Skip to main content
eScholarship
Open Access Publications from the University of California

About

The annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society is aimed at basic and applied cognitive science research. The conference hosts the latest theories and data from the world's best cognitive science researchers. Each year, in addition to submitted papers, researchers are invited to highlight some aspect of cognitive science.

Workshops

The Fine Art of Conversation

This workshop is aimed at giving human interaction re-searchers the conceptual and practical apparatus to balancetheir representations of data (mixes of drawings and pho-tographs in the most part), so as to “maximally incite, but alsoconstrain” their representations, just as artists sometimes suc-ceed in doing (Streeck, Grothues, & Villanueva, 2009, p.28).Why—as Streeck points out—are the drawings and visuali-sations of interaction researchers so halting and timid, com-pared to the ways artists have responded to the same kinds ofrepresentational problems? Are these heavily segmented andsparsely constructed representations of interaction the resultof a prevailing positivistic outlook with regard to representingshared space, where interaction is presented as staggered anddiscrete physical events with apparently little to connect them.The workshop seeks to redress this situation by examining thesolutions that artists have arrived at when representing humaninteraction, and asking participants to engage in a series of ac-tivities and discussions which will re-frame their approachesto this issue.

Symposia

Anthropological Contributions to Cognitive Science

Anthropology was a founding member of cognitive science(Bender et al., 2010; Gardner, 1985), sharing with othercognitive disciplines a deep interest in thinking and behav-ior. With its unique expertise in the cultural content, con-text, and constitution of cognition, it would still be essentialto any comprehensive endeavor to explore the human mind(Bloch, 2012), but rather has turned into cognitive science’s“missing discipline” (Boden, 2006), thus leaving importantquestions unanswered or even unasked. Given that substan-tial shares of knowledge are implicit and that cognition issituated, distributed, embodied, and grounded in variousother ways, anthropological approaches provide privilegedaccess to investigation: for arriving at reasonable hypothe-ses, ensuring ecological validity, and even for coming upwith new research questions and paradigms (Astuti &Bloch, 2012; Hutchins, 2010; Nersessian, 2006).In line with recent calls for rapprochement in Topics inCognitive Science (Bender et al., 2012; Beller & Bender,2015), our symposium brings together scholars that repre-sent different branches of contemporary anthropology withdistinct perspectives—including ‘traditional’ social anthro-pology, cognitive anthropology and ethno-linguistics, cogni-tive ecology, evolutionary anthropology, and archaeology—to present what they consider to be indispensable contribu-tions to cognitive science.With our selection of authors, we hope to demonstrate thevalue of anthropological approaches for cognitive science aswell as the potential benefits of cross-disciplinary collabora-tion. Cognitive archaeologist Overmann discusses a theo-retical perspective on how mind, behavior, and materialartifacts interact to shape human cognition. Combining theirexpertise in linguistics and evolutionary anthropology, Ráczand Jordan investigate the design principles of kinship sys-tems as near-universal conceptual tools. With his back-ground in (ethno-)linguistics and cognitive anthropology,Le Guen uses Yucatec Maya sign languages to illustrate theimportance of cultural practices for shaping cognitive be-havior. Based on Hutchins’ cognitive ecology approach,Solberg speaks to questions at the intersection of anthropol-ogy and philosophy of science by illuminating the culturalframework of science production in a biology lab. And so-cial anthropologist Astuti concludes by taking a bird’s eyeview on how efforts to understand the human mind cruciallybenefit from acknowledging its historical origins and fromtaking the specific sociocultural contexts into consideration.Based on work some of which is published in high-qualityjournals (such as Science, Nature, PNAS, BBS, TiCS, Cur-rent Anthropology, or Cognition), these participants willoffer invaluable contributions to a more diverse, more inclu-sive, and hence more comprehensive cognitive science.

