the unique interplay between perception, conceptualization, and language. In this study,we use an agent-based model to explore the link between perception and language in the context of color vision and itsvariations. Colorblindness is a congenital disorder that alters the color experience of those affected. Using a definitiveidentifier of colorblindness, the Just Noticeable Difference curve, we show that color vision deficiencies lead to impairedperceptual and linguistic categorization, without significant impact on social communication. The results provide insightsinto the color experience of the colorblind and how they cope with the language of color.

Sheer Time Spent Expecting or Maintaining a Representation FacilitatesSubsequent Retrieval during Sentence Processing

Previous research has shown that modified noun phrases(henceforth NPs) are subsequently retrieved faster thanunmodified NPs. This effect is often called the “semanticcomplexity effect”. However, little is known about itsmechanisms and underlying factors. In this study, we testedwhether this effect is truly caused by the semantic informationadded by the modification, or whether it can be explained bythe sheer amount of time that the processor spends expectingor maintaining an NP in the encoding phase. The resultsshowed that time spent expecting or maintaining an NP canexplain the effect over and above semantic and/or syntacticcomplexity. Our results challenge the current memory-basedmechanisms for the modification effect such as the“distinctiveness” and “head-reactivation” accounts, and offernew and valuable insight into the memory processes duringsentence comprehension.

Modelling Perceptual Effects of Phonology with ASR Systems

This paper explores the minimal knowledge a listener needs tocompensate for phonological assimilation, one kind of phono-logical process responsible for variation in speech. We usedstandard automatic speech recognition models to represent En-glish and French listeners. We found that, first, some typesof models show language-specific assimilation patterns com-parable to those shown by human listeners. Like English lis-teners, when trained on English, the models compensate morefor place assimilation than for voicing assimilation, and likeFrench listeners, the models show the opposite pattern whentrained on French. Second, the models which best predict thehuman pattern use contextually-sensitive acoustic models andlanguage models, which capture allophony and phonotactics,but do not make use of higher-level knowledge of a lexiconor word boundaries. Finally, some models overcompensate forassimilation, showing a (super-human) ability to recover theunderlying form even in the absence of the triggering phono-logical context, pointing to an incomplete neutralization notexploited by human listeners.

Leftward Visuospatial Bias Predicts Childrens Reading Fluency

Neurotypical children have been shown to display a leftward visuospatial attention bias while children with dyslexia(i.e., children with a reading disorder characterized by slow and/or inaccurate word recognition) have been shown todisplay a relatively rightward visuospatial attention bias. Researchers have speculated that leftward bias in young childrenmay be driven by their beginning reading education in languages read from left to right. Here, we investigated whetherspatial bias may be related to the acquisition of reading skills among a sample of children in grades 1 to 3. We assessedthe relationship between spatial bias (measured using the landmark task) and performance on (1) a rapid automatizednaming test (a predictor of reading fluency) and (2) a word-identification test. We found that leftward bias predicts rapidautomatized naming but not word identification. This finding has implications for understanding the potential role ofspatial bias in reading and dyslexia.

Impact of sleep deprivation on EEG markers of emotion regulation in young adults

Sleep deprivation (SD) has negative effects on emotional regulation, but few studies have evaluated electroencephalo-graphic (EEG) indices and none of these have used a within-subject design. Twenty-nine participants (17 female) com-pleted a repeated-measures study protocol involving a night of normal sleep (NS) and a night of SD, followed by resting-state EEG during the following morning. Established EEG indices of emotion regulation, frontal alpha asymmetry (FAS)and slow wave/fast wave (SW/FW) ratio in frontal sites (F3, F4, Fz), were investigated. Our results did not reveal SD ef-fects in FAS (t28= -.960, p = .345) or in SW/FW ratio (t28= 0.737, p = 0.467). Although other studies have demonstratedemotional dysregulation after SD, two well-studied EEG markers of emotional dysregulation did not reflect altered emo-tional states after SD in the current within-subject study. Future studies combining EEG and other indices of emotionalregulation may help elucidate these results.

Interaction with Context During Recurrent Neural Network Sentence Processing

Syntactic ambiguities in isolated sentences can lead to in-creased difficulty in incremental sentence processing, a phe-nomenon known as a garden-path effect. This difficulty, how-ever, can be alleviated for humans when they are presentedwith supporting discourse contexts. We tested whether re-current neural network (RNN) language models (LMs) couldlearn linguistic representations that are similarly influenced bydiscourse context. RNN LMs have been claimed to learn avariety of syntactic constructions. However, recent work hassuggested that pragmatically conditioned syntactic phenomenaare not acquired by RNNs. In comparing model behavior tohuman behavior, we show that our models can, in fact, learnpragmatic constraints that alleviate garden-path effects giventhe correct training and testing conditions. This suggests thatsome aspects of linguistically relevant pragmatic knowledgecan be learned from distributional information alone.

Computational cognitive requirements of random decision problems

Previous studies have found that for electronic computers the computational requirements of solving an instance of aproblem are related to a specific set of features of the problem. This mapping has been shown to apply to electroniccomputers on a multitude of problems and is referred to as Instance Complexity (IC). However, it remains an open questionwhether IC applies to humans. For this purpose, we ran a set of experiments in which human participants solved a setof instances of one of three, widely studied, computational problems (Knapsack, Traveling Salesperson and the BooleanSatisfiability). We found that, in line with our hypothesis, IC had a negative effect on human performance in all problems.Our results suggest that IC can be used as a generalisable measure of the computational resource requirements of a task.Moreover, given its properties, IC could serve a crucial role in the cognitive resource allocation process.

Entropy of Sounds: Sonnets to Battle Rap

Poetry and lyrics across cultures, from Sonnets to Rap, demon-strate an obvious human cognitive capacity for the perceptionand production of various multi-syllable sound patterns. Herewe use entropy to measure discrete serialized representationsof phones and to explore the complexity of these sound struc-tures across genres of creative language arts. The present ex-ploratory analysis has two main objectives. First, our aim isto broaden the scope of cognitive processes and data that areconsidered in statistical learning approaches to phonologicallearning and language acquisition. Second, we hope to to pro-vide a basis for more targeted computational and phonologicalinvestigations of these patterns. We compare the conditionalentropy of sequences of phonological patterns in lyrics and findthat, in general, Battle Rap and Sonnets maintain noticeablylower entropy than other genres across sequence sizes, whilelyrics from Electronic music and Hip-Hop display relativelyhigh entropy.

Gesture Production and Theory of Mind:Effective Disambiguation in Communication through Gesture

People design their speech acts with their listeners in mind,accounting for their knowledge and other mental states. Is thisability specific to spoken language and co-speech gesture, ordoes it appear in pantomimic gestures as well? We ask whetheradults flexibly shift their silent gestures to emphasize relevantinformation, representing different features of the target indifferent contexts. In a two-item reference game, adultsgestured to a partner to indicate which object was the target.Item pairs differed in one of three features (size, shape,pattern). We found that adults were more likely to gesture afeature when it was relevant to distinguishing the two possiblereferents, versus when it was not. Thus, adults flexiblymodified their gestures to meet their partners’ needs,emphasizing the relevant feature. These data lay a foundationfor future work on the development of use of theory of mind ingestural communication in childhood.

The Effect of Document Structure on Non-Native Readers in Web DocumentReading for Information Acquisition

While it is known that document design affects the reading process (Schriver, 1997), there are few studies on how designelements influence non-native readers’ reading. We conducted a study to examine how native and non-native (NN) readersread Japanese web documents with different structures (networked, hierarchical, and relational) using eye-tracking andhow differences in reading affect information lookup and comprehension evaluated by performance and comprehensiontests. We used municipal documents currently made available on the Web by local governments in Japan. Seven nativeand eight NN Japanese readers took part in the study. The results show that native readers are not influenced by differencesin document structure. NN readers, on the other hand, showed different patterns of reading depending on the documentstructure, and better information look up performance when they read documents with a relational structure. This seemsto be related to the amount of information available.

Effects of Coordination on Perspective-taking: Evidence from Eye-tracking

We investigated whether fine-grained coordination in a screen-based puzzle task with a (virtual) partner would influence on-line perspective-taking. Participants played a screen-basedpuzzle game with a computer player. In the high-coordinationcondition, the player presented participants with puzzle piecesthat could be placed near their partner’s last piece. In the low-coordination condition, pieces could only be placed furtheraway from their partner’s last piece. Participant’s eyemovements were then measured in a referential communicationtask, with the partner giving the instructions, and whetherpossible competitor referents were in shared or privilegedground. The results demonstrate clear effects of ground andcoordination. Participants in both coordination groups weresensitive to the perspective of the interlocutor. In addition,participants in the high-level coordination condition were moresensitive to statistical regularities in the input and theircomprehension was more time-locked to the utterance of thespeaker.

Differential Modulation Effects of Music Expertise on English and ChineseSentence Reading

Here we tested the hypothesis that music expertise modulates different aspects of language processing across differentlanguages, depending on the similarities of the cognitive processes involved. Chinese-English bilingual musicians and non-musicians read legal and semantically/syntactically incorrect sentences in both English and Chinese. In English reading,musicians showed higher sensitivity to linguistic irregularities than non-musicians as reflected in longer reading time andmore dispersed eye movements when reading semantically/syntactically incorrect than legal sentences. In Chinese reading,musicians higher sensitivity was reflected only in reading time but not in eye movement behavior. Thus, music expertisemodulated linguistic regularity processing in both English and Chinese reading, but modulated perceptual processes/eyemovement behavior only in English reading, which shared similar perceptual demands as music notation reading, i.e.,sequential symbol strings separated by spaces. Thus, transfer effects across expertise domains can happen at differentcognitive processing levels, depending on the similarities of the processes involved.

The transformative potential of decisions

People face consequential personal decisions throughout their lives. Immigrating to another country or separating froma life-partner are but two examples. How do individuals make such notoriously difficult decisions? Can they make themrationally? We posit that answering both these questions requires understanding a decisions transformative potential, ac-cording to which decisions range in (1) their perceived temporal impact (half-life), (2) the extent to which the decisionmaker can know whether a choice will generally make them better or worse off (valence uncertainty), and (3) the perceivedlikelihood of a decision to change the decision maker (personal change). We propose that under the conditions of incom-plete information that decisions with high transformative potential inevitably entail, people may make them by recruitingtheir social and cultural environment and by relying on heuristics. These conditions also render bounded rationality prin-ciples (e.g., satisficing) a more plausible rationality benchmark than maximizing expected utilities.

A Cross-linguistic Study into the Contribution of Affective Connotation in theLexico-semantic Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts

Words carry affective connotations, but the role of these conno-tations in the representation of meaning is not well understood.Like other aspects of meaning, connotation might be cultureor language-specific. This study uses a large-scale relatednessjudgment task to determine the role of affective connotationsin concrete and abstract words in English, Rioplatense Span-ish, and Mandarin Chinese. Across languages, word valence,or how positive or negative a word is, was one of the main or-ganizing factors in both concrete and abstract concepts. More-over, predicted culture-specific affective connotations were re-liably found in the similarity space of abstract concepts. Afollow-up analysis was conducted to investigate whether distri-butional semantic representations derived from language simi-larly encodes these connotations using word embeddings. Thelanguage models did only partly captured the overall similaritystructure and the affective connotations shaping it.

Encoding or Post Encoding Mechanisms Invoke Enhanced Memory for Event Boundaries?

We perceive our environment by breaking it down into segments known as events. Event segmentation influences memory by enhancing the retention of information at boundaries as compared to information that is contained within the boundaries of an event (the event boundary advantage). This effect has been attributed to changes in attention during perception of events. Prior studies have demonstrated greater attention while perceiving event boundaries but have failed to demonstrate attention as the underlying mechanism for the event-boundary advantage. Two behavioral experiments were conducted to investigate, a) whether the event boundary advantage is observed even for events that are perceived while performing a concurrent task? and b) Is there a decrease in the boundary advantage when the concurrent task complexity is increased? In both experiments, participants watched videos related to performance of daily tasks, while simultaneously performing a probe detection task; either a simple dot detection (Experiment 1) or a go/ no-go task (Experiment 2). The probe was presented either at an event boundary or at pre-defined non-boundary time point and the memory for both temporal locations was measured after the completion of the detection task. A mixed effects logistic regression revealed an interactive effect for both detection accuracy and the boundary advantage; probe detection at event boundaries remained unaffected throughout an event irrespective of the level of the task complexity while, contrary to prediction, a boundary advantage in memory was also observed. But detection and memory accuracy for non-boundaries decreased successively for both low and high secondary task complexity suggesting greater interference for processing non-boundary information. These results indicate that greater attention may not be the only predictor of better memory for event boundaries as postulated by Event Segmentation theory.

Gesture and pause can facilitate chunking syntactic information in ambiguousphrases

It is known that phrases and sentences can be interpreted to have multiple meanings. Previous studies have focused mostlyon prosodic cues and pauses in the disambiguation mechanism of syntactic structures. In this study, we looked into thedisambiguation effects of gestures (iconic or beat) and three different duration of pauses (0.1, 0.5, 1.0 sec) at critical wordfor branching. The participants looked at a computer monitor that showed an actor doing gesture, and two pictures thatdepict different meanings. The participant was asked to choose the matched picture with the shown gesture. Reactiontime was also measured. The result was that participants responded more correctly when gesture of sequential chunkingwas shown than non-sequential chunking. More pause facilitated interpretation of the non-sequential stimulus, whereasmore pause facilitated the reaction for the sequential chunking stimulus. The study showed the importance of chunkingsyntactic information shown by gesture and pause.

Does bilingual input hurt? A simulation of language discrimination and clusteringusing i-vectors

The language discrimination process in infants has been suc-cessfully modeled using i-vector based systems, with re-sults replicating several experimental findings. Still, recentwork found intriguing results regarding the difference betweenmonolingual and mixed-language exposure on language dis-crimination tasks. We use two carefully designed datasets,with an additional “bilingual” condition on the i-vector modelof language discrimination. Our results do not show any dif-ference in the ability of discriminating languages between thethree backgrounds, although we do replicate past observationsthat distant languages (English-Finnish) are easier to discrimi-nate than close languages (English-German). We do, however,find a strong effect of background when testing for the abilityof the learner to automatically sort sentences in language clus-ters: bilingual background being generally harder than mixedbackground (one speaker one language). Other analyses revealthat clustering is dominated by speakers information ratherthan by languages.

Learning from explanations

What do we learn from a causal explanation? Upon being told that The fire occurred because a lit match was dropped, welearn that both of these events occurred, and that there is a causal relationship between them. However, causal explanationsof the kind E because C typically disclose much more than what is explicitly stated. Here, we offer a communication-theoretic account of causal explanations and show specifically that explanations can provide information about the extentto which a cited cause is normal or abnormal, and about the causal structure of the situation.In Experiment 1, we demonstrate that people infer the normality of a cause from an explanation when they know theunderlying causal structure. In Experiment 2, we show that people infer the causal structure from an explanation ifthey know the normality of the cited cause. We find these patterns both for scenarios that manipulate the statistical andprescriptive normality of events.Finally, we consider how the communicative function of explanations, as highlighted in this series of experiments, mayhelp to elucidate the distinctive roles that normality and causal structure play in causal explanation. Link to pre-print:https://psyarxiv.com/x5mqc

Toddlers recognize multiple meanings of polysemous words

Languages often reuse words for related meanings, such asbaseball cap and bottle cap, a phenomenon known aspolysemy. In English, it is estimated that 40-80% of allwords are polysemous, yet little is known about children’searly knowledge of polysemous words. In an eye-trackingstudy with monolingual English-learning 2-year-olds(n=40), we found that participants recognized multipleconventional meanings for polysemous nouns. We furtherinvestigated whether toddlers succeeded at this task becausethey were already familiar with multiple, learned meaningsfor words, or whether they simply guessed the correct targetbased on a single or vague meaning. To test this, we alsopresented participants with novel, related meanings for thesame English labels that are not conventional in English,e.g., the meaning “lid” for the label cap. The recognition ofconventional English meanings (baseball cap, bottle cap)was significantly higher than that of the novel extensionmeanings (e.g., a lid) for the same label (cap). These resultsshow that toddlers’ knowledge of polysemy goes beyond asingle or vague representation. At the same time, recognitionof the novel extended meanings was above chance,indicating that toddlers inferred that a related meaning wasthe better of the two options. Word learning theories must befurther developed to account for these complexities inlearning.

7.5-month olds remember the location of a displaced object only if an agent actedon it

Most infant studies on location memory involve an agent hiding or retrieving the object. Recent work indicates that, foryoung infants, the presence of other agents enhances encoding of the targets of their actions and perceptions, and in apilot study we did not find evidence for location memory with a paradigm where we removed agency cues. Here, wesystematically compared whether 7.5-months-old infants remembered the location of an object better when it was placedthere by an agent compared to a highly similar but non-social setting where a conveyor belt transports the object. Locationmemory was tested through infants’ looking times in response to outcomes showing unexpected vs. expected absences ofthe object. Contrary to our preliminary results, at n=58/64 of this preregistered study we see no main effects of conditionand outcome, as well as no interaction between them (all 95% credible intervals contain 0).

Social Learning with Sparse Belief Samples

We present a model of social learning over networks were individuals with insufficient and heterogeneous sources ofinformation aggregate their private observations with samples from belief distributions of their neighbors in order to learnan underlying state of the world. We presume two behavioral assumptions. The first assumes communication constraintsin that agents can only share, in each round, a single sample from their belief on the true state with their neighbors. This isin contrast with standard models of sharing the full belief, i.e. the entire probability distribution over the set of parameters.The second behavioral assumption points to an updating scheme according to which agents use simple linear rules toaggregate their neighbors’ actions with their private Bayesian posterior. We rigorously analyze the asymptotic behaviorof such an update and show that so long as all the individuals trust their neighbors more than their private informationsources, they do not learn the true parameter with positive probability. Social learning can occur, however, if the societycontains confident individuals that are experts in distinguishing different alternatives from truth, even though no singleindividuals may be able to distinguish the truth on her own. Our results indicate that social learning is possible even whenagents only share a single sample from their belief distribution.

Balancing Personal and Social Outcomes: Cultural Differences in ChildrensMoral Decision-Making

Previous work by Tasimi and Wynn (2016) suggests that children (5 to 8 years old) prefer to affiliate with other peoplebased on evaluations of their moral valence, but that this tendency is balanced against the childs personal costs andbenefits. We predicted that children from individualistic cultures may prioritize individual outcomes, whereas childrenfrom collectivistic cultures may consider social outcomes and harmony as more important. We applied a forced-choiceparadigm to measure childrens rejection of associating with a wrongdoer (mean person) by refusing stickers they offered,even though the alternative reward offered by a nice person was much smaller. Results suggest that overall, Asian childrenare more likely to reject wrongdoers than Caucasian children at the expense of personal rewards. We also found that suchcultural effects occur only among 7 to 8 years old children.

Strategy Inference and Switch Detection Method Generalizes to CategoryLearning

Lee, Gluck, and Walsh (2019) developed a series of Bayesian inference models that use multiple behavioral measuresto infer the use and switching of strategies in a decision-making task. Their approach addresses common deficiencies instrategy inference, such as the assumption that participants use a single fixed strategy and the methodological reliancesolely on decision outcomes to inform inference. These deficiencies are addressed by incorporating trial-level informationprocessing data and by allowing switch points in strategy use throughout the experiment protocol. Here we evaluate thegeneralizability of this approach using data from a Brunswik face category learning experiment (Gluck, Staszewski, Rich-man, Simon, & Delahanty, 2001). Results support the cross-domain generalizability of the Bayesian inference models forinferring both strategy use and switching using multiple sources of behavior. We compare these results to the conclusionsreached in the original research by Gluck et al. (2001).

Self-reference effect for faces is mediated by attention

Self is a central construct for various phenomenon in the history of psychology, and the pattern of being biased towardsthe information related to self is known as self-reference effect. Ones own face presents a unique stimuli to look at thecognitive processing self-reference effect. With help of two experiments, we investigated self-referential effect for facesand its relationship with attention. The first experiment looked at processing advantage for self-face compared to friendsface and a strangers face while participants performed orthogonal task of emotion perception. The second experimentinvolved manipulation of attention prior to emotion perception task used in experiment 1. Results indicate that RT forself-face were significantly shorter compared to friend face and stranger face. This processing advantage disappearedwhen cues were used prior to the attention task. We suggest that self-faces enhance processing by attentional capture.

Can visual object representations in the human brain be modelled by untrainedconvolutional neural networks with random weights?

Convolutional neural networks (CNNs) have proven effective as models of visual semantic responses in the inferior tem-poral cortex (IT). The belief has been that training a network for visual recognition leads it to represent visual features in away similar to those the brain has learned. However, a CNNs response is affected by its architecture and not just its train-ing. We therefore explicitly measured the effect of training different CNN architectures on their representational similaritywith IT. We evaluated two versions of AlexNet and two training regimes, supervised and unsupervised. Surprisingly, wefound that the representations in an untrained (random-weight) variant of AlexNet, reflected brain representations in ITbetter than the benchmark supervised AlexNet and also better than the corresponding network trained in either a super-vised or unsupervised manner. These results require a re-evaluation of the explanation of why CNNs act as an effectivemodel of visual representations.

Unsupervised categorization as similarity-based generalization

Unsupervised learning is widely recognized as an important problem in cognitive science, but unsupervised learning inhumans has received relatively little empirical investigation to date. We investigate unsupervised categorization usinga new task in which people generate verbal labels to novel objects, with objects given the same label assumed to bein the same mental category. Our main finding is that categorization is determined by similarity, i.e., the probabilityof placing two objects into the same category is an exponentially declining function of their dissimilarity, consistentwith Shepard’s (1987) universal law of generalization. We present data demonstrating the overall exponential pattern,plus specific predictions regarding selective attention, sensitivity to correlated features, and the effects of category size(number of examples). Taken together, the results suggest that the similarity-based approach used successfully in modelsof supervised categorization (e.g., Nosofsky 1986, 1992) may also extend to the domain of unsupervised categorization.

