Linguistics Department
Parent: UC San Diego
eScholarship stats: Breakdown by Item for August through November, 2024
Item | Title | Total requests | Download | View-only | %Dnld |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
4xd7t5ww | Rhetorical questions as redundant interrogatives | 223 | 20 | 203 | 9.0% |
5wt1s829 | Dictionaries and phonologists: English accentuation and stress | 212 | 5 | 207 | 2.4% |
9pj2s50f | Aspectual Verbs and the Aspect Phrase Hypothesis | 154 | 15 | 139 | 9.7% |
8cc2z9nx | On the linguistic behavior of keysmashes | 146 | 41 | 105 | 28.1% |
3qx6t51c | Linguistic practices in Cyprus and the emergence of Cypriot Standard Greek | 111 | 7 | 104 | 6.3% |
9hz911cx | Japanese passives, external arguements, and structural case | 82 | 3 | 79 | 3.7% |
5j61c2w4 | On the acoustical features of vowel nasality in English and French | 77 | 33 | 44 | 42.9% |
2526g7ms | The second generation of “New Shanghainese”: their language and identity | 75 | 58 | 17 | 77.3% |
7zw206pt | Vowel harmony and stem identity | 74 | 18 | 56 | 24.3% |
7f26m713 | Challenges for a theory of islands: A broader perspective on Ambridge, Pine, and Lieven | 73 | 2 | 71 | 2.7% |
5zt9h41v | Vowel dispersion and Kazakh labial harmony | 72 | 16 | 56 | 22.2% |
2jz957jm | Triple Take: Tigre and the case of internal reduplication | 64 | 4 | 60 | 6.3% |
40w4w14q | Experimental syntax and the variation of island effects in English and Italian | 60 | 12 | 48 | 20.0% |
8gb8209s | Noun class agreement and the elements of the noun phrase in Gitonga-Inhambane | 60 | 44 | 16 | 73.3% |
0691p7tp | Learning Mathematics in a Visuospatial Format: A Randomized, Controlled Trial of Mental Abacus Instruction | 56 | 40 | 16 | 71.4% |
8mg0x5pb | The source of creak in Mandarin utterances | 56 | 39 | 17 | 69.6% |
4q90b3fm | Erasure as a means of maintaining diglossia in Cyprus | 52 | 8 | 44 | 15.4% |
7sd5987t | Circumstances and Perspective: The Logic of Argument Structure | 50 | 11 | 39 | 22.0% |
8kj817s8 | Folk definitions of Korean ideophones | 49 | 24 | 25 | 49.0% |
3bj9x13m | Intrinsic fundamental frequency of Amharic vowels | 48 | 27 | 21 | 56.3% |
6f20b9xx | On the proper treatment of weak determinism: Subsequentiality and simultaneous application in phonological maps | 48 | 18 | 30 | 37.5% |
8f979841 | Phonological Abstraction in The Mental Lexicon | 46 | 16 | 30 | 34.8% |
58j408sd | Perception of ATR contrasts by Akan speakers: a case of perceptual near-merger | 45 | 25 | 20 | 55.6% |
25n2f41s | Abstractness and Motivation in Phonological Theory | 44 | 1 | 43 | 2.3% |
6vg240kc | Counting with fingers symbolically: basic numerals across sign languages | 43 | 29 | 14 | 67.4% |
2bb4829q | Non-Intervocalic Geminates: Typology, Acoustics, Perceptibility | 41 | 10 | 31 | 24.4% |
4mq0b3pz | 'What' clauses can and 'which' cannot: A Romanian puzzle | 41 | 13 | 28 | 31.7% |
55x0q967 | Patterns in Kirundi reduplication | 38 | 14 | 24 | 36.8% |
7fp45641 | Prediction in a visual language: real-time sentence processing in American Sign Language across development | 36 | 23 | 13 | 63.9% |
7p03v0c3 | Who's on First? Investigating the referential hierarchy in simple native ASL narratives | 36 | 25 | 11 | 69.4% |
0h9362zf | Validating a psychoacoustic model of voice quality. | 35 | 15 | 20 | 42.9% |
7pj8j0pr | Revisiting non-idempotency in Tibetan vowel harmony | 35 | 23 | 12 | 65.7% |
6jb6r71k | A mixed height and ATR system of vowels in Rere | 34 | 21 | 13 | 61.8% |
8st0926b | Free choice free relative clauses in Italian and Romanian | 34 | 23 | 11 | 67.6% |
1m56m1ht | Elsewhere Effects in Optimality Theory | 33 | 4 | 29 | 12.1% |
3025n0rg | A lexical indexation account of exceptions to hiatus resolution in Mushunguli | 32 | 12 | 20 | 37.5% |
5mm8k8t3 | Do Adults Show an Effect of Delayed First Language Acquisition When Calculating Scalar Implicatures? | 32 | 19 | 13 | 59.4% |
6dz9d525 | A Compositional Semantic Analysis of Echo Questions in Korean | 31 | 6 | 25 | 19.4% |
5d47g99f | Unbounded circumambient patterns in segmental phonology | 30 | 19 | 11 | 63.3% |
8zj8m7jz | A revised typology of opaque generalisations* | 30 | 19 | 11 | 63.3% |
14k8p79b | Grammatical gender agreement with nominal compounds in Somali | 29 | 7 | 22 | 24.1% |
4cr2r2cq | Plosive voicing in Afrikaans: Differential cue weighting and tonogenesis | 29 | 13 | 16 | 44.8% |
5vb9412h | Agreement and Pronoun Incorporation in ASL Verbs from a Bantu Perspective: Reporting of Pilot Data | 29 | 3 | 26 | 10.3% |
6tv8g2xj | Apparent ‘sufficiently similar’ degemination in Catalan is due to coalescence | 29 | 14 | 15 | 48.3% |
3x50j54v | Language-to-music transfer effects depend on the tone language: Akan vs. East Asian tone languages | 28 | 18 | 10 | 64.3% |
5gp0k1r4 | The time course of individuals' perception of coarticulatory information is linked to their production: Implications for sound change: Supplemental Material | 28 | 12 | 16 | 42.9% |
3c41c7tk | Investigating clausal wh-constructions in Romanian | 27 | 5 | 22 | 18.5% |
3xt17479 | Clusters and Classes in the Rhythm Metrics | 27 | 7 | 20 | 25.9% |
6nw663v5 | Head Internal Relative Clauses, Quantifier Float, the Definiteness Effect and the Mathematics of Determiners | 27 | 5 | 22 | 18.5% |
7tf3d01k | Perceptual evaluation of voice source models. | 26 | 15 | 11 | 57.7% |
Note: Due to the evolving nature of web traffic, the data presented here should be considered approximate and subject to revision. Learn more.