Game-XP: Action Games as Cognitive Science Paradigms

Why games? How could anyone consider action gamesas experimental paradigms for Cognitive Science? In 1973,as one of three strategies he proposed for advancing Cogni-tive Science, Allen Newell exhorted us to “accept a singlecomplex task and do all of it.” More specifically, he told usthat rather than taking an “experimental psychology as usualapproach” that, we should “focus on a series of experimentaland theoretical studies around a single complex task” so as todemonstrate that our theories of human cognition were pow-erful enough to explain, “a genuine slab of human behavior”with the studies fitting into a detailed theoretical picture. Ac-tion games represent the type of experimental paradigms thatNewell was advocating and the current state of programmingexpertise and laboratory equipment, along with the emer-gence of Big Data (Griffiths, 2015) and Naturally OccurringData Sets (NODS, Goldstone & Lupyan, 2016), provide thetechnologies and data needed to realize his vision. ActionGames enable us to escape from our field’s regrettable fo-cus on novice performance to develop theories that accountfor the full range of expertise through a twin focus on ex-pertise sampling (across individuals) and longitudinal studies(within individuals) of simple and complex tasks.This Symposium is inspired by the recent Action Gamesas Experimental Paradigms for Cognitive Science (Game-XP), issue of Topics in Cognitive Science (topiCS), April2017. It includes late-breaking work from some of the re-searchers represented in that topic as well as new work bynew researchers.

Talks: Papers

Burstiness across multimodal human interaction reveals differences betweenverbal and non-verbal communication

Recent studies of naturalistic face-to-face communication havedemonstrated temporal coordination patterns such as thesynchronization of verbal and non-verbal behavior, which providesevidence for the proposal that verbal and non-verbalcommunicative control derives from one system. In this study, weargue that the observed relationship between verbal and non-verbalbehaviors depends on the level of analysis. In a re-analysis of acorpus of naturalistic multimodal communication (Louwerse et al.,2012), we focus on measuring the temporal patterns of specificcommunicative behaviors in terms of their burstiness. Weexamined burstiness estimates across different roles of the speakerand different communicative channels. We observed moreburstiness for verbal versus non-verbal channels, and for moreversus less informative language sub-channels. These findingsdemonstrate a new method for analyzing temporal patterns incommunicative behaviors, and they suggest a more complexrelationship between verbal and non-verbal channels thansuggested by prior studies.

Enactive Mechanistic Explanation of Social Cognition

In this paper we examine an enactive approach to social cog-nition, a species of radical embodied cognition typically pro-posed as an alternative to traditional cognitive science. Ac-cording to enactivists, social cognition is best explained byreference to the social unit rather than the individuals that par-ticipate in it. We identify a methodological problem in thisapproach, namely a lack of clarity with respect to the modelof explanation it adopts. We review two complaints abouta mechanistic explanatory framework, popular in traditionalcognitive science, that prevent enactivists from embracing it.We argue that these complaints are unfounded and propose aconceptual model of enactive mechanistic explanation of so-cial cognition.

Human Visual Search as a Deep Reinforcement Learning Solution to a POMDP

When people search for a target in a novel image they oftenmake use of eye movements to bring the relatively high acuityfovea to bear on areas of interest. The strategies that controlthese eye movements for visual search have been of substantialscientific interest. In the current article we report a new com-putational model that shows how strategies for visual searchare an emergent consequence of perceptual/motor constraintsand approximately optimal strategies. The model solves a Par-tially Observable Markov Decision Process (POMDP) usingdeep Q-learning to acquire strategies that optimise the trade-off between speed and accuracy. Results are reported for theDistractor-ratio task.

Executive function and attention predict low-income preschoolers’active category learning

Recent studies find that school-age children learn better whenthey have active control during study. Yet little is knownabout how individual differences in strategy or cognitive con-trol skills may affect active learning for preschoolers, nor ifexperimental measures of active learning map onto real-worldlearning outcomes. The current study assesses 101 low-income5-year-olds on an active category learning task, and measuresof executive function, attention, and school readiness. We findthat preschoolers use an informative sampling strategy for cat-egories defined by stimuli features in 1D and when presentedwith a distractor dimension (2D). Children accurately classifyin 1D, but show mixed performance in 2D. Attention predictssampling accuracy, and working memory and inhibitory con-trol predict classification accuracy. Performance in the activelearning task predicts early math and pre-literacy skills. Thesefindings suggest that trial-by-trial learning decisions may re-veal insight into how cognitive control skills support the ac-quisition of knowledge.