Prosodic Features Carry Information About a Questions Intent

Research has shown that pragmatic, social, and prosodic cues are used to infer the communicative intent of a speaker,including pedagogical intentions (Bohn & Frank, 2020; Cristia, 2013; Csibra & Gergely, 2009). However, little is knownabout whether prosodic features can signal pedagogical intent in syntactically equivalent utterances. We asked whetherprosodic features can carry information about the intent of a question (i.e., whether it is a pedagogical or an informationseeking question), both within child- and adult-directed speech. Eighty naive participants were asked to classify questionsgenerated by five different speakers. We found that participants could reliably discriminate between questions intendedto be pedagogical from those that were intended to be information seeking, both within child- and adult-directed speech,although pedagogical questions were detected more successfully when spoken with child-directed speech. These findingsindicate that prosody may convey pedagogical intent, which in turn may facilitate learning.

The Picture Guessing Game:The Role of Feedback in Active Artificial Language Learning

Language is acquired within a complex, interactiveenvironment. A key question for cognitive science is whetherand how different types of environmental cues might affectthe learning and processing of language. In this paper, weexplore the role of feedback as a possible cue in a novelactive artificial language learning task: The Picture GuessingGame. Subjects were instructed to guess which scenecorrectly displayed the meaning of a spoken sequence ofunfamiliar monosyllabic words. After their response, eitherpositive, negative, or no feedback was provided. Theprediction was that feedback would help the subject toeventually learn the vocabulary, syntax, and semantics of theartificial language. The results indeed showed that feedback(both positive and negative) is beneficial and necessary toattain a certain level of learning. Interestingly, the datashowed that positive feedback may be particularly helpful forthe learner, promoting more in-depth learning of the artificiallanguage.

Investigating the Structure of Emotion Concepts: Evidence from PropertyGeneration

Although work on conceptual knowledge has recently begun addressing the nature of abstract semantic representations,relatively little remains known about the structure of our knowledge of emotion concepts, an important subset of abstractconcepts. Property generationa common paradigm used to elaborate the featural representations of concepts that arecomponents of many models of semantic memoryhas been used extensively with concrete nouns, but in a limited numberof studies investigating abstract concepts. No prior work, to our knowledge, has systematically investigated the process ofproperty generation specifically for emotion concepts. In the present study, participants performed a property generationtask in which they listed features of emotion concepts and a matching number of concrete and abstract, non-emotionconcepts. Our results are interpreted with an emphasis on the distinction between emotion concepts and other abstractconcepts, which differ in the distribution of features generated.

Cross-modal ratio abstraction in children

In two experiments, we tested whether pre-schoolers can extract proportional information in the auditory modality andmatch it to a visual display. We familiarized 240 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds to a 2-minute stream of dog barks and frog croaksin a 4:1 ratio. In a forced-choice paradigm, we then presented a visual display of dogs and frogs (varying total number ofobjects in the display) in the target 4:1 ratio, against comparison ratios of 1:4, 2:1, 1:1, and 6:1. Children correctly chosethe matching 4:1 visual display over the 1:4 and 6:1 displays at above-chance rates regardless of absolute number, but onlyshowed a significant preference for the 4:1 display over 2:1 and 1:1 displays when the number of objects in the displaywas large. These findings provide preliminary support for cross-modal ratio abstraction in preschoolers and suggest thatthe absolute number of items in a display impacts childrens performance.

Hands in Thought and Motion

Theories of event-predictive, anticipatory behavior controlsuggest that complex action planning and control is segmentedinto sequences of anticipated subgoals and according behav-ioral events, which accomplish the subgoals. Here we focus onthe cognitive dynamics during successive subgoal activations.We combined a virtual object interaction task (prehension andtransport of a bottle) with a crossmodal congruency task. An-ticipatory crossmodal congruency effects (aCCEs) occur at thegoal of the current behavior, before the goal is reached. TheseaCCEs appear to be stronger during prehension, while visualdistractors at the currently irrelevant movement target have noeffect. While the results so far provide only partial supportfor the proposed anticipatory, sequential control process, theparadigm is well-suited to probe the dynamic changes of spa-tial body representations in object interactions.

Referent Management in Discourse: The Accessibility of Weak Definites

In this paper, we experimentally investigate the discourse prop-erties of weak definites (go to the doctor), and compare themto indefinites (go to a doctor) in German. While indefinite andweak definite noun phrases are highly similar when it comesto their sentence-level meaning, our visual world eye trackingstudy shows that weak definites are significantly less accessi-ble than indefinites when an ambiguous pronoun needs to beresolved in the subsequent discourse. However, contra someaccounts of weak definites, our results also show that it is verymuch possible for an anaphoric expression to access a weakdefinite. In sum, our experiment suggests that weak definitesintroduce new referents into a discourse, but that those refer-ents are embedded into an event structure associated with thestereotypical meaning of a weak definite construction. As a re-sult, referents introduced by weak definites are less prominentthan referents introduced by indefinites.

Using Think-Aloud Protocols to Explore Students’ Use of Knowledge ForumAnalytic Tools

Digital technologies have drastically transformed the way in which we communicate, visualize, and work with information,giving rise to new research areas, such as child-computer interaction (Read & Bekker, 2011) and computer-supportedcollaborative learning (Dillenbourg, Jrvel, & Fischer, 2009). Consequently, cognitive scientists are increasingly interestedin understanding how children think and learn with digital technologies (e.g., Greenfield & Yan, 2005). This study usesconcurrent think-aloud protocols to elicit childrens explanations of how they use analytic tools to support their learning onan online platform called Knowledge Forum (Scardamalia, 2017). After using Knowledge Forum for eight months (Ma &Akyea, 2019), five third-graders participated in 20-minute sessions to interpret their online activities using analytic tools(e.g., bar charts, sociograms, word clouds). Generally, they were cognizant of their online behaviours, and the tools raisedmetacognitive awareness toward productive social interactions. Practical implications for using analytic tools to supportself-regulated learning are discussed.

Do social cues promote cross-situational verb learning and retention?

Children learn words using a range of social, statistical, and perceptual information. One proposal for how childrendetermine word meanings is cross-situational learning, in which children track ambiguous word-object mappings overtime (e.g., Yu & Smith, 2007). However, previous studies have not evaluated how children use natural social cues duringlearning (e.g., eye gaze). We taught 3-year-olds three novel verbs (c.f., Scott & Fisher, 2012) and hypothesized that socialcues not only support cross-situational learning, but also support retention of verbs after a delay. In between-subjectsconditions, children either did or did not have access to eye-gaze and head-turn cues during exposure. We tested forparticipants learning after 12 learning trials and after a delay. Pilot data suggest that children who have access to naturalsocial cues successfully learned and retained links between novel verbs and their corresponding actions.

Artificial Language Learning: Combining Syntax and Semantics

Artificial Grammar Learning (AGL) paradigms are a powerful method to study language learning and processing. How-ever, unlike natural languages, these tasks rely on grammars specifying relationships between meaningless stimuli with noreal-world referents. Therefore, learning is typically assessed based on grammaticality or familiarity judgements, assess-ing how well-formed a sequence is. We combined a meaningful vocabulary (in which nonsense words refer to propertiesof visual stimuli (colored shapes)) with different grammatical structures (adjacent, center-embedded, or crossed dependen-cies). Using an incremental, starting-small paradigm, participants were asked to interpret increasingly complex sequencesof nonsense words and select the set of visual stimuli that they described. High levels of learning were observed for allgrammars, including those which have previously been difficult to learn in traditional AGL paradigms. Here, the addi-tion of semantics not only allows closer comparisons to natural language but also aids learning, representing a valuableapproach to studying language learning.

Applying the Common Model of Cognition to Resting-State fMRI Leads to theIdentification of Abnormal Functional Connectivity in Parkinson’s Disease

A complete understanding of cognitive function in humansmust incorporate a model of interactions between networkedbrain regions. Alterations to these network interactions under-lie cognitive impairment in many neurodegenerative diseases,providing an important physiological link between brain struc-ture and cognitive function. Cognitive architectures have of-ten been used to explain how healthy brains function, typi-cally using task-based activity. However, this description isincomplete. Most systems-level brain activity is spontaneous,or intrinsic, and occurs whether or not a subject is performinga task. Here, we provide evidence that the Common Modelof Cognition, a consensus model derived from an analysis ofexisting cognitive architectures, can (a) be generalized to ac-count for brain activity at rest, rather than during tasks, and (b)correctly identify differences in basal ganglia connectivity inParkinson’s Disease.

You said something about me: Contextual self-relevance during a first encounterwith a face impacts later face recognition

Self-relevant information (i.e. related to the observer) is better remembered than other-relevant information. However, itremains to be seen how self-relevance during an initial social encounter can impact later face recognition. We presented63 participants with sentences describing an opinion varying in self-relevance (self/other-relevant) and valence (posi-tive/negative), followed by neutral face pictures of each opinion holder. Eye-tracking ensured the sentences were readand participants rated the valence and affective arousal of how each face made them feel. Participants then completed asurprise recognition task for the target faces. Recognition accuracy was greater when faces were preceded by self-relevantthan other-relevant sentences, and these faces were more arousing. Sentence self-relevance and valence interacted to affectparticipant valence ratings of the face, but not recognition accuracy. This indicates that initial social encounters can havea lasting effect on ones memory of another person, producing an enhanced memory trace of that individual.

Social influence and informational independence

We frequently use social information when making decisions.For instance, other people may know more about a problemthan we do, so we might update our initial beliefs in light oftheir opinions. The epistemic value of these social cues de-pends in part on their informational independence. Peopleshould thus be sensitive to nonindependence in their weightingof social information. However, the current literature yieldsconflicting results. In one recent study, participants valued so-cial information less when it was nonindependent; in another,participants were insensitive to nonindependence. We identifypossible causes of this inconsistency, and present an experi-mental paradigm that aims to fill these gaps. Then, in a study(N=200) with pre-registered hypotheses and analyses, we findthat participants were not sensitive to cue dependence. Wehighlight the relevance of this finding for the modern mediacontext, where nonindependence of both traditional and socialmedia sources can lead to the spread of bias or false belief.

Seeking Meaning: Examining a Cross-situational Solution to Learn Action VerbsUsing Human Simulation Paradigm

To acquire the meaning of a verb, language learners not onlyneed to find the correct mapping between a specific verb andan action or event in the world, but also infer the underlyingrelational meaning that the verb encodes. Most verb naminginstances in naturalistic contexts are highly ambiguous as manypossible actions can be embedded in the same scenario andmany possible verbs can be used to describe those actions. Tounderstand whether learners can find the correct verb meaningfrom referentially ambiguous learning situations, we conductedthree experiments using the Human Simulation Paradigm withadult learners. Our results suggest that although finding theright verb meaning from one learning instance is hard, there isa statistical solution to this problem. When provided withmultiple verb learning instances all referring to the same verb,learners are able to aggregate information across situations andgradually converge to the correct semantic space. Even in caseswhere they may not guess the exact target verb, they can stilldiscover the right meaning by guessing a similar verb that issemantically close to the ground truth.

Punishment: Incentive or Communication?

Humans are adept at using punishments to influence and modify the behavior of others. Current approaches model pun-ishment as a direct, immediate imposition of cost. In contrast, our research suggests that people interpret punishment as acommunicative act. We show that people expect costless, yet communicative, punishments to be as effective as cost impos-ing punishment (Experiment 1). Under some situations, people display a systematic preference for costless punishmentsover more canonical, cost imposing punishments (Experiment 2). People readily seek out and infer the communicativemessage inherent in a punishment (Experiment 3). And, people expect that learning from punishment depends on theease with which its communicative intent can be inferred (Experiment 4). Taken together, these findings demonstrate thatpeople expect punishment to be generated and interpreted as a communicative act.

Look before you leap: Quantitative tradeoffs between peril and reward in actionunderstanding

When we reason about the goals of others, how do we balance the positive outcomes that actions led to, with the potentiallybad ways those actions could have ended? In a four-part experiment, we tested whether and how adults (full study) and6- to 8-year-old children (ongoing study) expect other agents to take account of the ways their goal-directed action couldhave failed. Across 4 different tasks, we found that adults expected others to negatively appraise perilous situations (deeptrenches), to minimize the danger of their actions, and to trade off danger and reward in their action plans. Our preliminarychildrens study shows similar trends. These results suggest that people appeal to peril-how badly things could go if onesactions fail-when explaining and predicting other peoples actions, and also make quantitative inferences that are finelytuned to the degree of peril and reward that others face.

Learning a Generative Model of Human Faces Through Inverse Rendering

Generative models in an inverse graphics framework are appealing models for visual perception. How might childrenacquire them? We present a computational procedure for learning generative models of human faces using developmen-tally plausible input. Our statistical model of shape and appearance initially uses the average face as a template with asimple Gaussian process model of deformations. We iteratively learn the statistical distribution of faces by performinganalysis-by-synthesis on a small number of images and combine the results to construct an improved generative model.Our analysis-by-synthesis framework combines a convolutional neural network for fast inference with a Markov chainMonte Carlo process for detailed refinement. This learning strategy quickly captures the variation of natural faces anddemonstrates an efficient way to learn the distribution of faces.

Cross-Domain Adversarial Reprogramming of a Recurrent Neural Network

Neural networks are vulnerable to adversarial attacks. These attacks can be untargeted, causing the model to make anyerror, or targeted, causing the model to make a specific error. Adversarial Reprogramming introduces a type of attackthat reprograms the network to perform an entirely new task from its original function. Additional inputs in a pre-trainednetwork can repurpose the network to a different task. Previous work has shown adversarial reprogramming possible insimilar domains, such as an image classification task in ImageNet being repurposed for CIFAR-10. A natural questionis whether such reprogramming is feasible across any task for neural networks a positive answer would have significantimpact both on wider applicability of ANNs, but also require rethinking their security. We attempt for the first timereprogramming across domains, repurposing a text classifier to an image classifier, using a recurrent neural network aprototypical example of a Turing universal network.

What counts as seeing? Young childrens understanding of perceptual reports

Young children can reason about direct and indirect visual information, but fully mapping this understanding to linguisticforms encoding the two knowledge sources appears to come later in development. In English, perception verbs with smallclause complements (I saw something happen) report direct perception of an event, while perception verbs with sententialcomplements (I saw that something happened) can report inferences about an event. In two experiments, we explore when4-9-year-old English-speaking children have linked the conceptual distinction between direct perception and inferenceto different complements expressing this distinction. We find that unlike older children or adults, 4-6-year-olds do notrecognize that see with a sentential complement can report visually-based inference, even when syntactic and contextualcues make inference interpretations highly salient. Until around age seven, children are still learning the syntax andsemantics of perception verbs like see and how distinct syntactic forms encode different kinds of perceptual experience.

Does looking time predict choice in domestic dogs? Examining visual attention inmans best friend

Dogs live in an environment built around humans dominant sense of sight. Despite millenia of co-habitation, little isknown about how dogs visually evaluate objects when making perceptual decisions, and whether they do this in a human-like manner. To explore this question, we analyzed visual attention patterns of pet dogs (N=39) in a 2-object choicetask. Two foods of unequal reward value (hotdogs and dried corn) were presented over ten trials in four experimentalconditions: i) in open palms; ii) on plates; iii) in cups; and iv) in filled jars. Dogs chose one food item per trial. We codedvisual attention measures of total looking time at each item and frequency of looks to each item from video and comparedthem with dogs subsequent item choice strategies. We discuss gazing and choice behaviour in a comparative context ofperceptual decision making.

Time flies: Hippocampal time cells represent the distant past with less resolution

Hippocampal time cells carry a record of recent experience by firing during a circumscribed period of time after a triggeringstimulus. Different cells have “time fields’ at different delays up to at least tens of seconds. The observation that timefields representing events further in the past are wider supports the hypothesis that the more distant past is recorded withless resolution. However, previous studies have analyzed time fields averaged over trials, leading to the possibility thatthis could be a trial-averaging artifact. We analyzed single-unit recorded time cells with a hierarchical-Bayesian modelthat separately estimated within-trial receptive field width and across-trial variability. Even after isolating across-trialvariability, time-field width covaries with delay, consistent with less resolution for past events. Moreover, the shift oftime-fields for simultaneously recorded time cells correlates with each other, suggesting that time runs at different ratesfor the population from one trial to the next.

Causality and Self-Signaling in Economic Games

Our ability to cooperate is one of the cornerstones of our success as a species, and the story of how humans have been ableto put aside immediate personal gain in favor of a longer view is widely studied. We add to this literature by exploringcertain seemingly irrational behaviors observed in economic games. Modes of cognition such as those reflected in self-signaling theory may serve to explain how the seemingly irrational might sometimes be quite sensible. We elicit thesebehaviors using real-time multiplayer economic games and suggest mechanisms whereby players may incorporate thevalue of receiving certain signals themselves into their utility calculations, thus making for rational behaviorand rationalinferencein cases where it is not obviously so. These phenomena are consistent with a combination of self-signaling and alimit on the direction of inference in time.

English Speakers Produce and Understand Expletive Negation

Romance languages are well known for their use of expletive negation (henceforth, EN), i.e., the occurrence of a negator in the complement clause of certain verbs, adpositions or adverbs that is “illogically” not part of the meaning of the sentence. This study explores the hypothesis that such “illogism” that recurs across languages must be due to universal properties of the message to be encoded and the language production system. Jin & Koenig (2019) proposed a language production model to account for the striking similarity of EN-triggers between two unrelated languages (French and Mandarin). Their model makes several predictions which our paper tests: (i) languages like English where EN is purported not to occur should in fact include the same range of EN-triggers; (ii) English speakers can understand a negator within the scope of an EN-trigger expletively; (iii) the likelihood a speaker of English will understand a negator expletively is correlated with how frequently she has encountered an expletive interpretation of negators for that particular trigger. To test the first prediction, we conducted a corpus study of unrehearsed English speech on Google. To test the second prediction, we conducted a semantic Stroop-like comprehension experiment where participants’ semantic judgements (both logical accuracy and response time) was dependent on whether a negator was interpreted logically or expletively. Overall, this paper suggests that EN is by no means specific to Romance languages and that expletive uses of negators occur in the same contexts in both production and comprehension in languages where EN is not conventionalized to the same degree it is in Romance. Overall, our results support the claim that “illogical” properties of natural languages that recur across languages of the world reflect universal properties of the language production system.

Knowing when to quit:Children consider access to solutions when deciding whether to persist

Although persistence is essential to overcoming challenges andmaking new discoveries, continued effort can be costly. Evenvery young learners must make decisions about when to investeffort and when to abandon a task. In the current study, weexplore whether children’s decisions about when to exert effortare influenced by the information they stand to gain in aparticular learning situation. That is, we examine whetherproviding children with solutions after they attempt tocomplete a challenging task reduces their persistence. Sixty 4-and 5-year-old children completed a series of iSpy puzzles andthen attempted to activate a novel toy. Children were eitherpresented with the solutions after attempting each task or givenno information about the answers. Our results demonstrate thatchildren persisted longer at attempting to activate a novel toywhen their effort was more likely to be the only source ofinformation: children who expected to be provided with thesolution gave up faster than those who did not. We discuss theimplications of these findings on children’s rational decisionsabout when effort is worthwhile, and consider how providinganswers might impact motivation and curiosity more broadly.

Evidence for Win-Stay-Lose-Shift in Puppies and Adult Dogs

Many organisms encounter situations where they lackinformation required to successfully exploit a resource. Onestable strategy that may be particularly useful is a win-stay-lose-shift strategy, in which an individual continues toperform a behavior that has proven fruitful in the recent pastor otherwise shifts to a new behavior. Here we investigatewhether domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a win-stay-lose-shift strategy utilizing data from 326 puppies and 323 adultdogs on a repeated object-choice task. We found a significanteffect of previous-trial success on dogs’ subsequent searchpatterns. Specifically, dogs were more likely to shift searchlocations if they were unsuccessful on the previous trial.These findings suggest that puppies and adult dogs win-stay-lose-shift.

Striatal and Cortical Components of Inattentional Responses: An Experimentaland Computational Study of theWisconsin Card Sorting Test in Adults withADHD traits

Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neuropsychiatric condition with a neurodevelopmental coursethat often persists in adulthood. Although it is conceptualised as a categorical disorder, ADHD traits are present in thegeneral population. ADHD constitutes an important paradigm because its aetiology is related to both frontal and striatalcircuits, but it is unclear what localised operations could be at fault when ADHD symptoms arise. We present a study where50 adults, of which 14 had a diagnosis of ADHD, performed a speeded and unspeeded variation of the Wisconsin CardSorting Test (WCST) and completed a set of questionnaires, including the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS).Results indicate that sorting errors on the WCST did not differ between groups. However, when response times werecharacterised in terms of parameterised ex-Gaussian distributions for the unspeeded part of task, moderate correlationswere found between the parameter corresponding to the thickness of the tail of the distribution and subscales of theCAARS measuring inattention and impulsivity. This suggests that inattention and/or impulsivity explain the occasionalslower responses of ADHD participants. We consider the results in the context of an existing computational model thatsimulates cortical and basal ganglia operation in the WCST, where a qualitative exploration supports a distinction betweencortical and striatal components of the psychological processes that lead to performance of participants with ADHD traits.

Semantic Adaptation in Quantifier Meanings in Preschool Aged Children

How flexible are children’s semantic representations? It is unknown whether children can adapt to different speaker’slanguage use and form speaker-specific representations to facilitate comprehension. Adults update their expectations abouthow a speaker uses quantifiers after exposure to the speaker (Yildirim et al., 2016). Here, we explore whether this abilityis also present in preschool-aged children. In Experiment 1, we show that preschoolers have adult-like expectations abouthow a generic speaker would use the quantifiers ’some’ (less than 50%) and ’many’ (greater than 50%). In Experiment 2,forty 4 and 5-year-olds (mean = 4.6) were exposed to a speaker who was biased to either prefer using ’some’ or ’many’ ina situation with a proportion of 50%. After exposure, participants updated their expectations about the use of ’some’ and’many’, such that they aligned better with the exposure speaker’s usage, suggesting that preschoolers are able to engage insemantic adaptation.

Directional biases in durative inference

Descriptions of durational relations can be ambiguous, e.g., thedescription ‘two different meetings happened at the same time’could mean that one meeting started before the other ended, orit could mean that the meetings both started and endedsimultaneously. A recent theory posits that people mentallysimulate events with durations by representing the starts andends of events along a chronological axis (Khemlani et al.,2015). To draw conclusions from this durational mental model,reasoners consciously scan it in the direction of earlier timepoints to later time points. The account predicts that peopleshould prefer descriptions that are congruent with achronological scanning procedure, e.g., descriptions thatmention the starts of events before the ends of events. Twoexperiments corroborate the prediction, and show thatchronological biases in temporal reasoning manifest in caseswhen reasoners consciously evaluate the durations of events.

An emotionally intelligent actor model for virtual conference presenters

Hosting a scientific conference in Virtual or Mixed Reality becomes a reality. One key advantage of this format is thepossibility to use Virtual Actors controlled by Artificial Intelligence as conference participants, in such roles as a VirtualPoster Presenter, a Discussion Panel Moderator, a Lightning Session Chair, and a Virtual Party Servant. All these rolesrequire human-level socially emotional functionality and can be implemented using one approach, which is based on theemotional Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architecture (eBICA). At the core of the model is a semantic map of humanemotional states. Interaction modalities include facial expression, gaze and other body language, voice intonation, and thesentiment of verbal content of communications - using both recognition and expression technologies for each modality.Paradigms involve establishment and maintenance of believable socially emotional contact with a human participant. Theconference BICA*AI 2020 (https://bica2020.bicasociety.org) is used as a testbed. Support: Russian Science FoundationGrant #18-11-00336.

Bayesian inference in dialogue

A word is referentially ambiguous if it has several potential referents. Observing how listeners make choices among thosereferents can reveal their hidden beliefs and preferences, as well as reflect their reasoning strategies. We asked subjectsto observe how one of the objects is chosen following a possibly ambiguous utterance and to infer which preferences thelistener may have had in mind when choosing that particular object. In order to adjust this interaction to a dialogue-likesetting, we extended the traditional one-shot reference game to a round of 4-trial games. Moreover, we modeled theprocess within the Rational Speech Act framework, implementing iterative inference over multiple trials, where posteriorsfrom previous trials carry over to the next trial as priors. The model predicts human inference behavior better than abaseline uniform model, as well as better than a non-iterative model. The results imply that, in principle, humans areable to compute Bayesian-like inferences in dialogue, learning about the beliefs and preferences of others in a cumulativemanner.

Enculturing cognition: integrating material culture in human cognitive evolution

Debates about human cognitive evolution include the uniqueness, antiquity, and foundations of the modern mind. Widelyaccepted models often pose progressive cognitive stages ascribed to particular species from apes to humans, placingthe emergence of fundamental aspects of modern human cognition late in evolution. Given that recent archaeologicaldiscoveries suggest that many traits traditionally used to define H. sapiens mentality (i.e. symbolism, language) are olderand likely shared with archaic hominins (e.g. Neanderthals), how can we identify truly distinctive aspects of cognition inphylogeny? Topical studies are demonstrating how different facets of material culture (e.g. tool use, tool production, skilllearning) can shape the mind. Considering this, models of hominin cognition based on material culture can provide moreaccurate and testable accounts that need not appeal to progressistic criteria. This way, material culture studies can bridgethe current chasm between the archaeological and fossil records and theories of cognitive evolution.

Adventures in Flatland: Perceiving Social Interactions Under Physical Dynamics

People make fast, spontaneous, and consistent judgementsof social situations, even in complex physical contexts withmultiple-body dynamics (e.g. pushing, lifting, carrying, etc.).What mental computations make such judgments possible? Dopeople rely on low-level perceptual cues, or on abstract con-cepts of agency, action, and force? We describe a new exper-imental paradigm, Flatland, for studying social inference inphysical environments, using automatically generated interac-tive scenarios. We show that human interpretations of events inFlatland can be explained by a computational model that com-bines inverse hierarchical planning with a physical simulationengine to reason about objects and agents. This model out-performs cue-based alternatives based on hand-coded (multi-nomial logistic regression) and learned (LSTM) features. Ourresults suggest that humans could use a combination of intu-itive physics and hierarchical planning to interpret complex in-teractive scenarios encountered in daily life.

What you didn’t see:Prevention and generation in continuous time causal induction

How do people use temporal information to make causal judg-ments? A number of studies have investigated the role of timein inferring generative causal structure, while few have exam-ined prevention. Here, we focus on a challenging task in whichparticipants learn the structure of several causal “devices” bywatching the devices’ patterns of activation over time. Eachdevice potentially includes both generative (producing an acti-vation of its effect) and preventative (blocking any effect acti-vations within a short time window) causal relationships. Weexamine judgment patterns through the lens of a normativemodel which incorporates actual causation with considerationsof prevention. We contrast this with a more computationallytractable feature-based approximation. Participants’ perfor-mance was substantially above chance in all conditions. Themajority of participants’ causal judgments were best fit by thefeature-based approximation based on delay and count heuris-tic cues.

The One-Voice Expert

Producing and processing speech involves complex feedbackloops of sensory and motor signals. Vocal sounds are par-tially processed as a movement affordance, allowing us to learnspeaking patterns through imitation, which can be beneficialfor language learning. In this study, we examine this pro-cess as a type of social embodiment illusion — the blurringof boundaries between self and other. Participants performedan altered version of a theatrical game called the ‘one-voiceexpert’, where they improvised speech in same-gender dyads.Unlike previous studies, we looked separately at the effectsof simultaneousness (‘speaking at the same time’) and syn-chronicity (‘saying the same thing’). These two variables werefound to influence vocal characteristics and self-voice recog-nition in a distinct way, with synchronicity leading to strongerpitch adaptation and simultaneousness to suppression of pho-netic convergence. We conclude that linking embodiment pro-cesses to joint speech in real world social interactions could bea promising new conceptual framework, with possible applica-tions for language learning.

Affect and syntactic anomaly

In an event-related potential (ERP) language experiment, we investigated whether variability in the P600 component, amarker of syntactic anomaly, could be accounted for by dispositional affect. Sentences such as (i) The broker plannedto conceal the transaction * was sent to jail vs. (ii) The broker persuaded * to conceal the transaction was sent tojail (critical words are underlined) were read by 25 participants. These stimuli were adapted from Osterhout & Holcomb(1992), an influential early study on the P600 waveform. We expected to replicate previous findings, where P600 effectswere expected at to in (ii) vs. (i) and at was in (i) vs. (ii). The P600 effect at to did not replicate, whereas it didat was . Regarding affect, our results showed a significant positive correlation between positive affect scores and P600amplitude. Results are discussed in terms of the family of P600 components and affect.

Improving Predictive Accuracy of Models of Learning and Retention Through BayesianHierarchical Modeling: An Exploration with the Predictive Performance Equation

Human learning has been characterized by three robust effects (i.e.power law of learning, power law of decay, and spacing), whichhave been validated across multiple domains and time intervals. Toaccount for these different effects mathematical model of learningand retention have been developed. These models hold a great dealof potential for application a wide range of educational and trainingscenarios. However, many models are not validated according fortheir ability to make accurate predictions of human performance.The predictive demand of these models is made increasinglycomplex by the needs of training domain, needing both to predictboth skill decay and reacquisition from little historical data. In thispaper, we examine the predictive capability of the PredictivePerformance Equation (PPE) implemented in a Bayesianhierarchical model. Through a comparison of two Bayesianhierarchical models we show how hierarchical model fit to aparticipant’s performance across a set of items compared to only asingle item improves PPE’s predictive accuracy of both skill decayand reacquisition over multiple learning schedules

Emotional Valence of Narratives Is Preserved Across Multiple Retellings

Frederic Bartlett pioneered the research on serial reproduction in 1932 and suggested that the stereotypical or schematicform of narratives consists in rationalization, a causal connection within a story and its plot. We conducted the largestretelling experiment to date with two different studies (19,086 retellings; 12,840 participants) that both reach the conclu-sion that retelling of narratives is focused on the precise preservation of the storys degree of happiness and sadness, evenwhen many other aspects related to coherence and rationalization of the story deteriorate. These findings, supported by anovel statistical model with Bayesian estimation, suggest that the happiness and sadness of a story operates as the anchorof stability for both reception-encoding and for reproduction-retrieval of narratives. We suggest that happiness and sadnessin narratives function not simply as discrete emotions, but also as verdicts concerning the outcome of a story.

Tracking Age-Related Cognitive Decline: Insights from the Detection ofAdvertisements

Most research suggests that older adults experience declines in cognitive abilities, but these outcomes are often drawn fromexperimental paradigms that do not employ naturalistic materials. The present study addresses this issue by examiningolder and younger adults’ ability to detect so-called native advertisements, which are designed to seamlessly blend intotheir medium. Participants viewed either real webpages (visual) or listened to abbreviated content from real radio/podcasts(auditory). Both age groups were less accurate and slower at detecting native compared to traditional advertisements. Fur-ther, older adults had greater difficulty detecting native advertisements on webpages, but no age-related differences wereobserved with auditory materials. The lack of differences in the auditory domain is intriguing, yet it is broadly consistentwith work suggesting spoken language abilities show little or moderate decline. Together, the results demonstrate hownaturalistic stimuli help reveal the extent to which specific domains are affected in cognitive aging.

Investigation of Attentional Decay: Implications for Instruction

Given that attention is a limited capacity resource we are onlyable to selectively attend to a small subset of information atany one time. Endogenously regulating attention during aninstructional activity is effortful and can be challenging forchildren as well as adults. Although improvements inattention regulation have been documented with age, less isknown about the duration of time individuals are able toselectively sustain attention during instruction, due in part tomethodological limitations. The present study leverages eye-tracking technology to provide an objective examination ofattentional decay during a lecture. Adult participants (N=96)watched a geography screencast lecture while a mobile eye-tracker was utilized to measure changes in attention over thecourse of the lecture. Results indicate that attention declinesover time and reductions in attention occur before Minute 15.Implications for instruction are discussed.

Research-Based Teaching Practices for Improving Students’ Understanding ofMathematical Equivalence Have Not Made it into Elementary Classrooms

Elementary math instruction traditionally has emphasizedprocedures rather than concepts. Thus, students tend to lack astrong understanding of foundational concepts likemathematical equivalence. Cognitive scientists andmathematics educators have found small yet effective ways tomodify traditional arithmetic instruction to promote students’conceptual understanding of math equivalence. Educationalstandards also now reflect this academic research. However, itis unclear whether classroom practices have caught up withresearch and policy. In the current study, we observed teachers’practices during arithmetic instruction. The goal was todetermine if teachers are using research-based practices thatpromote understanding of math equivalence and if variation inuse of research-based practices is associated with students’growth in understanding of math equivalence across the schoolyear. Eight second and third grade classrooms (M students perclassroom = 23) were observed twice during math instruction.Students completed a math test both before and after theobservation period. Research-based practices were rarelyobserved in any classrooms, so there was not much variation inclassroom use of research-based practices to predict studentgrowth. Students improved their performance on all problemtypes tested, but performance on math equivalence problemswas significantly lower than on other problem types. Resultssuggest that policies and practices designed to improvestudents’ understanding of math equivalence may not havefiltered down to affect instructional practices in classrooms.

Perceiving unseen objects

We regularly make inferences about the presence and properties of objects or entities in our environment that we cannotsee directly, be it while driving, playing sports, or making scientific discoveries. But how do we know what these unseenobjects are, and what properties they have? Our studies explore these questions by showing participants scenes of a balltraveling beneath, then later exiting, a covered region, and asking them to recreate a configuration of unobserved obstaclesthe ball could have bounced off to produce the observed trajectory. We find that in many cases people were able to recoverthe approximate world structure; however, there were also instances in which people consistently used a configurationwith fewer blocks that would cause modest deviations from the observations of the time or direction of the balls trajectory.Inferring unseen objects thus appears to involve a trade-off between parsimony and explanatory power.

Intelligence in humans, non-human animals, and machines

Artificially intelligent systems are unlike other intelligences in a crucial yet vastly under-appreciated respect. For anaturally-evolved species, its survival needs are not only what ought to properly measure that species intelligence, butalso what most fundamentally shape it. However, artificial systems are not shaped by evolutionary forces. Instead, wemust provide for such systems a suitable equivalent for the evolutionary shaping of a natural species intelligence. But wecannot. As a result, I maintain that we cannot currently develop artificial systems that are intelligent in anything like theway that the members of a naturally-evolved species are intelligent. On any of the main approaches to AIwhether classical,deep learning, or a combination of bothwe must either explicitly represent or instead replicate a suitable equivalent forwhat evolution provides in its shaping of a naturally-evolved species intelligence. I maintain that is unclear how to do anysuch thing.

Representing Typological Prevalence in Graph-Based Semantic Maps

A graph-based semantic map is a visual representation of presumptively universal conceptual structure underlying seman-tic variation across languages. In such maps, vertices (nodes) represent semantic functions (e.g., the spatial relation ofsupport) and edges connect conceptually similar functions. Using an algorithm that selects edges based on the frequencywith which pairs of semantic functions co-occur across words (or other linguistic forms), Regier and colleagues inferredparsimoniousbut not maximally informativesemantic maps from cross-language data on indefinite pronouns and spatial re-lations. Here, using the same data, we present several alternative map inference methods that prioritize informativeness byaccounting for typological prevalencethe frequency with which pairs of semantic functions co-occur across languagesviathe selection and/or weighting of edges. We suggest that these methods may provide a more complete picture of theuniversal conceptual bases of cross-language semantic variation.

Food sharing gave birth to social networks

Social networks present distinctive features when compared with other types of networks, particularly the presence ofcommunities, which are subsets of nodes much more densely connected among themselves, than with the rest of the net-work. In this work, we propose an explanation for this pattern based on the following: groups may be the communitysolution of hunter-gatherer societies to the survival problem posed by the uncertainty of food. We propose a multi-agentmodel inspired by a food-sharing dynamic, which combines and formalizes two main notions discussed by some anthropo-logical literature: the reciprocity in the exchanges of food, plus the care for the general welfare of agents. Our preliminaryresults show that near-to-optimal food-sharing networks exhibit highly-connected groups around special agents that wecall hunters, those who inject food into the system. We show the robustness of these results by computer simulations andalso by analytical arguments for these simulations.

Openness to Fictional Experience: Measuring Readers’ and Viewers’ NarrativeAbsorption as a Function of Personality

Social media uses narrative templates to present information, whether news (real or fake) or advertisements. The perpetualengagement with stories influences our attention, memory, thinking and behaviour. This study addresses two researchquestions: What kind of story engages what kind of audience? Are people high in openness to experience more susceptibleto getting lost in counterfactual worlds? Participants with high/low scores in openness to experience are presented withliterary and film vignettes independently rated as engaging/non-engaging. Narrative absorption and openness to experiencequestionnaires provide preliminary data indicating reliable narrative absorption-openness correlation. Eye tracking willprovide implicit narrative engagement measures for attention (eye fixation), cognitive load (pupil dilation) and engagement(gaze duration). Eye-movement, self-reports, and personality questionnaires will indicate which narrative designs engagespecific audiences efficiently.

Looking downward to the future: Chinese minds eye in time space

Westerners are reported to more often direct their eyes upward when thinking about the future and downward whenconceptualizing the past. It is unknown whether this vertical space-time mapping is universally true. We studied Mandarinspeakers gaze positions when they mentally displaced themselves for one minute into the past or future. Unlike westerners,Chinese directed their eyes more downward when conceptualizing the future than the past; such effects were not due todifferences in emotion or thinking difficulty between the past and future. Another study of Chinese peoples eyes duringsentence comprehension showed that participants had higher gazing positions when processing past-related sentencesthan when processing future-related sentences. These eye-gaze related correlates of a vertical mental timeline appearedearlier when processing sentences with space-time metaphors than with neutral time expressions. The differences betweenChinese and westerners show that language and culture can shape peoples eye movements when processing time.

Epistemic Beliefs, Language, and Sources: Interactive Effects on Belief and Trustof Scientific Information

Research suggests that peoples learning may be influenced by individual differences in their epistemic beliefs, such asFaith in Intuition (FiN), Need for Evidence (NfE), and belief that Truth is Political(TiP). This study investigated the extentto which these epistemic beliefs influenced belief in scientific information about global warming and trust in sources.Participants read statements about global warming and rated how much they believed the information and trusted thesource. Each statement was presented with a conservative, liberal, or scientific source and framed in certain or tentativelanguage. We found that epistemic beliefs significantly interacted with source and language tentativeness. For example,those with low FiN believed certain language statements more than tentative language statements. Those with low NfEbelieved conservative sources more than scientific or liberal sources. These findings demonstrate how individuals epistemicbeliefs interact with source and language factors to influence belief and trust of scientific information.

Dissociating adaptation to word-specific and color-specific conflict frequency in the Stroop task

In the Stroop task, congruency effects are typically larger for color words presented mainly in their congruent color than for color words presented mainly in incongruent colors. However, the nature of this item-specific proportion congruent (ISPC) effect is debated: It might be produced by either conflict-adaptation processes (e.g., focus attention to task-relevant information when the word BLUE appears) and/or a more general contingency-learning process (e.g., anticipate a green response when the word BLUE appears). We re-examined the role of conflict-adaptation processes in this paradigm in two experiments. In both experiments, a conflict-adaptation effect emerged on stimuli matched on contingency. Further, in Experiment 2, we found separate effects of adaptation to the frequency of conflict specific to the color and word dimensions of individual stimuli. These results challenge the contingency-learning account of the ISPC effect and suggest that conflict-adaptation processes in this paradigm may depend on both task-relevant and task- irrelevant information.

Visual Quality and Lexical Quality Reduce Readers Reliance on Sentence Contextfor Word Recognition

Readers use predictions about upcoming words to facilitate word recognition, particularly when the visual input is degraded(e.g., viewed in parafoveal vision; Staub & Goddard, 2019) or when the reader has poor lexical quality (Hersch & Andrews,2012). To test how these factors interact participants, who were assessed for spelling ability, made a two-alternative forced-choice regarding one letter, which differentiated the target from an orthographic neighbor (e.g., worm was followed byW or D?). The target was presented either in foveal or parafoveal vision and was preceded by a sentence contextthat made (1) the target predictable, (2) the neighbor predictable, or (3) neither predictable. We found that worse spellersrelied on sentence context in both foveal and parafoveal vision whereas better spellers only relied on context in parafovealvision, suggesting that both visual quality and lexical quality affect reliance on sentence context to identify words.

Designing Referential Descriptions for Children, Young Adults, and Computers: A Comprehensive Examination of Talker Informativity

Research on referential communication has explored talkers’ ability to tailor descriptions for the current context. The present study examines this issue alongside talker adaptations for different addressees. Participants were asked to provide a child, adult, or computer with instructions to select and move objects on a display. Each target object was either unique or accompanied by a same-category competitor. Targets in the latter condition could be differentiated with either a modifier or subordinate term. In addition to examining speech onset latencies, we analyzed referential descriptions for informational adequacy (just enough, underinformative, overinformative), noun type (basic-level or subordinate), and incidence/type of modifiers. The most noticeable effects were observed when addressing children, with participants using more basic terms and more modifiers (particularly color). These results reveal the spontaneous adaptation of referential strategies according to audience type, providing evidence for models of language in which speakers actively consider addressees' needs and cognitive abilities.

Boundary Extension in Response to Food: Exploring the Role of Appetitiveness

Boundary extension (BE) is a cognitive phenomenon in which people seem to misperceive visual scenes. Increasedattention and emotion have been shown to reduce or reverse the effects of BE (e.g., Mathews & Mackintosh, 2004). Wouldpeople for whom food is highly appetitive (vs. not) have similar responses when shown photographs containing food (vs.no food)? Our hypothesis was not supported: All participants experienced BE. More BE was observed in response to food(vs. nonfood) photographs, but this difference was more pronounced for those who served as controls and less pronouncedfor those who think of food as highly appetitive. We suggest that having similar perceptual experiences in response to food(vs. nonfood) photographs might be related to difficulties involving the inhibition of automatic behaviors (e.g., Mobbs etal., 2010) but argue that more research is needed to determine whether BE could be used for clinical purposes.

An empirical estimate of the dimensionality of face space

Learned generative models of human identity and appearance are typically high dimensional. However, social perceptionof faces is low dimensional. What is the dimensionality of face space in the mind of an observer? To estimate thisdimensionality, we begin with a simple observation: for any given person, there are many unrelated people who looksimilar to them. Next, we note that the very concept of strong resemblance exists only in low-dimensional spaces; inhigh-dimensional spaces, even nearest neighbors are far apart. Therefore, face space is of low dimensionality. How low?Using the scaling relationship between dimensionality and nth-nearest-neighbor distances, we empirically estimate thedimensionality of face space by measuring the ratio of JNDs between random pairs of faces and faces paired with theirnearest neighbors. We empirically estimate this ratio to be 0.76 [0.73, 0.79; 90% CI], which implies a dimensionality ofhuman face space between 7 and 12 dimensions.

Evidence for a Community of Knowledge Across Culture

We tested an implication of the community of knowledge hypothesis, that people fail to distinguish their own knowledge from other people’s knowledge in a collectivist society (China) as they do in individualistic societies like the United States. As predicted, despite the absence of any actual explanatory information, people rated their own understanding of novel natural and economic phenomena as higher when they were told that experts understood the phenomena than when they were told that experts did not yet understand them. This suggests that the community of knowledge effect may hold across cultures.