Simulating behavioural interventions for developmental deficits: When improvingstrengths produces better outcomes than remediating weaknesses

Computational models of cognitive development have beenfrequently used to model impairments found in developmentaldisorders but relatively rarely to simulate behaviouralinterventions to remediate these impairments. One area ofcontroversy in practices of intervention is whether it is betterto attempt to remediate an area of weakness or to build on thechild’s strengths. We present an artificial neural networkmodel of productive vocabulary development simulatingchildren with word-finding difficulties. We contrast anintervention to remediate weakness (additional practice onnaming) with interventions to improve strengths (improvingphonological and semantic knowledge). Remediatingweakness served to propel the system more quickly along thesame atypical trajectory, while improving strengths producedlong-term increases in final vocabulary size. A combinationyielded the best outcome. The model represents the firstmechanistic demonstration of how interventions targetingstrengths may serve to improve behavioural outcomes indevelopmental disorders. The observed effects in the modelare in line with those observed empirically for children withword-finding difficulties.

A Bayesian Model of Memory for Text

The study of memory for texts has had an long tradition of re-search in psychology. According to most general accounts oftext memory, the recognition or recall of items in a text is basedon querying a memory representation that is built up on the ba-sis of background knowledge. The objective of this paper is todescribe and thoroughly test a Bayesian model of this generalaccount. In particular, we develop a model that describes howwe use our background knowledge to form memories as a pro-cess of Bayesian inference of the statistical patterns that areinherent in a text, followed by posterior predictive inference ofthe words that are typical of those inferred patterns. This pro-vides us with precise predictions about what words will be re-membered, whether veridically or erroneously, from any giventext. We then test these predictions using data from a memoryexperiment using a relatively large sample of randomly chosentexts from a representative corpus of British English.

Numbers Uniquely Bias Spatial Attention: A Novel Paradigm for UnderstandingSpatial-Numerical Associations

Over the past two-and-a-half decades, numerous empiricalstudies have demonstrated a relationship between numbersand space. A classic interpretation is that these spatial-numerical associations (SNAs) are a product of a stablemental number line (MNL) in the mind, yet others haveargued that SNAs are a product of transient mappings thatoccur in working memory. Importantly, although the latterinterpretation has no implications for the representation ofnumber, the former suggests that the representation ofnumber is inherently spatial. Here, we tease apart questionsof spatial representation (à la an MNL perspective) andspatial strategy (à la alternative accounts). In a novel place-the-number task, we demonstrate that numbersautomatically bias spatial attention whereas other ordinalsequences (i.e., letters) do not. We argue that this isevidence of an inherently spatial representation of numberand explore how this work may help answer futurequestions about the relationship between space andnumber.

Highly Proficient Bilinguals Maintain Language-Specific Pragmatic Constraints on Pronouns: Evidence from Speech and Gesture

The use of subject pronouns by bilingual speakers using both a pro-drop and a non-pro-drop language (e.g. Spanish heritage speakers in the USA) is a well-studied topic in research on cross-linguistic influence in language contact situations. Previous studies looking at bilinguals with different proficiency levels have yielded conflicting results on whether there is transfer from the non-pro-drop patterns to the pro-drop language. Additionally, previous research has focused on speech patterns only. In this paper, we study the two modalities of language, speech and gesture, and ask whether and how they reveal cross-linguistic influence on the use of subject pronouns in discourse. We focus on elicited narratives from heritage speakers of Turkish in the Netherlands, in both Turkish (pro- drop) and Dutch (non-pro-drop), as well as from monolingual control groups. The use of pronouns was not very common in monolingual Turkish narratives and was constrained by the pragmatic contexts, unlike in Dutch. Furthermore, Turkish pronouns were more likely to be accompanied by localized gestures than Dutch pronouns, presumably because pronouns in Turkish are pragmatically marked forms. We did not find any cross-linguistic influence in bilingual speech or gesture patterns, in line with studies (speech only) of highly proficient bilinguals. We therefore suggest that speech and gesture parallel each other not only in monolingual but also in bilingual production. Highly proficient heritage speakers who have been exposed to diverse linguistic and gestural patterns of each language from early on maintain monolingual patterns of pragmatic constraints on the use of pronouns multimodally.

Rise and fall of conflicting intuitions during reasoning

Recent dual process models proposed that the strength of competing intuitions determines reasoning performance. A key challenge at this point is to search for boundary conditions; identify cases in which the strength of different intuitions will be weaker/stronger. Therefore, we ran two studies with the two-response paradigm in which people are asked to give two answers to a given reasoning problem. We adopted base-rate problems in which base rate and stereotypic information can cue conflicting intuitions. By manipulating the information presentation order, we aimed to manipulate their saliency; and by that, indirectly the activation strength of the intuitions. Contrary to our expectation, we observed that the order manipulation had opposite effects in the initial and final response stages. We explain these results by taking into account that the strength of intuitions is not constant but changes over time; they have a peak, a growth, and a decay rate.