Resource management across brain regions supports auditory and visual-spatialprocessing in older age: An ERSP Study

Investigating how the brain integrates multi-modal information is critical for understanding the deleterious effects of ageon performance for tasks that integrate visual and auditory stimuli (e.g., driving or flying). We report on how auditoryprocessing was impacted by age during the encoding and maintenance phases of a visual-spatial task using electroen-cephalography in a sample of 10 older (50-80 years) and 10 younger (18-32 years) participants. Event-related spectralperturbation analyses reveal how both the online processing and memory stages of visual-spatial working memory tasksaffected auditory processes differentially across the age groups. Results reveal that older age may restrict the resourcesavailable for online processing of auditory information, particularly in brain regions that are also normally lateralizedfor visual-spatial tasks. Our findings point to the importance of designing interfaces, such as those found in aircraft orautomobiles, that support optimal performance and accommodate normal age-related changes in neural processes.

Resource-rational Task Decompositionto Minimize Planning Costs

People often plan hierarchically. That is, rather than planningover a monolithic representation of a task, they decompose thetask into simpler subtasks and then plan to accomplish those.Although much work explores how people decompose tasks,there is less analysis of why people decompose tasks in theway they do. Here, we address this question by formalizingtask decomposition as a resource-rational representation prob-lem. Specifically, we propose that people decompose tasks ina manner that facilitates efficient use of limited cognitive re-sources given the structure of the environment and their ownplanning algorithms. Using this model, we replicate severalexisting findings. Our account provides a normative explana-tion for how people identify subtasks as well as a frameworkfor studying how people reason, plan, and act using resource-rational representations.

Infants infer different types of social relations from giving and taking actions

Anthropological observations suggest that specific sharingbehaviors may predictably covary with specific relationalcontexts, and thus can be used as relationally informativecues. Given their limited social experiences, cultural novices,such as infants, should be particularly likely to rely on thesecues to discover the relational makeup of their socialsurroundings on the basis of sparse observations. The presentstudy examines a particular hypothesis derived from thisproposal, namely that infants interpret giving as indicative ofsocial relations based on the principle of even balance. Bysystematically contrasting infants’ representation of giving tothat of superficially similar taking events, we showed that 12-month-olds, despite being equally likely to infer dyadicrelations from the observation of either transferring action(Exps. 1-4), infants encoded the direction of resource transferonly in the representation of giving (Exp. 5-6), and,conversely, transitively inferred novel relations only forsocial structures composed of taking relations (Exp. 7-8). Webelieve that the distinct inferences elicited by the observationof the two transferring actions reflects fundamentaldifferences in the models regulating the relations respectivelyinferred: one (for giving) based on a principle of evenbalance, which motivates the monitoring of changes inresource flow in the ongoing relation; the other (for taking),based on a principle of social equivalence, which gives rise totransitive social structure.

Investigating the role of student achievement goals in conceptual physics learning

Helping students develop a conceptual understanding of physics is a critical goal of physics education. To better understandconceptual learning in physics, we investigated individual differences in students achievement goals, and their relationto learning. Past work suggests that mastery-approach goals predict conceptual learning and transfer, whereas othergoals do not (Belenky & Nokes-Malach, 2013). However, little work has tested this prediction using different types ofphysics learning outcomes. In this study, students completed pre and post achievement goal surveys and received differenttypes of instruction, followed by an extensive learning assessment. As expected, we found that mastery-approach goalswere positively related to conceptual learning outcomes, whereas performance-approach goals were not. Unexpectedly,performance-avoidance goals, while not related to mastery-approach goals, were also predictive of conceptual learningunder some conditions. We discuss the implications of these results for theories of motivation and learning.

Describing and Comprehending Change in Quantitative Information

We investigate how people understand English text that describes changes in a numeric quantity over time. We hypothesizethat people find it easier to comprehend text that specifies the starting quantity and ending quantity in chronological order,in contrast to how some news media tend to report this type of information, stating the ending quantity first, presumablybecause the ending quantity is the ”news”. Our hypothesis is that it is more difficult for readers to comprehend a sentencepresenting quantities in reverse chronological order, requiring more processing time by the reader and leading to reducedaccuracy in answering follow-up questions about the quantities. The results of an experiment supported the hypothesis.This finding has theoretical implications for models of text comprehension, and practical implications for how to commu-nicate technical material in newspapers, educational texts teaching or requiring the use of quantitative information, andtests and assessments based on reading passages.

Frequency-dependent Regularization in Constituent Ordering Preferences

We examine how idiosyncrasies of specific verbs in syntac-tic constructions affect constituent ordering preferences. Pre-vious work on binomial expressions in English has demon-strated that the polarization of ordering preferences for a givenbinomial type depends on its overall frequency. The higherthe frequency of a binomial type, the stronger and more ex-treme preference/regularization language users will have forone alternative over the other (e.g. “facts and techniques” >“techniques and facts”; “bread and butter” >>> “butter andbread”). Here using the dative constructions in English as thetest case, we show that the same frequency-dependent regular-ization exists in syntactic structures above the word level. Themore frequent a dative construction type is, governed by thehead verb, the stronger preference there is for one alternationover the other. Further, we present evidence that the regulariza-tion patterns can be accounted for via iterated learning model-ing of language change, suggesting that frequency-dependentregularization emerges via the interactions between languageproduction, language learning and cultural transmission.

The benefits of practice with interruptions is step-specific

In two studies we investigated the effect of resumption practice following an interruption at the same step in a Computerized Physician Order Entry system (CPOE). The results of both studies showed that error rate decreased with increasing amounts of resumption practice. One reason people may have resumed more accurately following an interruption is improvement in a general resumption process. If true, we would expect that participants could be interrupted at any step in a task and show improved resumption with increased practice. Instead, our results suggest that repeatedly resuming from the same step likely produces associative priming between a specific task, interruption, and step. The associative priming allowed participants to resume more successfully with additional interruption practice, but only for that task-interruption-step triplet.

Dollar Sense? The Relationship Between Numeracy, Financial Management andEstimation of Cart Total After Shopping

Our sense of space, time, and number is well documented, but do we have a similar sense for money? Like the ability tosense the passage of time, can we sense the accumulation of expenses and make accurate estimates? The present studyinvestigated the ability to estimate grocery cart totals, and whether it relates to number sense and financial managementbehaviors. Participants were asked which of two options (same product: one bigger, one smaller, with different price-to-amount ratios) they would purchase. Afterwards, participants completed the Abbreviated Numeracy Scale as a distractortask. Participants were then asked to estimate the total cost of all the items they chose during the decision-making task.We found that greater numeracy skills and financial management behaviors predicted better estimation skills. Those withgreater numeracy skills were also more likely to consider price-to-amount ratios during decision-making and to choose thebetter deal.

Where does the conceptual spacetime asymmetry come from?

Why do people use space to think about time more than vice versa? On one account, a spacetime asymmetry in languagegives rise to the spacetime asymmetry in thought. If so, children should learn that polysemous words like long and shorthave primarily spatial meanings on the basis of language statistics. Yet usage statistics from which children could inferthe primacy of space are not obviously available in adult-to-child speech: Instead, caregivers use long and short moreoften in temporal senses than spatial senses (Casasanto & Ksa, 2019). Here we corroborate this result using word2vec, avector space model that reflects the co-occurrence structure of words. We show that the spacetime asymmetry is also notavailable in this semantic space: more words surrounding long and short are temporal than spatial. Rather than emergingfrom language, the spacetime asymmetry may reflect perceptual or conceptual asymmetries that precede the acquisition ofspatio-temporal language.

New insights from daylong audio transcripts of children’s language environments

Recent technological advances and research trends have enabled the collection and analysis of multi-hour or daylong recordings of children’s auditory environment. While this technology has allowed researchers to sample language experience from multiple contexts across the day, challenges remain with respect to how these audio recordings can or should be coded and analyzed. Daylong audio samples have the potential to transform our understanding of the language input that children encounter, but new analysis techniques may be necessary to take advantage of these new opportunities. The present work explores the linguistic content of the transcripts of three daylong recordings with the goal of understanding the content of these recordings in order to develop new ways to analyze and gain insight from these recordings.

Comparing the effects of frontal and temporal neurostimulation on second language learning

Successful language learning requires a dynamic balance between declarative and procedural mechanisms, yet individuals may engage them in less than optimal ways. The goal of the current experiment was to determine whether transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can tip the balance, specifically facilitating declarative or procedural learning. Seventy-nine subjects (31 no stimulation, 16 sham stimulation, 16 temporal, 16 frontal) completed an artificial grammar learning task followed by a two-alternative forced- choice test measuring sensitivity to the underlying grammar (procedural) versus the surface form (declarative). The pattern of results is consistent with separate engagement of declarative and procedural systems. Left temporal stimulation resulted in higher selection of strings with familiar surface features. In contrast, frontal stimulation resulted in a slower learning trajectory and more frequent selection of grammatical letter strings. We conclude that tDCS may be used to facilitate engagement of different learning systems required for language learning.

Quantitative Analyses of Gaze Duration from the viewpoints of Grounding Acts, Conversation Topic, and Linguistic Proficiency

Although many studies have analyzed the communicative functions of gaze, it is still unclear how linguistic proficiency and communication contexts affect gazing activities. Quantitative analyses of speaker’s and listener’s gaze were conducted taking the factors of grounding in communication, conversation topic, and linguistic proficiency into consideration. The results showed that the duration of a listener’s gaze is much longer during utterances that convey new information while the duration of a speaker’s gaze did not show much difference, suggesting that the characteristics of the grounding act factor affect a listener’s gazing activities but not those of the speaker. We also observed that linguistic proficiency and conversation topic have a much greater effect on the listener’s gaze. The results will contribute to multi-party interaction studies that examine the effect of linguistic proficiency, and provide valuable information that could assist in the design of interaction support systems for users with different linguistic proficiency.

Semantic influences on emergent preferences of word order: Evidence from silent gesture

Across the world’s languages, some word orders are more common. We focus on noun phrases, where it is more common for adjectives to follow the noun than to precede it. Because the interpretation of adjectives depends on the noun they modify, we propose and evaluate the new hypothesis that the order N- ADJ is more prevalent because it is beneficial for semantic processing. In a silent gesture task, speakers of four typologically-unrelated languages (English, Mandarin, Arabic and Spanish) communicated noun phrase meanings to a partner. We find, first, that our task tracks the typologically- preferred orders of nouns, adjectives and numerals in the noun phrase. More importantly, we find support for our semantic processing hypothesis: size adjectives, whose interpretation depend more on the noun they modify, were more likely to be gestured after the noun than shape adjectives whose interpretation is less dependent on the noun they modify.

Storage and Computation of Multimorphemic Words in Turkish

Whether morphologically complex words are stored as a whole or decomposed into constituents has been well-investigatedexperimentally in Indo-European languages like English, Italian, Dutch and French. There is substantial evidence in theselanguages in favor of and architecture which allows both decomposition and storage. This study investigates how mor-phologically complex words involving two or more morphemes are represented in Turkish which, unlike Indo-Europeanlanguages, is renowned for its highly rich morphology. Applying a probabilistic tradeoff-based model of morphologicalstorage and computation (ODonnell 2015) to a corpus of Turkish word forms, we derive predictions about stored patternsin the language. We discuss these patterns and propose several for future experimental investigation.

An Aha! Walks into a Bar: Joke Completion as a Form of Insight Problem Solving

The present work introduces a new insight problem task: joke completion. We found that performance and magnitude of insight within it correlated with an established task: rebus puzzles. However, participants performed worse on and took longer in joke completion problems than in their rebus counterparts. Further, the distribution of reported insight was bimodal only for rebuses, as should be expected of an insight problem. In joke completion problems, both self-estimated and externally-rated joke funniness correlated with reported insight. Challenging the assumption of impasse, performance and insight decreased as a function of trial time for both problem types, with the best and most insightful solutions submitted within the first 20 seconds. While this is a preliminary study, we argue that it signals a promising direction for the problem solving, humor, and creativity literatures by providing a new approach to capture insight in a manner conducive to linguistic and cognitive modeling techniques.

Exploring Dynamic Decision Making Strategies withRecurrence Quantification Analysis

Aggregate statistics, such as percentage of choices, drive manyinsights about sequential behavior in decision making re-search. However, aggregation leaves usable information andpotential insights unexamined. Here, we introduce the useof recurrence plots (RP) and recurrence quantification anal-ysis (RQA) to explore individual choice sequences and de-termine generalized patterns of decision making strategies ina dynamic decision task. We illustrate the insights that RPsand RQAs reveal in a data set collected in a past study in-volving a dynamic, binary choice task (McCormick et al., inpreparation). Patterns of recurrence reveal multiple, distin-guishable, individual choice patterns among participants whowere equally successful in adapting to the dynamic environ-ment. We discuss how RQA of choice behavior can augmentour understanding of decision strategies when paired with tra-ditional aggregate assessments.

The fine structure of surprise in intuitive physics: when, why, and how much?

We are surprised when events violate our intuitive physicalexpectations. Even infants look longer when things seem tomagically teleport or vanish. This important surprise signalhas been used to probe what infants expect, in order to studythe most basic representations of objects. But these studiesrely on binary measures – an event is surprising, or not. Here,we study surprise in a more precise, quantitative way, usingthree distinct measures: we ask adults to judge how surprisinga scene is, when that scene is surprising, and why it is surpris-ing. We find good consistency in the level of surprise reportedacross these experiments, but also crucial differences in theimplied explanations of those scenes. Beyond this, we showthat the timing and degree of surprise can be explained by anobject-based model of intuitive physics.

Morphological Parsing by Foveal Split: Evidence from Anaglyphs

We investigated the early moments of visual word recognition,when the retinal information—by hypothesis split verticallyalong the fovea—is divided into two visual pathways,projecting the right visual field into the left hemisphere (LH),and the left visual field into the right hemisphere (RH).Wearing red/blue anaglyph glasses, participants performed alexical decision task to compounds (FOOTBALL) andmonomorphemic words that were either pseudo-compounds(CARPET) or unsegmentable (JINGLE). The stimuli werepresented (masked, 60 ms exposure) in three colorcombinations: all black, red/blue (ipsilateral visual pathways),and blue/red (contralateral pathways). For the red/blue andblue/red conditions, the colors were split either at themorpheme boundary (legal split) or at a character to the left orto the right of the split (illegal split). We found an advantage(RT and accuracy) of compounds over non-compounds,independent of pathway, and an advantage of legal vs. illegalconstituent split. Results suggest that the visual wordrecognition system performs parsing analyses that are inconsonant with the morphological objects of the language. Theadvantage of pseudo-compounds over unsegmentablessuggests that at an early—pre-lexical—stage the system ispartially insensitive to the semantic properties of the wholeword.

Characterizing the relationship between lexical and morphological development

In learning morphology, do children generalize from their vocabularies on an item-by-item basis, or do they form globalrules on a developmental timetable? We use large-scale parent-report data to address this question by investigating relationsamong morphological development, vocabulary growth, and age. For three languages, we examine irregular verbs (e.g.go) and predict childrens correct inflection (went) and overregularization (goed/wented). Morphology knowledge relatesstrongly to vocabulary, more so than to age. Further, this relation is modulated by age: for two children with the samevocabulary size, the older is more likely to correctly inflect and overregularize, and the effect of vocabulary on morphologydecreases with age. Lastly, correct inflection and overregularization rates rise in tandem over age, and vocabulary effectson them are correlated across items. Our findings support that morphology learning is strongly coupled to lexical learningand that correct inflection and overregularization are related, verb-specific, processes.

Abstract strategy learning underlies flexible transfer in physical problem solving

What do people learn when they repeatedly try to solve a set ofrelated problems? In a set of three different exploratory phys-ical problem solving experiments, participants consistentlylearn strategies rather than generically better world models.Participants selectively transferred these strategies when thecrucial context and preconditions of the strategy were met,such as needing to “catapult”, “support”, “launch” or “desta-bilize” an object in the scene to accomplish their goals. Weshow that these strategies are parameterized: people can ad-just their strategies to account for new object weights despiteno direct interaction experience with these objects. Taken to-gether, these results suggest that people can make use of lim-ited experience to learn abstract strategies that go beyond sim-ple model-free policies and are instead object-oriented, adapt-able, and can be parameterized by model-based variables suchas weight.

Cognitive fluency and the spread of news on social media

What drives someone to share news stories online? Prior research has identified some possible factors: qualities of the newsconsumer, the news stories themselves and the news consumption environment. We explore an additional factor: cognitivefluency. Cognitive fluency, the ease with which a user reads and comprehends headlines, predicts the rate of sharing ofnews stories. We quantify over 100,000 stories from major news outlets from 2017 and use a bespoke rate-of-sharingmetric, determined by the rate a story was shared on Twitter shortly after appearing on an outlets RSS feed. Cognitivefluency was expressed in cognitive processing (English Lexicon Project). The effect of cognitive fluency is detectable butsmall, and may vary across news outlets. This suggests fluency may serve as a gating mechanism to the propagation ofnews online. We discuss the theoretical implications of this relationship: cognitive constraints of consumers, the structureof the news ecosystem and relationships between these levels of analysis.

Order Effects in One-shot Causal Generalization

We introduce a novel task exploring how people make causal generalizations over the abstract features of the objectsinvolved in a causal interaction. Specifically, we investigate how people generalize from a single observation of two sim-ple objects in which one (the agent, or cause) interacts with another (the recipient, or effect) resulting in some featurechange(s). In line with recent demonstrations of human strength in few-shot concept learning, we find strong and sys-tematic patterns of generalizations that are well explained by a Bayesian inference model favoring simpler causal rules.However, we also identify a clear order effect depending on what order generalizations are made. To capture the observedpatterns, we develop a causal hypothesis generation model that takes peoples natural generalization tendency and the ordereffect into consideration, and outperforms plain Bayesian inference both in computational efficiency and in match to thebehavioral data.

Exact number concepts depend on language

The ability to represent large exact numbers is unique to humans. On some proposals, this capacity depends crucially onlanguage; learning the count list (”one”, ”two”, ”three”, etc.) allows children to represent the exact cardinality of numberslarger than four. On alternative proposals, this ability depends not on language but on innate pre-verbal counting processes.Here, we conducted a non-verbal test of large exact number concepts in the Tsimane’, an indigenous Amazonian culturein which adults vary widely in their knowledge of the verbal count list. Participants correctly matched the number ofobjects in a response set to the number in a sample set but only for cardinalities that were within their verbal count range.For larger cardinalities, they reproduced sets that were only approximately matched in number. The findings challengeaccounts that posit pre-verbal number concepts and support the Whorfian view that language can enable new conceptualabilities.

Show or Tell? Demonstration is More Robust toChanges in Shared Perception than Explanation

Successful teaching entails a complex interaction between ateacher and a learner. The teacher must select and convey in-formation based on what they think the learner perceives andbelieves. Teaching always involves misaligned beliefs, butstudies of pedagogy often focus on situations where teachersand learners share perceptions. Nonetheless, a teacher andlearner may not always experience or attend to the same as-pects of the environment. Here, we study how misaligned per-ceptions influence communication. We hypothesize that theefficacy of different forms of communication depends on theshared perceptual state between teacher and learner. We de-velop a cooperative teaching game to test whether concretemediums (demonstrations, or “showing”) are more robust thanabstract ones (language, or “telling”) when the teacher andlearner are not perceptually aligned. We find evidence that (1)language-based teaching is more affected by perceptual mis-alignment, but (2) demonstration-based teaching is less likelyto convey nuanced information. We discuss implications forhuman pedagogy and machine learning.

The face inversion effect and the anatomical mapping from the visual field to theprimary visual cortex

The face-inversion effect, or the drastic decrease in accuracyseen when a participant is asked to identify inverted faces whencompared to upright faces, is an effect that is not found in objectinversion. Here we suggest a new explanation of this effect usingcomputational models to show that the phenomenon can beexplained by the anatomical mapping from the visual field toprimary visual cortex. We propose that the way inverted faces aremapped onto the cortex is fundamentally different from the wayupright faces are mapped. Our work first shows the advantages ofthis mapping due to its scale and rotation invariance when used asinput to a convolutional neural network. We train the network toperform recognition tasks and show it exhibits scale andrealistically constrained rotation invariance. We then confirm thatthe decline in accuracy seen when a participant is asked to identifyinverted faces is not seen in the network with inverted objectrecognition tasks. With the support of these two findings, we testthe face-inversion effect on our network and are able to show theunique decline in accuracy, suggesting that the way the visual fieldis mapped onto the primary visual cortex is a key facet in themanifestation of this effect.

Influences of both prior knowledge and recent historyon visual working memory

Existing knowledge shapes and distorts our memories, serv-ing as a prior for newly encoded information. Here, we in-vestigate the role of stable long-term priors (e.g. categoricalknowledge) in conjunction with priors arising from recentlyencountered information (e.g. ’serial dependence’) in visualworking memory for color. We use an iterated reproductionparadigm to allow a model-free assessment of the role of suchpriors. In Experiment 1, we find that participants’ reports re-liably converge to certain areas of color space, but that thisconvergence is largely distinct for different individuals, sug-gesting responses are biased by more than just shared categoryknowledge. In Experiment 2, we explicitly manipulate trialn-1 and find recent history plays a major role in participants’reports. Thus, we find that both global prior knowledge and re-cent trial information have biasing influences on visual work-ing memory, demonstrating an important role for both short-and long-term priors in actively maintained information.

What else could happen? Two-, three-, and four-year-olds use variabilityinformation to infer novel causal outcomes

Young children rapidly infer causal relations by trackingcontingencies between causes and their effects, and cangeneralize these rules to novel instances of the same cause.However, this is distinct from the ability to make inferencesabout whether a particular cause is likely to produce noveleffects. Here, we investigate the development of two-, three-,and four-year-olds’ ability to recognize and use informationabout a cause’s variability to make predictions about othernovel outcomes it might produce. Experiment 1 finds thatchildren as young as two years of age infer that a cause thathas produced variable, rather than deterministic outcomes ismore likely to produce a novel, previously unobserved effect.Experiment 2 finds that four-year-olds, but not two- andthree-year-olds, infer that a higher variability cause is morelikely to produce a novel outcome than a lower variabilitycause.

Linguistic Simplification in Human-Computer Interaction: Implications for theCognitive Foundations of Language

How can a range of syntactic variation within a language be explained, particularly when linguistic expressions producedby native speakers in one context clearly violate syntactic norms? To answer this question, we investigate the properties ofinformation requests that arise in the context of human-computer interaction, such as ’most home runs player 1975 age’.The results of a production study that compares the structural complexity of information requests in human-computervs. human-human condition show that participants in the former condition tend to use simpler syntactic structures andfewer relative clauses, compared to that in the human-human condition, despite syntactic priming. Our results suggestthat speakers in the human-computer context utilize a qualitatively different type of formal grammar, linear grammar(Jackendoff & Wittenberg, 2017) as opposed to hierarchical grammar. The study contributes to the theoretical discussionon what constitutes a lower bound on complexity in language (cf. Futrell et al., 2016).

Verbal labels promote representational alignment even in the absence ofcommunication

What affects whether one person represents an item in a similarway to another person? We examined the role of verbal labelsin promoting representational alignment. Three groups ofparticipants sorted novel shapes on perceived similarity. Priorto sorting, participants in two of the groups were pre-exposedto the shapes using a simple visual matching task and in one ofthese groups, shapes were accompanied by one of two novelcategory labels. Exposure with labels led people to representthe shapes in a more categorical way and to increasedalignment between sorters, despite the two categories beingvisually distinct and participants in both pre-exposureconditions receiving identical visual experience of the shapes.Results hint that labels play a role in aligning people's mentalrepresentations, even in the absence of communication.

Similarity judgments determine consistency of implicit number conceptions acrossages

Previous work has used pairwisesimilarityjudgments among numerals to reveal development in conceptions ofnumber,from exclusively attending to magnitude in kindergarten to including properties likeparityin middle school. In adulthood,these representations appear to settle on more advanced number properties. We extend this work with the goal of observingindividual rather than group-level number representations by administering pairwise similarity tasks at two separate timepoints to determine individual consistency. Specifically, we use two 10-item number (and kinship term for comparison)sets exemplifying a variety of mathematical concepts (e.g., primeness) to 48 students across grades 3-7. Multidimensionalscaling analyses reveal magnitude as the most pervasive feature and reflect differences in attended numerical featuresrelative to score on a math assessment. Analyses are ongoing, but the consistency of this measure in a short time-framewill validate its usability as a subtle pre- and post-test surrounding concept-specific education or interventions.

Linguistic stability and change under small-scale egalitarian language contact: amixture model approach

This paper investigates the outcomes of small-scale egalitar-ian language contact in an attempt to address whether differentlinguistic domains exhibit different degrees of stability and re-sistance to convergence among cohabitant speakers of Jahaiand Jedek, two closely related Aslian (Austroasiatic) languagevarieties spoken in northern Peninsular Malaysia. Using non-parametric Bayesian mixture models, we find that basic vocab-ulary items show a signal that strongly matches the linguisticidentity of individuals, while data from other domains do not.This result is in agreement with other findings from the studyof language contact: basic vocabulary is said to be a domainwhere distinctions in linguistic identity are often emphasizedand maintained, while other parts of the vocabulary may beless salient for the purposes of indexing speaker identity, andare thus more prone to the effects of convergence. We demon-strate that this finding is an artifact of neither data coverage normodel choice; at the same time, we are able to identify varia-tion in basic vocabulary items across linguistic groups which issuppressed by the model we use, and outline alternative meth-ods for analyzing data of this sort.

A Re-Implementation of a Dynamic Field Theory Model of Mental Maps usingPython and Nengo

In Dynamic Field Theory (DFT) cognition is modeled as the interaction of a complex dynamical system. The connectionto the brain is established by smaller parts of this system, neural fields, that mimic the behavior of neuron populations. Wereimplemented a spatial reasoning model from DFT in Python using the Nengo framework to test if the models results canbe reproduced. Moreover we aimed at providing an alternative to the existing DFT implementations to facilitate futureresearch in that direction. Our results show that the proposed spatial reasoning model works as described since we wereable to duplicate both the behavior of single neural fields and the whole model. However, there are statistical differencesin performance between the two implementations, and future work is needed to determine the cause of these differences,and to increase the speed of the Python implementation.

Approximating mental representation of verbs using semantic graphs

One of the most important questions in language sciences is concerned with argument structure acquisition. Here, wefocus the role of semantically most general verbs in argument structure bootstrapping. We propose a novel computationalframework that combines word embedding techniques with theories of semantic representation. Using graph vertex degreeas an index of semantic generality, we rank the semantic generality of verbs that appear in select five selective argumentstructures, the ditransitive, the spray/load, the conative, the causative-inchoative and the active-passive alternations (Levin,1993), from three corpora of children and their caregivers language productions (MacWhinney, 2000). We found Zipfiandistributions of vertex degrees in all three corpora, where verbs in children’s language input are semantically more re-stricted than adult-to-adult interactions. Except for the ditransitive, semantic general verbs do not take high rank in thevertex degree, suggesting that semantic generality might not play a role as important as previously argued.

Modelling brain activity associated with metaphor processing with distributionalsemantic models

In this study we investigate how lexical-semantic relations as-sociated with the literal meaning (and abstract meaning) arebeing accessed across the brain during familiar metaphor com-prehension. We utilize a data-driven whole-brain searchlightsimilarity-decoding analysis. We contrast decoding metaphoricphrases (”she’s grasping the idea”) using distributional seman-tic models of the verb in the phrase (VERB model) versus thatof the more abstract verb-sense (PARAPHRASE VERB model)obtained from literal paraphrases of the metaphoric phrases(”she’s understanding the idea”). We showed successful decod-ing with the VERB model across frontal, temporal and parietallobes mainly within areas of the language and default-modenetworks. In contrast, decoding with the PARAPHRASE VERBmodel was restricted to frontal-temporal lobes within areas ofthe language-network which overlapped to some extent withsignificant decoding with the VERB model. Overall, the re-sults suggest that lexical-semantic relations closely associatedwith the abstract meaning in metaphor processing are largelylocalized to language and amodal (multimodal) semantic mem-ory systems of the brain, while those more associated withthe literal meaning are processed across a distributed seman-tic network including areas implicated in mental imagery andsocial-cognition.

On the Malleability and Stability of Ignoring Group-Level Effects

Co-operation and group-serving behaviour of group members has increasingly been acknowledged as essential to theflourishing of groups in general and the success of teams in organizations or companies in particular. Studying this,however, presupposes dissociating individual-level and group-level effects (involving a Simpsons Paradox). We havestarted investigating settings where true individual- and group-level effects could be dissociated in a learning paradigmconcerned with individuals in changing teams. Our results show that participants often evaluated the overall most effectivegroup-serving team-player much more negatively than all less effective non-interacting workers. This suggested a potentialTragedy of Personnel Selection, when personnel managers, relying on number-based outcomes, tend to ignore even strongand crucial group-level effects of team-players. Here we briefly summarize some findings and present an experiment,where we tried to improve participants ability to dissociate individual- from group-level effects, by explicitly providingthem with hypotheses about a team player.

The Relevance of Subjective Benefits in Risky Choice Across ten Domains of Life

In risk-research, there are two traditions of measurement: the attribute-based and the vignette-based tradition. The attribute- based approach focuses on the impact that the attributes (prob- abilities and outcomes) of risky options have on the processing of risk-related information. The vignette-based approach fo- cuses on responses to questions about contextualized situations involving risk. We bring these two approaches together here to investigate the stability of risk preferences and information processing in risky choice tasks across different contextualized situations. To this end, we employ an evidence-based multi- attribute gamified risky choice task in a retest design. The re- sults (N = 226) show that risk propensities are very stable within domains across time. Participants’ explicit beliefs about risks and returns did not accurately reflect the actual rank order of the costs and benefits of actions in the real world, which we obtained from statistical databases. Also, we find that that pro- spect theory’s risk-attitude parameters are mostly unrelated to the risk-taking in the contextualized task, and that benefit per- ceptions influence risk-taking, in line with a risk-return trade- off view on risk-taking.

Child-directed word associations reveal divergent semantic structure thatimproves models of early word learning

How words are associated within the linguistic environment conveys semantic content, and it is well known that adultsspeak differently to children than to other adults. We present results from a new word association study in which adultparticipants are instructed to produce either unconstrained or child-directed responses to each cue, where cues included674 nouns, verbs, and adjectives from the McArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Child-directedresponses consisted of higher frequency words with fewer letters and earlier ages of acquisition. The correlations amongthe responses generated for each pair of cues differed between unconstrained and child-directed responses, suggestingthat child-directed associations imply different semantic structure. A comparison of growth models guided by semanticnetwork structure revealed that child-directed associations are more predictive of early lexical growth. Thus, these newchild-directed word association norms may provide more clear insight into the semantic context of young children.

The language of causation

People use varied language to express their causal understand-ing of the world. But how does that language map onto peo-ple’s underlying representations, and how do people choosebetween competing ways to best describe what happened? Inthis paper we develop a model that integrates computationaltools for causal judgment and pragmatic inference to addressthese questions. The model has three components: a causalinference component which computes counterfactual simula-tions that capture whether and how a candidate cause madea difference to the outcome, a literal semantics that mapsthe outcome of these counterfactual simulations onto differentcausal expressions (such as “caused”, “enabled”, “affected”,or “made no difference”), and a pragmatics component thatconsiders how informative each causal expression would befor figuring out what happened. We test our model in an ex-periment that asks participants to select which expression bestdescribes what happened in video clips depicting physical in-teractions.

Memory enhancement from surprise: Investigating threshold and incrementalaccounts

How might surprise influence memory and learning? Isolating an item from an established category induces surprise andresults in better memory. However, it is less clear whether the degree of induced surprise correlates with better memory,or whether – regardless of degree –surprise simply triggers a uniform improvement in memory. To investigate whether thedegree of surprise has an incremental effect on memory outcomes, we gave 158 participants lists of words, varying thedegree to which a single word in the list surprisingly conflicted with the lists overarching category. Although there wasan overall boost in learning for surprising words, we found no evidence of an effect of amount of surprise on memory.Lack of evidence is not evidence of a lack, however these results provide some suggestive evidence for a threshold modelof memory enhancement from surprise. Distinguishing these accounts has important implications for affective models ofmemory and learning.

How speakers avoid gender ambiguous pronouns: A cross-linguistic study

We examined how speakers avoid gender ambiguous pronouns, exploiting cross-linguistic variations in French, Italian,and English. The gender congruence between two human referents led to fewer pronouns (more nouns) in both Frenchand English, whereas the grammatical gender congruence between two inanimate referents had no effect on the use ofpronouns in English, where grammatical gender is absent, as well as French, where grammatical gender is present. InItalian, gender congruence did not affect the use of null pronouns in all conditions, which are ambiguous regardless. Theresults are compatible with the non-linguistic competition account: Speakers avoid gender ambiguous pronouns only whenthe gender congruence increases their non-linguistic similarity.

Where for what: A meta-analysis for the category-specific activationsfor living/nonliving concepts in the past two decades

The cortical organization of the semantic network has beenstudied extensively in neuropsychological and neuroimagingstudies. Recent theories have heavily relied on theobservation of category-specific activations, i.e., thepreferential activations in brain regions for specific semanticcategories. With decades of research, a full understanding ofthe organization has not yet been reached, since little isknown about the factors that contribute to the variances inobserved activation patterns across numerous neuroimagingstudies. In this study, we first reviewed 97 published papersthat reported category-specific activations for living ornonliving concepts in the past two decades. Then, using theActivation Likelihood Estimate (ALE) method, wecharacterized the brain activation associated with living andnonliving concepts, revealing the influences of relevantfactors (e.g., neuroimaging mode, task demands, and stimulimodality), and analyzing these findings in relation totheoretical accounts of cortical semantic networks.

Changes in cortical networks during motor imagery and action observation ofwalking

Motor imagery (MI) and action observation (AO) are cognitive motor processes. Previous studies have examined themodulation of corticospinal excitability, spinal reflex excitability, and cortical activity during MI and AO. However, howthe cortical network changes during these processes were still unknown. Here, this study investigated the cortical networkchanges during MI, AO, and MI combined with AO (MI+AO) by analyzing changes of phase relations (phase synchronyanalysis). 64-ch electroencephalographic signals were recorded from twelve healthy males while they were performingMI, AO, and MI+AO of walking. In our results, phase desynchronization occurred between the sensorimotor areas andthe visual areas during AO and MI+AO, while MI by itself did not cause phase desynchronization. These results suggestthat AO changes cortical connectivity between the sensorimotor and visual areas while the cortical connectivity staysduring MI. These findings have implications for understanding the cortical network changes induced by cognitive motorprocesses.

Modeling Second Language Preposition Learning

Hundreds of millions of people learn a second language (L2).1When learning a specific L2, there are common errors for na-tive speakers of a given L1 language, suggesting specific ef-fects of L1 on L2 learning. Nevertheless, language instruc-tion materials are designed based only on L2. We developa computational model that mimics the behavior of a non-native speaker of a specific language to provide a deeper un-derstanding of the problem of learning a second language. Weuse a Naive Bayes to model prepositional choices in English(L2) by native Mandarin (L1) speakers. Our results show thatboth correct and incorrect responses can be explained by thelearner’s L1 information. Moreover, our model predicts incor-rect choices with no explicit training data of non-native mis-takes. Our results thus provide a new medium to analyze anddevelop tools for L2 teaching.

Simulating length and frequency effects across multiple tasks with the Bayesianmodel BRAID-Phon

In visual word processing modeling, few models have success-fully accounted for a large variety of tasks, and large corpora ofbehavioral observations. We consider a dataset from a megas-tudy, in which participants performed three tasks (lexical de-cision, word naming, and word recognition in a progressivedemasking situation), on the same, large set of stimuli. Wedefine the BRAID-Phon model, an extension of a previousprobabilistic model, the BRAID model, whose originality isits visuo-attentional component, in which a visuo-attentionaldistribution spatially deploys sensory processing capabilities.BRAID-Phon includes phonological representations of words,allowing simulating the naming task. We simulated the threetasks on the dataset we considered, and analyzed predicted re-action times in terms of word frequency and word length ef-fects. Simulation results show that BRAID-Phon successfullycaptures the direction and order of magnitude of the observedeffects, in all three tasks.

Dipole sources localization of alpha activity in EEG neurofeedback training.

The neurofeedback training-induced alpha activity have been observed over widespread brain regions on topographicelectroencephalogram analysis. However, the generation mechanism of the alpha activity has not been clarified yet. Thepresent study was aimed to identify sources of the alpha activity through four different temporal/spectral analytic tech-niques, i.e., max peak average, positive average, negative average and event-related spectral perturbation average methods.Twenty participants were trained through 12 sessions by receiving feedback of alpha amplitude and showed significantalpha amplitude increment. The alpha activities were averaged through four different methods for dipole source analysis.Similar results from four methods showed that the sources of the alpha activity clustered in precuneus, posterior cingulatecortex and middle temporal gyrus. Our findings indicated that alpha activity is trainable through our NFT protocol. Thethree brain regions play important roles for enhancing the training-induced alpha activity.

Visual Attention during E-Learning: Eye-tracking Shows that Making Salient Areas More Prominent Helps Learning in Online Tutors

In this study, we investigate how high- and low-performance learners (N=12) act differently while using a cognitive tutoring system. We examine three research questions: (1) Can we predict learners’ performance using only their visual attention (eye movement data)? (2) Can we predict learners’ performance from visual attention data and initial performance? (3) Are age, gender, first language, where they look, and the sequence of Areas of Interests (AOIs) significant factors in the learners’ performance? Learners more correctly answer questions taken from larger rather than smaller AOIs. Our results show that high-performance learners pay more attention to the content that contains answers to later questions. Surprisingly, the tutor did not change the learners’ visual search to a goal-oriented search. Our analyses can help instructional designers create a more productive learning experience because visual search behavior as part of a learner model with acceptable accuracy in early stages can be used in adaptive tutors. Additionally, we trained a classifier on the eye movement data to predict learners’ performance for each question. Its results provide a list of suggestions for designing more productive learning experiences, such as enticing user attention by increasing the size of the content that contains answers and changing the order of contents.

Interleaving facilitates the rapid formation of distributed representations

Distributed representations, in which information is encodedin overlapping populations of neuronal units, are essential tothe remarkable success of artificial neural networks (ANNs) inmany domains, and have been posited to be employed through-out the brain, especially in neocortex. A fundamental signatureof ANNs employing distributed representations is that learningrequires exposure to information in an interleaved order; expo-sure to new information in a blocked order tends to overwriteprior knowledge (i.e., ’catastrophic interference’). Because itis difficult to match human learning to the learning conditionsof these networks, it is not known whether human learning ex-hibits these properties, which, if true, would implicate use ofsimilar representations. To test this, we leveraged a recent pro-posal that parts of the hippocampus host distributed represen-tations of the kind typically ascribed to neocortex, and adopteda hippocampally dependent task that contrasts the effects of in-terleaved versus blocked learning on a short timescale. Exper-iments 1a and 1b demonstrate that interleaved exposure facili-tates the rapid perception of shared structure across items. Ex-periment 2 shows that only interleaved exposure permits use-ful inference when item associations need to be inferred basedon statistical regularities. Together, these results demonstratethe power of interleaved learning and implicate the use of dis-tributed representations in human rapid learning of structuredinformation.

A naturalistic fMRI investigation into the possible co-evolution of language andtechnology

Recent findings of activation of language networks in the brain during stone tool manufacture support hypotheses aboutthe co-evolution of language and technology. Our study replicates these findings and demonstrates that distinct toolmakingbehaviors and levels of expertise affect how reliably these networks are activated. Subjects, including expert toolmakers(n = 7) and untrained participants (n = 10), watched naturalistic videos of an expert toolmaker making three technologiesand imagined themselves performing the same actions as the toolmaker while being scanned. We performed event-relatedGLM analyses on our data, focusing on activation during observation and flaking. All technologies recruited networksinvolved in language production and comprehension, including IFG, vPMC, dPMC, SPL, IPL, and pMTG. Flaking en-gaged language networks more reliably than observation. Our study considers whether expertise is required for Oldowan,Acheulean, and Levallois comprehension by exploring the extent to which activation in language networks increases withtool complexity.

Papers accepted as Talks, appearing in proceedings only

A Neural Network Model of Lexical Competitionduring Infant Spoken Word Recognition

Visual world studies show that upon hearing a word in a target-absent visual context containing related and unrelated items,toddlers and adults briefly direct their gaze towards phonolog-ically related items, before shifting towards semantically andvisually related ones. We present a neural network model thatprocesses dynamic unfolding phonological representations andmaps them to static internal semantic and visual representa-tions. The model, trained on representations derived from realcorpora, simulates this early phonological over semantic/visualpreference. Our results support the hypothesis that incremen-tal unfolding of a spoken word is in itself sufficient to ac-count for the transient preference for phonological competi-tors over both unrelated and semantically and visually relatedones. Phonological representations mapped dynamically in abottom-up fashion to semantic-visual representations capturethe early phonological preference effects reported in a visualworld task. The semantic-visual preference observed later insuch a trial does not require top-down feedback from a seman-tic or visual system.

An associative learning account for retrieval-induced forgetting

Retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF) is a paradigm where re-peated study and cue-based retrieval of words impair retrievalof related, but unstudied, words. We present a process model,situated in the ACT-R/E cognitive architecture, that accountsfor the RIF task using the architecture’s overarching theory ofassociative learning. In this theory, studying words strengthenstheir association with their related cues; this, in turn, weakensthe association between those cues and any other words theyare related to. We show this account fits a recent dataset thatexplores cueing in the RIF task (Perfect et al., 2004).

A Phylogenetic Perspective on Distributed Decision-Making Mechanisms

This paper challenges a common assumption about decision- making mechanisms in humans: decision-making is a distinctively high-level cognitive activity implemented by mechanisms concentrated in the higher-level areas of the cortex. We argue instead that human behavior is controlled by a multiplicity of highly distributed, heterarchically organized decision-making mechanisms. We frame it in terms of control mechanisms that procure and evaluate information to select activities of controlled mechanisms and adopt a phylogenetic perspective, showing how decision-making is realized in control mechanisms in a variety of species. We end by discussing this picture's implication for high-level cognitive decision-making.

An efficient communication analysis of morpho-syntactic grammatical features

Grammatical features vary widely across languages and thisvariation has been studied in detail. The functions of gram-matical features, however, are not entirely clear and a numberof puzzles remain. For example, why do some languages haverich feature inventories but others have few if any grammaticalfeatures? Why do many languages have features that appearto encode semantic information (e.g. animacy) that is alreadyknown to the listener? We present a computational frameworkthat addresses questions like these by formalizing one way inwhich grammatical features aid communication. We use themodel to illustrate how morpho-syntactic feature inventorieshelp to solve the problem of communicating semantic struc-tures under cognitive pressures.

A Resource-Rational Process Model of Fairness in the Ultimatum Game

Widely regarded as the cornerstone of justice (Rawls, 1971),fairness constitutes one of the pillars of human morality. TheUltimatum Game (UG), extensively studied in behavioral eco-nomics, is the canonical task for studying fairness. In sharpcontrast to the predictions of normative standards in game the-ory, people typically reject low offers in UG. In this work,we present the first resource-rational process model of UG.Concretely, by taking into account people’s expectations, weshow that Nobandegani et al.’s (2018) resource-rational pro-cess model, sample-based expected-utility, provides a unifiedaccount of several experimental findings in UG, namely, theeffects of expectation, competition, and time pressure. Assum-ing that expectation serves as a reference point for subjectivevaluation of an offer, we show that the rejection of low offers inUG can arise from purely self-interested expected-utility max-imization. We conclude by discussing the implication of ourwork for moral decision-making and, more broadly, human ra-tionality.

Meta-Analysis of the Neural Correlates of Finger Gnosis using ActivationLikelihood Estimation

Finger gnosis is the ability to mentally represent one’s fingersas distinct from one another in the absence of visual feedback.In the current paper, we conducted a quantitative meta-analysis of imaging data, using activation likelihoodestimation, to determine the neural correlates of finger gnosis.Fourteen studies contributed 294 activated foci from 225participants for analysis. The meta-analysis yielded sevenpeaks of activation located within the frontal-parietal network(i.e., medial frontal gyrus, pre- and post-central gyrus, andinferior parietal lobule) and cerebellum (i.e. culmen). Aqualitative comparison of the findings of our meta-analysiswith single-experiment fMRI investigations of finger gnosis(Andres et al., 2012; Rusconi et al., 2014) suggests thatexperimentalists’ choices of primary and control tasks haveinfluenced our understanding of the neural substrateunderlying finger gnosis. Our results may aid in the designand interpretation of behavioural and imaging experiments aswell as inform the development of computational models.

The Representational Formats of Cognition and Visual Perception and theirInterface: Part 1

I examine the representational formats of perceptual states andcognitive states related to perception, such as perceptual beliefsstored in long term memory, and argue, first, that despite theirimportant differences they both have an iconic ingredient.Then, I explain how this common iconic component ofperceptual and cognitive contents allow cognitive states tomodulate perceptual processing focusing on a recent argumentmade by Burnston (2017) to the effect that owing to theirdiffering representational formats cognition cannot affectdirectly perception.

Do children preferentially mark unpredictable material? The case of optional plural marking

Speakers tend to assign more linguistic material to less predictable elements. This tendency is typically explained by a bias for an efficient trade-off between production effort and understandability and is claimed to shape linguistic structures across languages. Recent work suggests this trade-off enters the linguistic system through learning processes with learners deviating from their input by increasing marking for less predictable elements. However, no study to date has tested whether child learners also show such predictability-based marking, an important gap seeing that children are the primary learners in real-life language acquisition. A recent study showed that adults increase predictability-based marking of an optional-plural marker, in line with communicative efficiency. Here, we ask if children show a similar pattern. Results show that children, unlike adults, do not show an efficient trade-off in their productions. We discuss implications for the role of different language learners on language change.

Papers accepted as Posters, appearing in proceedings only

Information Theory Meets Expected Utility: The Entropic Roots of Probability Weighting Functions

This paper proposes that the shape and parameter fits of existing probability weighting functions can be explained with sensitivity to uncertainty (as measured by information entropy) and the utility carried by reductions in uncertainty. Building on applications of information theoretic principles to models of perceptual and inferential processes, we suggest that probabilities are evaluated relative to a plausible expectation (the uniform distribution) and that the perceived distance between a probability and uniformity is influenced by the shape (relative entropy) of the distribution that the probability is embedded in. These intuitions are formalized in a novel probability weighting function, VWD(p), which is simpler and has less parameters than existing probability weighting functions. The proposed probability weighting function captures characteristic features of existing probability weighting functions, introduces novel predictions, and provides a parsimonious account of findings in probability and frequency estimation related tasks.

Appraising Science Textbooks through Quantitative Text Analysis andPsychometric Results of Students’ Reading Skills

The “primary-secondary learning gap” has long been discussedin Japan. Many students suddenly have difficulties inunderstanding subjects when they enter junior high school (7thgrade in Japan). Despite the fact that textbooks are one of themost important learning instruments, the qualitative andquantitative change in the content of textbooks has not beenexamined in light of the primary-secondary learning gap. Inthis paper, we show that students are overloaded with the steepincrease in the definitions of scientific concepts in textbooks.While the number of definition expressions in textbooksincreases rapidly toward junior high school, students’ skills inunderstanding definitions develop only gradually. Wedemonstrated this through a quantitative linguistic analysis oftextbooks and psychometric results of students’ reading skills.

Decentering Cognition

The neocortex figures importantly in human cognition, but it isnot the only locus of cognitive activities or even at the top of ahierarchy of cognitive processing areas in the central nervoussystem. Moreover, the form of information processingemployed in the neocortex is not representative of informationprocessing elsewhere in the nervous system. In this paper, wearticulate and argue against cortico-centrism in cognitivescience, contending instead that the nervous system constitutesa heterarchical network of diverse types of informationprocessing systems. To press this perspective, we examineneural information processing in both non-vertebrates andvertebrates, including examples of cognitive processing in thevertebrate hypothalamus and basal ganglia.

Analyzing the Differences in Human Reasoning viaJoint Nonnegative Matrix Factorization

Joint Nonnegative Matrix Factorization (JNMF) is a methodfor factor analysis that is capable of simultaneously decom-posing two datasets into related latent state representations.Enabling factor analysis for contrasting applications, i.e., tofind common and distinct structural patterns in data, JNMF hasgreat potential for use in the field of cognitive science. Appliedto experimental data, JNMF allows for the extraction of com-mon and distinct patterns of behavior thereby extending theoutcomes of traditional correlation-based contrasting methods.In this article, we introduce JNMF to the field of cognitive sci-ence and demonstrate its potential on the exemplary domainof syllogistic reasoning by comparing reasoning patterns fordifferent personality factors. Results are interpreted with re-spect to the theoretical state of the art in syllogistic reasoningresearch.

Controlling the retrieval of general vs specific semantic knowledge in the instancetheory of semantic memory

Distributional models of semantic cognition commonly makesimplifying assumptions, such as representing word co-occurrence structure by prototype-like high-dimensional se-mantic vectors, and limit how retrieval processes may con-tribute to the construction and use of semantic knowl-edge. More recently, the instance theory of semantics (ITS,Jamieson, Avery, Johns, & Jones, 2018) reconceived a dis-tributional model in terms of instance-based memory, allow-ing context-specific construction of semantic knowledge at thetime of retrieval. By simulation, we show that additional en-coding and retrieval operations, consistent with learning andmemory theory, can play a crucial role in flexibly controllingthe construction of general versus specific semantic knowl-edge. We argue this consolidation of processing principlesholds insight for distributional theories of semantic cognition.

Crazy for you! Understanding Utility in Joint Actions

Predicting others’ actions and inferring preferences from their choices is indispensable for successfully navigating social environments. Yet, the cognitive tools agents employ for prediction and decision may differ when involved in social interactions. When pursuing a goal individually, humans maximize utility by minimizing costs, while when engaged in joint actions utility maximization might not be the only heuristic in place. We investigate if human adults represent costs and rewards of joint vs. individual actions, and how do they decide whether to engage in a joint action. We test participants’ decisions when solving a task alone or together with a partner as a function of the cost of coordination. Our results show that human adults decide based on a preference for joint actions, despite engaging in coordination reduces their individual utility. We discuss a framework for decision-making which accounts for cognitive heuristics and preferences for joint actions characterizing agents’ cooperative behavior.

The Role of Feedback and Post-Error Adaptations in Reasoning

Monitoring our errors enables humans to adapt behavior whenactions fail to result in desired outcomes. Post-error adaptationshave been studied extensively using simple laboratory taskswhere people typically slow down after errors. Few studies,however, examined such behavioral adaptations in morecomplex tasks such as reasoning. In two experiments weinvestigated how participants adapt their behavior based onevaluative feedback in syllogistic reasoning tasks.Experiment 1 demonstrates that participants’ likelihood to givea logically correct response increased throughout theexperiment when given feedback. This feedback effect waslimited to syllogisms that have no logical conclusion and thusmostly driven by an increase in participants’ “No validconclusion” responses. Experiment 2 investigates post-erroradaptations on a trial-level and shows that participants with ahigh accuracy slowed down after errors while participants witha low accuracy slowed down after correct responses.Implications on error-monitoring and reasoning research arediscussed.

Schoolchildren’s Spatial Reasoning

We examine schoolchildren’s reasoning with spatial relations, such as ‘is to the left of’. Our aims are to obtain a more precise account of the effect of working memory on reasoning, a more detailed understanding of the internal representation of mental models and a developmental perspective. We discuss two experiments in which 348 children, between eight and twelve years old, needed to verify conclusions for 24 reasoning problems describing the spatial relations between pieces of clothing. In both experiments, children in the experimental condition were allowed to take notes by means of paper and pencil. In both experiments we find that the participants spontaneously draw iconic representations of the items’ spatial ordering, have a strong preference for only considering one possible state of affairs even when more are relevant, and that an explanation in terms of working memory capacity alone cannot fully explain the data.

Assessing the relationship between trait and state levels of mind wanderingduring a tracing task

The aim of this study is to investigate whether trait differencesin mind wandering can also predict state differences in mindwandering. More specifically, we ask whether dimensions ofdisengagement, improvisation, and navigation of mind wan-dering thoughts in daily life also influence these dimensions ofmind wandering states during performance of a tracing task.Previous findings concerning the relationship between trait andstate mind wandering are inconsistent. Although studies indi-cate a significant relationship between the two, the correlatesof trait mind wandering and state mind wandering are not al-ways the same. Because of this, we expect to shed some lighton these inconsistencies by using a novel measure of mindwandering, which captures essential individual differences inthe nature of the phenomenon. Our results indicate that indi-vidual differences in trait mind wandering significantly predictstate differences in content variation of mind wandering andtask performance, but not in perceptual decoupling or in men-tal navigation. Implications of these findings are discussed.

How children interface number words with perceptual magnitudes

How do children map symbolic number words to continuousand noisy perceptual magnitudes? We explore how 5- to 12-year-olds attach novel units to number, length, and area byexamining whether verbal estimation performance is primarilypredicted by access to number words, the precision ofchildren’s underlying perceptual systems, or a more generalprocess in structurally aligning number words with perceptualmagnitudes. We find that from age five onward, children canreadily form novel mappings between number words andperceptual magnitudes, including dimensions they have noexperience estimating in (e.g., length, area), and even whenfaced with completely novel units (e.g., mapping a collectionof three dots to “one” unit for number). Additionally,estimation performance was poorly predicted by the noise intheir underlying perceptual magnitudes and number wordaccess. Instead, we show that individual differences inchildren’s abilities to translate continuous perceptual signalsinto discrete categories underlie verbal estimationperformance.

Determinantal Point Processes for Memory and Structured Inference

Determinantal Point Processes (DPPs) are probabilisticmodels of repulsion, capturing negative dependenciesbetween states. Here, we show that a DPP inrepresentation-space predicts inferential biases towardmutual exclusivity commonly observed in word learning(mutual exclusivity bias) and reasoning (disjunctivesyllogism) tasks. It does so without requiring explicitrule representations, without supervision, and withoutexplicit knowledge transfer. The DPP attempts tomaximize the total ”volume” spanned by the set ofinferred code-vectors. In a representational system inwhich combinatorial codes are constructed by re-usingcomponents, a DPP will naturally favor the combinationof previously un-used components. We suggest thatthis bias toward the selection of volume-maximizingcombinations may exist to promote the efficient retrievalof individuals from memory. In support of this, we showthe same algorithm implements efficient ”hashing”,minimizing collisions between key/value pairs withoutexpanding the required storage space. We suggestthat the mechanisms that promote efficient memorysearch may also underlie cognitive biases in structuredinference.

The effect of context on decisions:Decision by sampling based on probabilistic beliefs

Previous studies have shown that people’s decisions are af-fected by context in various ways, even when they are providedwith the same or analogous information. In the present study,we analyzed decisions based on verbally expressed probabilis-tic phrases (verbal probabilities) and examined how contextualfactors affected such decisions. In particular, we focused on thedifference in contexts that produced different probabilistic be-liefs with regards to uncertain events. We hypothesized thatsuch contextual effects could be explained in terms of a Deci-sion by Sampling (DbS) account (Stewart et al., 2006). In orderto examine our hypothesis, we proposed a modified version ofDbS, Decision by Belief Sampling (DbBS), and conducted be-havioral experiment about decision making. In this experiment,we set different contexts that we expected to produce differentprobabilistic beliefs regarding uncertain events for decision-makers and examined how such differences affected decisionmaking. Results showed that decisions were significantly af-fected by the variation in contexts, and DbBS well explainedsuch effects.

General mechanisms of color lexicon acquisition: Insights from comparison of German and Japanese speaking children

This research investigated how German-speaking children learn color words, both in terms of centroid mappings and boundary delineation, and how they construct the color lexicon as a connected system. The results were compared to those of Japanese children to draw insights on general mechanisms that underlie the acquisition of words in the color lexicon. For both languages, input frequency and category size contributed to the ease of learning. In contrast, in both language groups, naming (in)consistency in adults predicted the adult-like boundary delineation.

Memory integration into visual perception in infancy, childhood, and adulthood

We compared the influence of prior knowledge on visualperception in infants, children, and adults in order to explorethe developmental trajectory by which prior knowledge isintegrated with new sensory input. Using an identical taskacross age groups, we tested how participants’ accumulatedexperience affected their ability to judge the relative saturationlevels within a pair of sequentially-presented stimuli. We foundthat infants and children, relative to adults, showed greaterinfluence of the current observation and reduced influence ofmemory in their perception. In fact, infants and childrenoutperformed adults in discriminating between different levelsof saturation, and their performance was less biased bypreviously-experienced exemplars. Thus, the development ofperceptual integration of memory leads to less precisediscrimination in the moment, but allows observers to make useof their prior experience in interpreting a complex sensoryenvironment.

Teachers Know Best: The Impact of Taxonomic Distance and TeacherCompetence on Evaluation of Negative Evidence

Inductive generalization involves extending knowledge fromsparse samples of evidence to arrive at broad conclusions.Most of the research in this area has focused on generalizationfrom sparse samples of positive evidence (cases known to shareproperties with known cases; e.g., birds have hollow bones).Much less is known about generalization from samples ofnegative evidence (cases known to lack the propertiesattributed to known cases; e.g., bats do not have hollow bones).This paper reports the results from three experiments thatexamined factors that were believed to influence adults’evaluation of negative evidence. Experiment 1 showed thatwhen selecting among samples most useful for teaching abouta particular category, participants (N=36) preferred sampleswith negative evidence rather than those with single, oradditional, positive evidence. Experiment 2 revealed thatparticipants (N=25) preferred samples with negative evidencethat included a closer (rather than more distant) taxonomicmatch with the category in question. Finally, Experiment 3revealed that adults (N=52) only preferred samples thatprovided a close match when evidence was provided by acompetent informant. Overall these results emphasize theimportant role of pragmatic expectations when reasoning aboutsamples that include negative evidence.

Effects of Prior Mention and Task Goals on Language Processing

This paper investigates the processing of linguistic elements whose interpretation depends on retrieving information that was available earlier in the situation. Using the visual-world paradigm, we examine the processing of the verb return, which requires that an object has previously moved. We manipulated whether the moved object (and the movement itself) was described using language, by its typical label or by its location, or whether it was seen moving without that movement being labeled. We also manipulated whether the instructions were positive (e.g., Return the X), therefore requiring the listener to perform an action, or negative (e.g., Don’t return the X), which required no action. Results reveal a sensitivity to how information was introduced. Most importantly, with positive instructions, the naming of the object did not have an effect, whereas with negative instructions, naming was important to interpretation. These results indicate that the way information is introduced affects the status of this information when it is retrieved; these findings also lead us to explicitly consider the hypotheses that link language processing and visual attention.

Graded Representations of Norm Strength

Previous work across multiple disciplines has shown thatnorms have a powerful impact on behavior. Little is known,however about how norms are represented in the mind. Herewe examine whether people’s norm representations come inreliably identifiable grades of strength. Classical models ofnorms distinguished between the broad deontic categories ofprescriptions, permissions, and prohibitions. Four studiesdemonstrate that people consistently and consensuallydistinguish between deontic expressions that denote grades ofprohibition (e.g., frowned upon < unacceptable < forbidden)and grades of prescription (e.g., called for < expected

A Novel Target Detection Task Using Artificial Stimuli: The Effect of Familiarity.

In this paper we demonstrate that a target detection taskis facilitated when the background on which the targetis presented is a familiar one, even though the targetappears at a random location. We compare performancein that condition with one where the background israndomly generated and establish a significantdifference between these two versions of the task interms of both d’ and criterion, C. We also go on to lookat the effect of a tDCS procedure that we know to affectdiscrimination performance on this difference,discovering that it seems to reduce or reverse thedifference in criterion for these two conditions. Weascribe this effect to the neurostimulation manipulationshifting the distribution of information used to reach adecision

A Resource-Rational Mechanistic Account of Human Coordination Strategies

Humans often coordinate their actions in order to reach a mu-tually advantageous state. These circumstances are chieflymodeled by coordination games, a well-known class of gamesextensively studied in behavioral economics. In this work,we present the first resource-rational mechanistic approachto coordination games, showing that a variant of norma-tive expected-utility maximization acknowledging cognitivelimitations can account for several major experimental find-ings on human coordination behavior in strategic settings.Concretely, we show that Nobandegani et al.’s (2018) ratio-nal process model, sample-based expected utility, providesa unified account of (1) the effect of time pressure on hu-man coordination, and (2) how systematic variations of risk-vs. payoff-dominance affect coordination behavior. Impor-tantly, Harsanyi and Selten’s (1988) theory of equilibrium se-lection fails to account for (1-2). As such, our work suggeststhat the optimal use of limited cognitive resources may lie atthe core of human coordination behavior. We conclude by dis-cussing the implication of our work for understanding humanstrategic behavior, moral decision-making, and human ratio-nality.

Effects of linguistic context and world knowledge on the processing of tense andaspect: evidence from eye-tracking

The present eye-tracking reading study investigated the real-time processing of the so-called Lifetime Effect, which involvesthe integration of temporal verb morphology and knowledge ofa referent’s lifetime (alive vs. dead). Critical stimuli containedfamous referents, meaning that their lifetime status is widelyknown. In addition, context sentences mentioned their lifetimestatus and occupation. Tense/aspect was manipulated in a fol-lowing target sentence to contain either the present perfect orthe simple future (e.g., She has performed / will perform...).For dead referents, the target sentence was infelicitous giventhe tense/aspectual marking; for living referents, the mark-ing was felicitous. This design permitted us to examine ef-fects of lifetime status conveyed via world knowledge and lin-guistic context on the processing of tense/aspect morphology.Eye-tracking reading times revealed longer total reading timesat the critical (verb) and post-critical regions for the presentperfect when following a deceased context, while the dead-simple future condition had shorter overall reading times thanany other condition. Naturalness ratings revealed the dead-simple future to be quickly and reliably rejected, while thedead-present perfect was deemed acceptable. However, thelatter was rated significantly lower than the living/present per-fect condition. Taken together, the results imply that worldknowledge and an immediate context defining a real-world ref-erent as being dead or alive can jointly modulate the processingof subsequent verb tense/aspect, but with striking differencesbetween the present perfect and simple future.

The Typology of Polysemy: A Multilingual Distributional Framework

Lexical semantic typology has identified important cross-linguistic generalizations about the variation and commonal-ities in polysemy patterns—how languages package up mean-ings into words. Recent computational research has enabledinvestigation of lexical semantics at a much larger scale, butlittle work has explored lexical typology across semantic do-mains, nor the factors that influence cross-linguistic similari-ties. We present a novel computational framework that quan-tifies semantic affinity, the cross-linguistic similarity of lexicalsemantics for a concept. Our approach defines a common mul-tilingual semantic space that enables a direct comparison of thelexical expression of concepts across languages. We validateour framework against empirical findings on lexical semantictypology at both the concept and domain levels. Our resultsreveal an intricate interaction between semantic domains andextra-linguistic factors, beyond language phylogeny, that co-shape the typology of polysemy across languages.

Do Models Capture Individuals?Evaluating Parameterized Models for Syllogistic Reasoning

The prevailing focus on aggregated data and the lacking group-to-individual generalizability it entails have recently been iden-tified as a major cause for the low performance of cognitivemodels in the field of syllogistic reasoning research. This arti-cle attempts to add to the discussion about the performance ofcurrent syllogistic reasoning models by considering the param-eterization capabilities some cognitive models offer. To thisend, we propose a model evaluation setting targeted specifi-cally toward analyzing the capabilities of a model to fine-tuneits inferential mechanisms to individual human reasoning data.This allows us to (1) quantify the degree to which models areable to capture individual human reasoning behavior, (2) ana-lyze the efficiency of the parameters used by models, and (3)examine the functional differences between the prediction ca-pabilities of competing models on a more detailed level. Weapply this method to two state-of-the-art models for syllogisticreasoning, mReasoner and the Probability Heuristics Model,analyze the obtained results and discuss their implication withrespect to the general field of cognitive modeling.

The Processing of German Verb-Object Metaphors

Competing accounts of metaphor processing makedifferentiating predictions regarding the role of a metaphor’selements: While some claim that the elements are role-neutral,others believe them to play different roles from the get-go. Wetested these predictions with an investigation of German verb-object metaphors such as Sebastian füttert eine Prinzessin(‘Sebastian feeds a princess’). Results are in line with accountsthat posit different roles for a metaphor’s elements.Additionally, we investigated the distinctive influence ofcontext and a verb’s selectional preferences when anticipatinga post-verbal object. The findings show that participantsanticipate an upcoming object less when these two factors clash(i.e., when context and a verb’s selectional preferences pointtowards different objects), compared to when they are aligned(i.e. when they point towards anticipating the same object).

Intentionality, speaker’s attitude and the processing of verbal irony

Does it take more or less time to read ironic sentences than toread literal equivalents? Though this question has beenextensively discussed in the literature, the results are mixed(seeeg. Filik & Moxey, 2010). The present work attempt to accountfor the differences in the literature by considering the variableeffect of anticipating the intentions of a speaker duringcomprehension of ironic utterances used to answer yes/noquestions, as well as the role of explicit cues regarding theattitude of a speaker. The results show that both of these factorsinteract and together modulate the interpretation of a sentenceas ironic or literal as well as the utterance’s reading times. Weinterpret the results are broadly in line with the predictionsmade by the echoic mention account.

Representational complexity and pragmatics cause the monotonicity effect

Psycholinguistic studies have repeatedly demonstrated thatdownward entailing (DE) quantifiers are more difficult to pro-cess than upward entailing (UE) ones. We contribute to thecurrent debate on cognitive processes causing the monotonic-ity effect by testing predictions about the underlying processesderived from two competing theoretical proposals: two-stepand pragmatic processing models. We model reaction timesand accuracy from two verification experiments (a sentence-picture and a purely linguistic verification task), using the dif-fusion decision model (DDM). In both experiments, verifica-tion of UE quantifier more than half was compared to verifica-tion of DE quantifier fewer than half. Our analyses revealedthe same pattern of results across tasks: Both non-decisiontimes and drift rates, two of the free model parameters of theDDM, were affected by the monotonicity manipulation. Thus,our modeling results support both two-step (prediction: non-decision time is affected) and pragmatic processing models(prediction: drift rate is affected).

Probability Without Counting and Dividing:A Fresh Computational Perspective

Recent experiments show that preverbal infants can reasonprobabilistically. This raises a deep puzzle because infants lackthe counting and dividing abilities presumably required tocompute probabilities. In the standard way of computingprobabilities, they would have to count or accurately estimatelarge frequencies and divide those values by their total. Here, wepresent a novel neural-network model that learns and usesprobability distributions without explicit counting or dividing.Probability distributions emerge naturally from neural-networklearning of event sequences, providing a computationallysufficient explanation of how infants could succeed atprobabilistic reasoning. Several alternative explanations arediscussed and ruled out. Our work bears on several other activeliteratures, and it suggests an effective way to integrate Bayesianand neural-network approaches to cognition.

Simulating Feature- and Relation-Based Categorisation with a Symbolic-Connectionist Model

Participants in Goldwater et al. (2018) reported using either feature- or relation-based strategy during a series of category learning tasks. A computational modeling study was conducted to investigate whether performance on Experiments 1 and 2 of Goldwater et al. (2018) might be explained by the assumption that participants used either feature- or relation-based representational encoding during learning. Human participants’ and model performance are compared and implications are discussed

On causal claims, contingencies, and inference:How causal terminology affects what we think about the strength of causal links

The communicative goal behind a causal claim like “Smok-ing causes heart attacks” is to inform recipients about the ex-istence of a causal link between the factors mentioned in theproposition. Different terminologies can be used to accomplishthis goal. Sometimes people use formulations of the form “Ccauses E”, like in the tobacco warning above, and sometimesthey use other formulations, such as modal propositions like“C can cause / lead to E.”, or statements like “C increases therisk of E.”. We investigate the hypothesis that different causalstructure claims, by means of different terminologies, not onlycommunicate the existence of a causal link but also implic-itly elicit intuitions about that link’s strength. Experiment 1revealed that claims like “C causes E” imply a stronger linkthan, for example, modal formulations like “C can cause E”.Experiment 2 tested implications of this finding for researchon causal structure learning.

Causal scope and causal strength:The number of potential effects of a cause influences causal strength estimates

Causal scope, the number of different effects a cause can pro-duce, is a salient feature of causes. In the present research, weaddress the question whether reasoners use causal scope as adiagnostic cue to infer the strengths of individual causal links.In three experiments, we manipulated the number of effects ofa cause, and asked subjects to assess the causal strengths ofsingle causal links. The results document a clear influence ofcausal scope on perceived link strength. In particular, subjectstended to display a “dilution” effect. They perceived a causallink to be weaker if that link belonged to a cause that is capa-ble of producing additional effects. This dilution effect can beexplained by a dispositional notion of causality according towhich a cause possesses a certain amount of causal power orcapacity that it distributes across its different causal pathways.

A methodology for distinguishing copying and reconstruction in cultural transmission episodes

Information transmission between individuals through social learning is a foundational component of cultural evolution. However, how this transmission occurs is still debated. The copying account draws parallels with biological mechanisms for genetic inheritance, arguing that learners copy what they observe as they see it. On the other hand, the reconstruction account argues that learners recreate only what is relevant and reconstruct it using pragmatic inference, environmental and contextual cues. Distinguishing these two accounts empirically using typical transmission chain studies is difficult because they generate overlapping predictions. In this study we present an innovative methodological approach that generates different predictions of these accounts by manipulating the task context between model and learner in a transmission episode. We provide an empirical proof-of-concept showing that, when a model introduces embedded signals to their actions that are not intended to be transmitted, learners’ reproductions are more consistent with a process of reconstruction than copying.

Word Aversion and Consumer Behavior

Word aversion is characterized by visceral disgust in responseto seeing or hearing a word. Unlike taboo words or profanity,aversive words do not seem to have an obvious historicalcontext, referent, or pejorative function that causes people toreact negatively to them. “Moist” is a prototypical example ofan aversive word: roughly 20% of American English speakersequate hearing the word with the sound of fingernailsscratching a chalkboard. Despite widespread aversion to“moist,” the word frequently appears on the packaging ofconsumer products like cake, shampoo, and towelettes. Thepresent study tests whether word aversion affects consumerbehavior. We find that moist-averse participants are less liketo choose hygiene-related, but not food-related, products thathave “moist” on the package. We discuss the implications ofthis finding for theories of language processing and disgust inthe context of consumer behavior.

Better learning of partially diagnostic features leads to less unidimensionalcategorization in supervised category learning

Previous studies of supervised category learning show that par-ticipants often prefer a unidimensional categorization strat-egy. Studies also report that the perfectly diagnostic featureis learned better compared to the partially diagnostic features.We replicate these results, and we show that better learning ofpartially diagnostic features leads to less preference for uni-dimensional categorization. When participants have perfectknowledge about all the diagnostic features, then it becomesequivalent to memorizing the prototypes of the categories. Wecompare our results with the match-to-standards procedure,where category prototypes are shown during categorizationand unidimensional strategy is seldom preferred. We interpretour results to suggest that the preference for unidimensionalcategorization in supervised category learning, shown in ear-lier studies, could be due to poor learning of the partially diag-nostic features.

Children’s spontaneous inferences about time and causality in narrative

How do children understand the temporal and causalrelations among events in a narrative? We explored theroles of (a) connectives like before and because, (b)chronology, and (c) world knowledge in supportingchildren's inferences about causal and temporal relations innarrative. We told 3- to 7-year-old children storiescontaining two events. We then unexpectedly asked them toretell the stories from memory, to test what they hadencoded. Children attended to and recalled the causal andtemporal relations from the stories. They were more likelyto modify their retellings when the events in the story werenot described chronologically, or when the causal relationswere inconsistent with children’s knowledge of the realworld. These tendencies interacted with the specificconnectives in the story and their positioning. Thesefindings indicate that children as young as 3 spontaneouslyintegrate their knowledge of connectives, sentencestructure, and the world when processing narratives.

Can we match the variance across different visual features?

“Sensibility to variation” is considered to be a significant cognitive mechanism for adaptive decision making and action. It has been demonstrated that humans as well as animals have the ability in many perceptual properties. Here we tested whether people can compare and match the variance across perceptual domain. We examined subjective equal levels of variance across different perceptual properties, size and orientation, using the method of adjustment. The size- and the orientation-adjustment tasks were conducted in a between- subjects design. The point of subjective equalities (PSE) of the three target set variance levels were obtained. The results indicate that observers could adjust the size variance according to the direction variance in the size-adjustment task and do the reverse in the direction-adjustment task, and that the relation between the variance magnitudes of the two domains is linearly related. The result implies that people can sense the magnitude of variability of set of items and match the magnitude across perceptual domains.

Keep Calm and Learn the Language: Do Multilinguals Have Lower Intolerance ofUncertainty than Monolinguals?

This paper presents the results of an observational study on therelationship between multilingualism and lower Intolerance ofUncertainty (IoU). A group of over two hundred multilingualand monolingual individuals filled in an online survey that con-tained items about one’s language profile, cross-cultural expe-rience, and the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12 (IUS-12)– a psychometrically-sound instrument to assess one’s vulner-ability towards uncertain situations on an emotional, behav-ioral and cognitive level. We ask whether highly multilingualpeople are less likely to fear unknowns as a result of their ex-posure to linguistic and/or cultural uncertainty while learningforeign languages and/or staying abroad. The results show thatan advanced knowledge of multiple languages and longer staysabroad correlate with lower aversion towards uncertain situa-tions, thus, lower scores on the IUS-12. The study opens upnew avenues for further investigation into how multilingual-ism and multiculturalism shape one’s cognition and might havepositive effects on mental well-being.

Influence of Topic Knowledge on Curiosity

Given the vast nature of information available in the world,humans must select a small subset from which to learn in alifetime. Yet we know little about the factors that motivatelearners’ decisions to attend to select certain informationsources over others. We investigate the role of topicknowledge on curiosity in a new domain: novel news stories.We influenced listeners’ perception of their topic knowledgein these novel domains by independently varying the numberof sentences they heard and the number of sentences thatremained after a decision point. Listeners were most curiouswhen they reported intermediate levels of topic knowledge.As expected, learners were less likely to switch away fromcontent that they were curious about. This resultdemonstrates that topic knowledge directly impacts learners’curiosity and thus has downstream influences on their futureinterests and information-seeking behaviors.

Risk Taking and Impulsivity in Boredom: an EEG investigation

Previous research on boredom suggest it function as an important self-regulatory signal, indicating that the current state of the environment carries opportunity-costs and therefore driving the need to explore alternative activities. Trait boredom proneness is associated with negative consequences including increased risk-taking and impulsivity. These findings often rely on self-reports and not much is known about the role of state and trait boredom in controlled laboratory tasks, or their neural correlates. Sixty-two participants completed the Balloon Analogue Risk Task and a go/no-go task while electrical brain activity was recorded using EEG. Results showed that state boredom leads to impulsivity and poor performance monitoring, as evident by behavioral, subjective and ERP metrics. Trait boredom was associated with increased risk-taking, and modulated the correlation between errors and state boredom: high boredom proneness increased the sensitivity of trait boredom to errors. Overall, these findings emphasize the involvement of executive functions in the interaction between state and trait boredom.

Does top-down information about speaker age guise influence perceptualcompensation for coarticulatory /u/-fronting?

The current study explores whether the top-down influence ofspeaker age guise influences patterns of compensation forcoarticulation. /u/-fronting variation in California is linked toboth phonetic and social factors: /u/ in alveolar contexts isfronter than in bilabial contexts and /u/-fronting is moreadvanced in younger speakers. We investigate whether theapparent age of the speaker, via a guise depicting a 21-year-old woman or a 55-year-old woman, influences whetherlisteners compensate for coarticulation on /u/. Listenersperformed a paired discrimination task of /u/ with a raised F2(fronted) in an alveolar consonant context (/sut/), compared tonon-fronted /u/ in a non-coronal context. Overall,discrimination was more veridical for the younger guise, thanfor the older guise, leading to the perception of more inherentlyfronted variants for the younger talker. Results indicate thatapparent talker age may influence perception of /u/-fronting,but not only in coarticulatory contexts.

Top-down effect of apparent humanness on vocal alignment toward human anddevice interlocutors

Humans are now regularly speaking to voice-activatedartificially intelligent (voice-AI) assistants. Yet, ourunderstanding of the cognitive mechanisms at play duringspeech interactions with a voice-AI, relative to a real human,interlocutor is an understudied area of research. The presentstudy tests whether top-down guise of “apparent humanness”affects vocal alignment patterns to human and text-to-speech(TTS) voices. In a between-subjects design, participants heardeither 4 naturally-produced or 4 TTS voices. Apparenthumanness guise varied within-subject. Speaker guise wasmanipulated via a top-down label with images, either of twopictures of voice-AI systems (Amazon Echos) or two humantalkers. Vocal alignment in vowel duration revealed top-downeffects of apparent humanness guise: participants showedgreater alignment to TTS voices when presented with a deviceguise (“authentic guise”), but lower alignment in the twoinauthentic guises. Results suggest a dynamic interplay ofbottom-up and top-down factors in human and voice-AIinteraction.

Auricular Transcutaneous Vagus Nerve Stimulation (tVNS) Affects Mood and Anxiety during Second Language Learning

Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) has been used to address the symptoms of treatment-resistant depression (Rush et al., 2000) and is proposed to also alleviate anxiety effects (George et al., 2008). Transcutaneous VNS (tVNS) offers a less invasive treatment mechanism for clinical populations; however, little is known about tVNS effects on mood and anxiety in a non- clinical adult population. Using auricular tVNS, the present study showed that 10 minutes of tVNS immediately preceding second-language learning across three consecutive days reduced state negative affect, somatic anxiety, and cognitive anxiety, dependent on task performance and/or trait mood/anxiety.

Papers accepted as Posters, appearing in proceedings only (abstract-only publication)

How does over-specification affect referent identification?

Five eye-tracking experiments examined whether and under what circumstances over-specified adjectives hinder or facili-tate referent identification. We show that when the referring expressions and visual displays are presented simultaneously,the adjectives are processed incrementally, such that after a fully discriminating first adjective, the inclusion of a secondadjective will not facilitate early identification, even if the second adjective denotes a highly salient attribute and thereforeimproves fixations to the target. By contrast, when all the attributes have been heard before the display presentation, theattributes could be used in parallel to identify the referent. In such situations, a later-mentioned adjective speeds up iden-tification, as well as enhances looks to the target if it denotes a salient discriminating attribute (e.g., color); however, theinclusion of a less salient attribute (e.g., pattern) delays identification and tends to hamper fixations to the target.

Cross-linguistic investigation of the representations underlying pronoun choice

When making a reference, speakers must choose between nouns and pronouns. At what level of representation do speakersmake such a choice? The non-linguistic competition account predicts that the choice of using a pronoun occurs at thenon-linguistic level, so speakers should use fewer pronouns when the potential referents compete more strongly at thenon-linguistic level. By contrast, the linguistic competition account predicts that the pronoun choice occurs at the lexicallevel; speakers should use fewer pronouns when the potential antecedents are semantically or phonologically more similar.We show that regardless of whether the selection of a pronoun requires access to the antecedent (French pronouns) ornot (English pronouns, Italian null pronouns), speakers use fewer pronouns and more repeated nouns when the referentialcandidates compete more strongly in the non-linguistic context, whilst the similarities of their linguistic antecedents playno role. The finding provide support for the non-linguistic competition account.

Member Abstracts, appearing in proceedings only

Cognitive consequences of structured education in a connectionist model ofanalogical reasoning

Education has a profound impact on human cognition. People who have participated in education are better at solvingabstract reasoning tasks, can flexibly transfer knowledge across domains and are better at explaining their solutions.However, the properties of education that are responsible for these cognitive changes are poorly understood. We explorethe hypothesis that a structured education consisting of a cumulative, compositional curricular learning regime usingculturally constructed concepts and tools can account for many of these observations. In particular, we demonstrate thata connectionist model that learns to solve difficult analogical reasoning problems using a structured education is betterat knowledge reuse, while simultaneously providing explanations for solutions. We predict that premature progressionthrough a curriculum, before proficiency in a foundational stage has been established can fundamentally limit the potentialfor subsequent abstract reasoning performance or knowledge transfer ability.

Grid-Navigation Tasks involve Skill Learning

Several canonical experimental paradigms (serial reaction task, mxn task, etc.) have been proposed to study the typicalbehavioural phenomenon in sequential key-press tasks. However, not much work has been done on studying motor se-quencing in grid-navigation tasks. In this work, using empirical examinations, we systematically show grid-navigationtask as an instance of skill learning paradigm. The participants performed Grid-Sailing Task (GST), which required nav-igating (by executing sequential key-presses) a 5x5 grid from start to goal position while using a particular key-mappingamong the 3 cursor movement directions and the 3 keyboard buttons. We employ two different experiments to argue forthe learning of cognitive strategies as well as motor sequences. By rejecting the motor adaptation argument and validatingthe law of practice, we characterize GST as a skill learning task. We further argue for advantages of GST as a general,canonical task over others for use in skill learning studies.

Value-of-Information based Arbitration between Model-based and Model-freeControl

There have been numerous attempts in explaining the general learning behaviours using model-based and model-freemethods. While the model-based control is flexible yet computationally expensive in planning, the model-free control isquick but inflexible. Multiple arbitration schemes have been suggested to achieve the data efficiency and computationalefficiency of model-based and model-free control schemes, respectively. In this context, we propose a quantitative ’value-of-information’ based arbitration between both the controllers in order to establish a general computational frameworkfor skill learning. The interacting model-based and model-free reinforcement learning processes are arbitrated using anuncertainty-based value-of-information estimation. We further show that our algorithm performs better than Q-learning aswell as Q-learning with experience replay.

A Self-Learned Arbitration Between Model-Based and Model-Free NavigationStrategies in Autonomous Driving

Neuroscience research shows that mammals use two systems in spatial navigation: a flexible model-based strategy and aspontaneous model-free strategy. Mammals shift from model-based to model-free strategy as skills become ”habitized”and mostly use model-based strategy when high-level planning is necessary. Inspired by this line of work, the present studyproposes a model with a novel arbitration structure that solves the navigation problem in the autonomous-driving domain.This model takes into account the information from a model-based mapping/planning system and a model-free reactivecontroller, and adopts a learning-based gating method to adaptively arbitrate between the two systems. Experiments showthat the agent generally uses the reactive system when following lanes and driving through familiar intersections, andtend to rely on the planning system at unfamiliar intersections to get information about turning directions. The results aresimilar to mammal behaviors and provide insight for autonomous driving in the real world.

What is the Influence of Scale Format? A Study on the Likert and VisualAnalogue Scale

Scales are widely used to evaluate subjective dimensions in questionnaires. Two main formats are used: Likert scalesand Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). Previous studies have shown mixed results regarding which format to favor. The aimof the current study is to compare formats and presentation types for each type of scale. 658 participants participated inthe study and completed a trust scale. Several characteristics of scales (e.g., valence of anchors) were explored, and 11formats of scales were compared. The results show that participants’ responses were different according to the type ofscale (i.e., Likert or VAS), the initial cursor’s position in the VAS, and the anchors’ valence in the VAS. Differences interms of reliability were found between VAS formats and the number of categories in Likert scales. These findings suggestthat the scale format is crucial and may influence data collection as well as suggesting related conclusions.

Enhancing generalization through an optimized sequential curriculum: Learning(to read) through machine teaching

Learning environments are rich with structure but learning that structure can take considerable effort. Given that thesequence with which knowledge is accumulated is important for development (Smith & Slone, 2017), we consider whetheroptimizing the sequence of training examples can accelerate learning, as evaluated by out of sample generalization. Toexamine this issue we used established connectionist networks that map an orthographic input to a phonological output(Cox, Cooper Borkenhagen, & Seidenberg, 2018; Plaut et al., 1996). Utilizing machine teaching (Sen et al., 2018; Zhu,2015) to optimize word selection for a 10,000 word sequence, we observe an 8% average gain (over 100 sequences) ongeneralization accuracy (from 51% to 59%) compared to matched random sequences. These findings have implicationsfor learning domains where generalization is critical, like reading development where the child needs to gain as muchknowledge as possible from limited experience.

Childrens generalization of food properties: the role of transformation, propertyvalence, and neophobia

Children build concepts for food categories which they use in property induction-generalization situations. Which factorschildren do favor in their inductive strategies and to what extent interindividual differences, such as food neophobia, affectthem remains unclear. We used an induction task with negative and positive properties, and manipulated the familiarity(i.e., familiar and unfamiliar) and the state (i.e., untransformed and transformed) of foods. This study is the first to addressthe role of interindividual differences in inductive reasoning strategies in the case of opposed valence properties. Resultsrevealed that positive and negative properties are not generalized the same way, depending on the food familiarity andstate. In addition, we observed that neophobic children were characterized by different inductive strategies for negativeproperties compared to their neophilic counterparts. We conclude that food neophobia is sensitive to risk uncertainty andtherefore, caution should be taken when introducing new foods to preschoolers.

Ontogenesis of social interaction: Review of studies relevant to the fetal socialbehavior.

The paper discusses the ontogenesis of social interaction by reviewing different studies of fetal voice recognition, mimicry,and twin fetuses co-movement. The review found that fetuses behave socially, but they are unable to do this on their owndue to a lack of understanding social reality, which requires linking certain social cues with corresponding social cases.The article hypothesizes the facilitation of social learning of fetuses through mental interaction with the mother. Thismodality of interaction was explored in 12 online experiments with 67 adults and children. Participants were requiredto translate unfamiliar foreign words themselves (independently), by choosing one correct translation from 10 variants intheir native language in a congruent design and, with the opposite task, in incongruent one. The confederates receivedhints about the correct answers. These online experiments in different languages found evidence of a 98% increase in agroup performance (the p-value 0.001).

To Move or not to Move: An ERP Study on the Processing of Literal and FictiveMotion Constructions

This study used ERP method to investigate the processing of fictive motion and literal motion during natural languagecomprehension. A hypothesis is that the motion component of a verb is preserved in both literal and fictive motion con-structions (The army/The bridge crossed the river). However, the incorporation of a motion-event frame into fictive motionconstructions requires reanalysis or reconstruction both syntactically and semantically. The ERP results reveal that a P300effect on the subject NPs, a P600 effect on the motion verbs and an N400 modulation on the sentence-final complementNPs were uncovered in the processing of fictive motion constructions in relative to literal motion constructions. Theseresults suggest that the processing of fictive motion requires increased cognitive effort than the processing of literal motioncondition.

The impact of context and content similarity on risky choices: Insights from amemory-component model for decisions from experience

How do different memory components impact risky choices? We developed a computational model that unifies compo-nents from memory research with decisions from experience. Our model chooses options based on expectations, observesoutcomes, stores them in memory, and forms new expectations based on observed outcomes. Their memory activationresults from recent encounters, binding outcomes to the context of options, and encoding according to similarity to exist-ing representations, and impacts how much each outcome drives new expectations. We tested the model on data from amulti-armed bandit task: Participants chose repeatedly between three options and received outcome feedback. Two coreoptions appeared in two choice sets with different third options. Core options were chosen less often when they wereaccompanied by similar (compared with dissimilar) third options. The model matched choice-proportion levels, direction,and size of this similarity effect. We present Bayesian estimates for memory components and discuss implications fortheory advancement.

Jointly learning motion verbs and frame semantics from natural language andgrounded scenes

We propose a computational model of verb learning implemented as a probabilistic compositional semantic parser, thatjointly learns individual verb meanings and overarching associations between syntactic verb frames and compositionalsemantic predicates from distant supervision on grounded natural language data. In tandem, we present a new corpus fortraining and evaluating grounded language learning models, containing natural language descriptions of scenes generatedin a rich environment that simulates realistic interactions between animate agents and physical objects. We demonstratehow the model can acquire interpretable correspondences between syntactic frames by incrementally parsing individ-ual sentences, evaluating candidate verb meanings on grounded scenes, and investigate how the models acquired framesemantics priors generalize to support efficient inferences about the meanings of novel verbs on a few shot learning task.

The rational side of decision bias based on verbal probabilities

Verbal probabilistic expressions (verbal probabilities) contain a communicative function called directionality and can becategorized as positive (e.g., likely or probable) or negative (e.g., unlikely or doubtful) on the ba-sis of their directionality.Previous studies have demonstrated that the directionality of phrases affects decisions. In particular, people tend to be morerisk seeking when presented with positive phrases and risk averse when presented with negative phrases. The rationality(i.e., maximizing utility) of such seemingly biased decisions is examined in this study. We hypothesize that because aspeaker tends to choose a positive or negative expression on the basis of context, the selected phrase works as an adaptivecue for understanding the situational change, and that decision biases based on differences in expressions will lead tomore rational decision making. Computer simulations were conducted regarding decisions with uncertainty based onverbal probabilities. We found that despite speaker biases in probability judgments, miscommunication generated by thevagueness of verbal expressions, and individual differences in subjective values, biased deci-sion makers who changedtheir risk attitude on the basis of the directionality of verbal probabilities could make more decisions that were rationalthan could those who did not show such decision biases.

Learning Communication Policies for Knowledge Transfer between Agents

We present an agent model in the predictive coding framework that selectively communicates with other agents to predictthe state of its environment efficiently. Selective communication is a challenge when the internal models of other agentsare unknown and unobservable. Communication helps agents to transfer the knowledge they have acquired in differentsituations. Recognition of daily activities of individuals living in different homes served as a testbed for evaluating themodel. Two publicly-available datasets, collected from unique homes, are used. Behavioral patterns of individuals in thosehomes are also unique. Each home is assumed to be monitored by an agent. We experimentally show that the agents cantransfer knowledge by communicating the most informative messages. The messages are interpretable. The agents learnpatterns of daily activities for any individual, and communicate using a vocabulary of words. Our model is more accuratethan traditional transfer learning models for the same task.

Conditional Reasoning and Relevance

The paper concerns conditional reasoning and, in particular, the case, where the antecedent of a conditional is true butits consequent is unknown. We pursue the idea to apply abduction in order to find an explanation for the consequent.If such an explanation can be abduced then new conditionals can be generated which are known to be true. This leadsto two problems, viz. that a consequent should not abduce itself and that the antecedent should be strongly relevant tothe consequent of a conditional. Both problems are solved within the Weak Completion Semantics, a new, computational,multi-valued, and non-monotonic logic paradigm which has already been successfully applied to different human reasoningproblems including the suppression and the selection task. The notion of strong relevance developed in the paper is withrespect to the models of a logic program representing the background knowledge of a human reasoning episode and, thus,deviates from the mostly proof theoretic definitions of relevancy in relevance theory.

Musical Pitch Affects Brightness Judgment of a Concurrent Visual Object

Given an apparent prevalence of audio-visual information in everyday lives, understanding how humans perceive thisinformation has gained considerable attention in cognitive science. Previous research has demonstrated that lower (vs.higher) auditory pitch and visual darkness (vs. brightness) are conceptually associated. However, little is known whetherpitch level can affect brightness judgment of a concurrent visual object. To examine this, we presented 27 participants witha random sequence composed of both higher- and lower-pitched versions of 40 musical excerpts, during each of which agrey square appeared on a white background screen. At the end of every excerpt, participants judged the brightness of eachsquare on a 7-point scale (I think this square is ; 1= dark, 7= bright). Although participants were told beforehand thatthe square brightness could be varied across questions, an identical square appeared constantly. A wilcoxon signed-ranktest showed that the same grey square was judged darker (vs. brighter) when it was presented with lower-pitched (vs.higher-pitched) music (Z=-2.931, p¡0.005).

Distributional Information in Speech to Children: Nouns Come First

One proposal for how children acquire lexical categories is on the basis of their distributional signatures. Given that thelanguage children are exposed to gradually changes as they get older, it is possible that such changes impact the qualityof distributional information, and therefore the efficiency with which lexical categories are acquired. To test this idea, wecompiled a corpus of American-English child-directed speech and ordered it by increasing age of the target child. Next,we investigated the quality of distributional cues about lexical category membership in the first and second half of theage-ordered corpus. As predicted, we found that the quality of distributional information co-varies with age of the targetchild. Specifically, we found that distributional evidence for the noun category was of higher quality in speech to youngercompared to older children. In light of these findings, we recommend that distributional accounts of lexical categoryacquisition take into consideration language change during the first six years of development.

Order matters: Developmentally plausible acquisition of lexical categories

One proposal for how children acquire syntactic and semantic lexical categories is by inducing them from their distribu-tional signatures in speech. Because the language children are exposed to gradually increases in complexity as they getolder, it is possible that inducing lexical categories from initially simplified speech supports acquisition. We set out to testthis hypothesis using a simple recurrent neural network trained to predict 5 million words of child-directed speech from theAmerican-English portion of the CHILDES database. Evaluation of learned representations showed that models trainedin order in which children actually experience language performed better on a semantic, but not syntactic, categorizationtask. To understand why, we examined how the models encoded words during the earliest stages of training. Our resultsare relevant to important questions in language acquisition, such as the role of early experiences in organizing children’slinguistic representations.

How does simulating aspects of primate infant visual development inform trainingof CNNs?

Primate visual development is characterized by low visual acuity and colour sensitivity besides high plasticity and synapticgrowth in the first year of infancy, prior to the development of specific visual-cognitive functions. In this work, weinvestigate the possible synergy between the gradual variation in visual input distribution and the concurrent growth of astatistical model of vision on the task of large-scale object classification. We adopt deep convolutional neural networks(CNNs) as a statistical model of vision and study its performance in 4 training setups each varying in either the modelbeing static or growing in parameters or the visual input being fully-formed or refining in saturation, contrast and spatialresolution. Our experiments indicate that a setup reflective of infant visual development, wherein a gradually growingmodel is trained on a refining visual input distribution, converges to a better generalization at a faster rate in comparisonto other setups.

Understanding Computational Thinking Assessment through Text Mining

With the ever-increasing need for teaching computational thinking (CT) to learners of the digital age, teacher educatorsneed to better guide teachers to embed CT activities across subjects and contexts while discovering the positive effectsof computer programming in K-12 education. However, computational thinking assessment (CTA) have yet to be fullyunderstood in the literature. To address this challenge, this paper used text mining with the aim of reviewing CTA in theliterature for both pre-service and in-service educators. By analyzing 267 papers, we identified 14 clusters of CTA topicsby exploring the application of computational techniques including rudimentary vector space models and unsupervisedmachine learning algorithms. We also performed a network analysis for further interpretation of our unsupervised machinelearning results. This visualization of the network allows us to select main themes and perform an exploratory factoranalysis. Implications for educational design and future research are discussed.

Co-speech gestures reflect non-linguistic thinking: evidence from mental abacus

Why do people gesture when they speak? On one proposal, people gesture because they speak: Gestures reflect speechproduction processes. Alternatively, people gesture because they think: Gestures reflect non-linguistic thinking processes.If gestures during speech grow out of thinking, not simply speaking, then co-speech gestures should look similar to thegestures that are produced during silent thinking without speech. Here, we looked at spontaneous gestures during mentalabacus, a non-linguistic technique for rapid arithmetic operations via imagining moving beads on an abacus. We comparedhow expert mental abacus users spontaneously gesture during silent thinking (no-speech) and during explaining how theysolved the arithmetic problems (speech). In both the speech and the no-speech condition, gestures reflected operations ona mental abacus in the same way (e.g. depicting the trajectory of beads). These results suggest that at least some co-speechgestures grow out of thinking processes that are independent of speaking.

Can misconceptions be forgotten? Evaluating the efficacy of a directed-forgettingparadigm in revising science misconceptions

Science misconceptions persist across development and have long-term consequences for achievement. Researchers haveattempted to replace science misconceptions with correct information. Intentional forgetting, often studied using a directedforgetting (DF) paradigm, is one approach used to eliminate incorrect material. The present study aimed to identify whichscience misconceptions persist among adults and determine whether DF can be implemented to forget misconceptions. 147undergraduates saw two lists of 11 science statements. For each statement, they provided a truthfulness and confidencerating before receiving the correct True/False rating. Half were told to remember both lists; half were told to forget thefirst list and remember the second. Results revealed that although accuracy and confidence increased overall, there weresignificant differences between science domains and no observable DF effect. This suggests that science misconceptionsare even more persistent than previously thought, particularly for certain domains, and additional supports are needed tocorrect them.

To repeat or not to repeat: Competitor repetition and variability in childrensmemory for words

To successfully learn words, children must map words to referents in the presence of competitor objects, and retain thesemappings across time. Past research suggests that competitor repetition supports word mapping. However, these studieshave not implemented delayed tests. Relying on a desirable difficulties framework, we predicted that competitor variabilitywould lead to better long-term retention of novel words. To test this prediction, children ages 2-6 completed a novel wordlearning task. Children were assigned to a competitor repetition or competitor variation condition. In Experiment 1,we tested retention of novel word-referent mappings at an immediate and 10-minute delayed test. In Experiment 2, weassessed whether retrieval dynamics during learning explained retention performance. Results revealed that competitorvariation engendered less retrieval success during learning. Competitor variation also reduced forgetting of novel wordsacross time. We highlight the importance of moving beyond immediate tests when characterizing competition in wordlearning.

Intentionally forgotten food pictures are perceived less delicious.

Instruction to forget a memory after learning can lead to forgetting of the memory. This phenomenon is known as directedforgetting. Instruction to forget cause not only forgetting but also devaluation. Previous evidence demonstrated thatpleasantness of to-be-forgotten words and faces decreased relative to to-be-remembered items. Here, we examined whetherdevaluation by directed forgetting is generalized to food. In our experiment, participants learned pictures of foods andthen received instructions to forget or to remember them. Then, participants rated perceived deliciousness for half ofto-be-remembered pictures and half of to-be-forgotten pictures. Finally, participants took an old/new recognition test forremained pictures. The results showed successful directed forgetting: memory performance of to-be-forgotten pictureswas lower than that of to-be-remembered pictures. Additionally, a similar pattern was observed for deliciousness. Thus,instruction to forget induces devaluation as well as forgetting, suggesting that memory plays an important role in evaluatingthe deliciousness of food.

Are analogies enough? Assessing long-term retention of and cognitive supports forscience concepts learned using structural alignment

One major challenge in science learning involves acquiring understanding of abstract concepts. Structural alignment (SA)has been shown to aid childrens learning of science concepts; however, research has yet to investigate how analogies affectchildrens ability to retain concepts over time. The current study addresses this gap by examining what information childrenremember and forget about science concepts using SA. Experiment 1 (N=120) instructed children 4-9 years on examplesof animal adaptation using SA, then tested their memory or generalization of these concepts immediately or after a delay.Experiment 2 (N=118) used the same design, but prompted children to recall only perceptual or relational information.Results revealed that children rapidly forget and fail to generalize relational information relative to perceptual information,and that this pattern persists even with linguistic supports to recall it. This suggests that additional cognitive supports areneeded to facilitate long-term relational learning of science concepts.

Effect of Active Pre-Learning Activities on Humans and Machines

There are numerous studies that show that the more students actively participate in class, the more they learn. Despiteample evidence, education still relies on lecturers or professors. Although active learning to increase learners’ engagementhas recently been introduced in a variety of methods, quantitative and empirical experiments are lacking. In this study,we conducted two experiments in order to empirically confirm the effect of active learning on learning performance. Wecompared humans and machines to investigate that active learning is more effective than passive learning. In Experiment1, we compared watching a lecture, the passive form of learning with having a discussion, the active form of learning.Comparing students’ learning performance of each condition, results of the present study showed higher performancein active learning. In the additional experiment that imitated the human learning frameworks in machines, the activelearning framework performed better than the passive learning framework. Through the results of humans experiment andvalidation of machines experiment, we found that active learning have crucial effect on learning performance.

A Generalization Test of Conjunction Errors in Physical Reasoning

Ludwin-Peery, Bramley, Davis, and Gureckis (2019) reported finding evidence of conjunction fallacy errors in an intuitivephysics reasoning task. However, this finding was limited to a single paradigm involving the behavior of only two objects,interacting in a consistent manner, in a highly regular setting. In this project, we provide an important generalizationtest of this result, and examine several new paradigms under which conjunction errors might be observed. We find somecases that produce the expected errors, representing an important generalization of the original finding, as well as someparadigms which do not appear to produce conjunction errors.

Is the structure of the belief in conspiracy theory equivalent across cultures?

In the literature, scholars often postulate a uni-dimensional structure of beliefs in conspiracy theory except for the GenericConspiracist Belief scale (GCB, Brotherton et al., 2013) which posits a five-factor structure of the belief. On the otherhand, a recent study extending the GCB to non-Western population proposed a two-factor structure which dissociatesextraterrestrial conspiracy from other beliefs and suggested that the belief structure might not be equivalent across Westernand non-Western population. In this study, 616 participants from two cloud-sourcing pools (309 Westerners / 307 Japanese) answered to questionnaires including GCB. A multi-group confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the validity of two-factor structure across two pools, however only partial metric invariance has been achieved. Results suggests that overallstructure of conspiracy belief is similar across cultures, however several aspects of beliefs might not be equivalent.

Spatial structure in the cultural ecosystem of number

Cognition and culture shape each other. Private thinking is externalized in public artifacts, which can shape habits ofthought. Within individual minds, for instance, numbers are associated with space. Do similar regularities exist withinthe cultural ecosystem of written numbers? We analyzed three contexts: English books, childrens picture books, andalgebraic expressions created during mathematical activity. Within individual numbers, digits were ordered spatially fromleft-to-right, with lower-value digits appearing more often to the left and greater-value digits to the right (e.g., 179). Ona larger scale, lesser-valued numbers were more likely to appear first in phrases and algebraic expressions (e.g., 19 dogsand 32 cats, 19x+32). The cultural ecosystem of number thus exhibits spatial regularities at multiple scales. We discussimplications for the development and dissemination of individual mental associations (mental number lines) and defend anecological perspective in which cognition reflects mutual constraints between artifacts, practices, and individual thought.

The Acceptability of AI at Work: Predicting the Intention to Use relying onUTAUT

Thanks to research breakthroughs, Artificial Intelligence (AI) has gained popularity in recent years. Nevertheless, itsacceptability by general public is a poorly researched subject. The unified theory of acceptance and use of technology(UTAUT), a popular model to evaluate acceptability, is generally used for technological products. The purpose of thisstudy is to ensure that UTAUT is an appropriated model to evaluate AI acceptability. In this paper, 705 participants wereinvited to evaluate the acceptability of tools that integrate AI at work in 2030. Relying on UTAUT, performance expectancy,effort expectancy, social influence, facilitating conditions, and intention to use were evaluated. Structural modeling sug-gests a significant influence of performance expectancy, social influence and facilitating conditions on intention to use AI(explained variance of 81%). This research paves the way for prospective research on the overall acceptability of AI.

Procedures and principles of number: Evidence from the Tsimane

By about age four, children in industrialized cultures can use verbal counting to correctly name the number of objectsin sets larger than four. On some accounts, this full counting ability signals abstract knowledge about the fundamentalprinciples governing number (e.g. the successor function and later-greater principle). However, many children who qualifyas full counters fail to grasp these principles. Why? This failure could reflect number-specific conceptual deficiencies ordomain-general cognitive immaturity. Here we tested these alternatives in the Tsimane, an indigenous group in whichage and numerical knowledge are largely unconfounded. Although many Tsimane performed at ceiling, a subset of full-counters demonstrated poor understanding of fundamental counting principles. Performance was not reliably predicted byage or schooling, but rather by highest verbal count. These findings suggest that at any age, the development of numericalabilities is characterized not by sudden induction of abstract principles, but by piecemeal procedural learning.

Effect of wordings on public perception toward Artificial Intelligence

Artificial Intelligence (AI) is an increasingly prevalent field that influences a number of other areas. It generates a multitudeof reactions among the general population, particularly anxiety, which impacts on the development, deployment, andregulation of AI. Nevertheless, experimental data on public perceptions toward AI are generally lacking. To fill thisgap, this paper presents a large-scale experiment conducted on the influence of the terms used to describe AI on peoplesperception. In a preliminary study (705 participants), words related to AI were extracted. In a second experiment (552participants), the impact of these terminologies on anxiety toward AI was explored. An unprecedented effect of wordingand a positive bias of perception toward computers was revealed by these experiments, compared to robots and newtechnologies. This research paves the way for future studies on the effect of words on perception in the field of AI andnew technologies.

To Dye or Not to Dye : The Effect of Hair Color on First Impressions

How does hair color affect people’s first impression of a face? Equipped with state-of-the-art Generative AdversarialNetwork (GAN) models, we are able to re-investigate the questions with strictly and precisely controlled image stimuli.By creating triplets of the same face image with different hair colors, we examine how black/brown/blond hair colorsaffect perception of attractiveness, trustworthiness and intelligence. Our study finds that if the original hair color is dark,the optimal choice in most cases is to stay in dark colors. If your original hair color is blond, changing into brown will,in general, make you look more intelligent, sometimes at the cost of attractiveness. The specific best color choice variesa lot more for people with blond hair. Furthermore, we train a neural network model that predicts people’s impressionson faces in different trait dimensions accurately. This study could provide guidance to people regarding their image andimpression control.

Effect of a colour-based descriptor and stimuli presentation mode in unsupervisedcategorization

In unsupervised categorization, studies have shown that fewer stimuli dimensions are used for categorization with serialpresentation compared to concurrent presentation of stimuli. In this study, we investigate how a colour-based multidimen-sional descriptor might affect the number of dimensions used in categorization. Our results show that a fewer numberof dimensions are used when stimuli are presented serially irrespective of the presence of a colour-based descriptor. Wefound main effects for both the stimuli presentation mode and the colour-based descriptor. The stimuli has the same logicalstructure across all the conditions. Our results show that the notion of a natural and intuitive grouping of items is affectedby meta-level feature descriptors, that are not part of a feature-based representation of stimuli. We discuss the implica-tions of our findings for computational models of categorization, which make predictions based solely on feature-basedrepresentation of stimuli.

Large-Scale Survey of Students Skills in Reading Math Definitions

Mathematical text reading seems to require a different type of literacy than others since it heavily introduces abstractconcepts and require strict logical and literal reading. In this paper, we focus on grades 611 (elementary through highschool) students skill in reading math definitions, but not problem solving. Our experiment showed that their skills inreading and understanding math definitions improves as long as math is obligatory (through the 10th grade) but reachesa plateau very quickly after that. However, teaching a math definition and using it to solve exercise problems in theclassroom do not seem to improve students ability of reading that specific definition.

Should we always log-transform looking time data in infancy research?

Researchers often measure infants looking time (LT) as a dependent variable to measure how infants pay attention to certainstimuli. Using a large repository of data from their lab and the literature, Csibra and colleagues (2016) reported that thedistribution of LT is positively skewed and thus proposed that researchers should log-transform LT before running anyparametric analysis. In this study, we investigated whether log-transformation of LT will make the distribution normallydistributed by using data from a large-scale replication infancy study (ManyBabies Consortium (MB1), in press). Further,we simulated positively skewed LT data to examine whether log-transformation of LT would improve power. We foundthat log-transformation of the MB1 LT data did not make the LT data normally distributed. Also, we found that log-transformation of LT only slightly increased power. Implications and benefits of log-transformation of LT data will bediscussed.

Disguising self-esteem caused changes in academic achievements differently forboys and girls in Japanese junior high school.

Japanese youth (13-29 years old) showed lower self-esteem than other countries in the recent survey. The proportions ofthose who agreed to the statements I have my own unique strengths were 62.3% of Japanese, while 91.4% of Germany,91.2% USA, and 90.6% France (Japanese Government Cabinet Office, 2019). We assumed that Japanese youth mighthave disguised their self-esteem. To examine the hypothesis, we assessed the self-esteem of 159 Japanese junior highschool students implicitly and explicitly with a paper-based IAT and a questionnaire. As expected, we found 26.4% ofthe students having disguised self-esteem: They performed positively on the IAT while they answered negatively on thesurvey. We further examined the relationships of the disguises of self-esteem and the longitudinal changes in academicachievement. The results were different for boys and girls; disguising boys raised their academic performances six monthslater while disguising girls lowered their performances one year then.

Assessing children’s perceptual sensitivity to social information

Recent theories of social-cognitive development have generally focused on the development of theory of mind betweeninfancy and preschool. However, social understanding involves more than developing an inferential understanding of mindand continues beyond the early childhood years. We present preliminary findings from a study that evaluated childrensperceptual sensitivity to subtle kinematic cues that distinguish between intentions in others behaviour, based on Pesquita etal. (2016). On each trial, children observed videos of an actor reaching to touch one of two buttons. On half the trials theactor chose which button to touch and on the other half they were directed. A paired-samples t-test showed that participantswere reliably faster at correctly predicting the actors movement in the chosen condition than the directed condition [t(39)= 6.23, p ¡ .01, Cohens d = 0.99)]. We argue that social understanding comes in various forms and at different levels ofawareness.

Online Ratings: A Case Study of Information Integration

Building upon previous literature that demonstrates the effect of average rating and number of reviews on consumerbehavior, the present study begged the question of how rating distributions influence perception of product quality at theindividual consumer level. To address this question, we presented a wide range of rating variances for each average ratingfrom 1.1 to 4.9 in a 5-star system and asked participants to indicate their perceived quality of each product on a scale of1 10. The behavioral study revealed an interaction between average rating and rating variance: Among all products of thesame average rating, when the average rating was low (below 2.5), people judged less-variable products to be of higherquality, whereas when the average rating was high (above 2.5), people judged more-variable products to be of higherquality. A utility-based cognitive model was developed to identify the underlying mechanisms of this reversed preference.