The Refugees’ Dilemma: not all deontological moral choices are of the same kind

The focus of the present work concerns the nature ofdeontological decisions. We test the hypothesis that it ispossible to specify deontological moral choices based on anunemotional rule, norm or principle and that such moralchoices can be distinguished from emotion-driven ones.Using a novel paradigm for moral choice that we call TheRefugees’ Dilemma, we provide evidence for such a rule-based route to moral choice. We show that participants withhigh scores in a Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) were morelikely to adopt utilitarian or rule-based responses, as opposedto emotional ones. We also found that rule-based respondentsreported the highest average psychological distance, more sothat even utilitarian respondents. These findings show howemotional and rule-based influences can be separated with theappropriate scenario and challenges the approach of assumingboth influences can be combined into a single deontologicalroute in dual-process models.

I know what you need to know: Children’s developing theory of mind andpedagogical evidence selection

Natural pedagogy emerges early in development (Knudsen &Liszkowski, 2012), but good teaching requires presenting ev-idence specific to learners’ knowledge (Shafto, Goodman, &Griffiths, 2014). How might the development of Theory ofMind (ToM) relate to the ability to select pedagogical evi-dence? We present a training study in which we investigatedthe link between preschool-aged children’s false-belief under-standing and their ability to select evidence for teaching. Ourresults suggest that children with more advanced ToM abili-ties were better evidence selectors, even when controlling foreffects of age and numerical conservation abilities. We alsofound that children who improved more in false-belief under-standing from pre- to post-test performed better on the peda-gogical tasks over the course of the training. Finally, we reporttentative evidence for a link between the pedagogical train-ing and improvements in ToM. Our findings suggest importantconnections between ToM and evidential reasoning in naturalpedagogy in early childhood.

Didn’t know, or didn’t show? Preschoolers consider epistemic state and degree ofomission when evaluating teachers

The ability to recognize and evaluate reliable informants is acritical skill for effective social learning. Building on priorwork showing children’s sensitivity to informants who omitrelevant information, here we asked whether children’s teacherevaluations incorporate information about 1) the epistemicstate of the teacher, and 2) the amount and value of informationtaught. Preschool-aged children rated informants who taughtlearners about a novel toy with four functions; we systemat-ically varied the number and value of functions the teachersknew and taught. Our results indicate that children exoner-ated unintentional omissions of teachers who had incompleteknowledge, and provided graded ratings based on the degree ofomission. These findings are consistent with the predictions ofprior computational work, and suggest that the ability to reasonabout others’ knowledge plays an important role in children’sinferences about others’ efficacy as informants.

odeling human categorization of natural images using deep featurerepresentations

Over the last few decades, cognitive scientists have developed sophisticated formal models of human categorization,and computer vision researchers have achieved increasingly impressive performance in natural image classification. In thispaper, we combine the strengths of these approaches, using representations from a convolutional neural network to evaluatecognitive models of categorization against >300,000 human judgments of natural images. We find that a prototype modelperforms best overall, and that an exemplar model performs best when the network’s most abstract features are used. Altogether,our results demonstrate that the optimal categorization strategy over a set of stimuli is deeply linked to how they are represented,suggesting that any satisfying characterization of categorization behavior over naturalistic stimuli must consider it the resultof a dual process of feature learning and strategy selection. The paradigm we present herein offers one avenue to begin thisundertaking.

Semantic Typology and Parallel Corpora: Something about Indefinite Pronouns

Patterns of crosslinguistic variation in the expression of wordmeaning are informative about semantic organization, but mostmethods to study this are labor intensive and obscure the gra-dient nature of concepts. We propose an automatic method forextracting crosslinguistic co-categorization patterns from par-allel texts, and explore the properties of the data as a potentialsource for automatically creating semantic representations forcognitive modeling. We focus on indefinite pronouns, com-paring our findings against a study based on secondary sources(Haspelmath 1997). We show that using automatic methods onparallel texts contributes to more cognitively-plausible seman-tic representations for a domain.

How Relative is the Relative Frame of Reference?Front and back in Norwegian, Farsi, German, and Japanese

Across languages, people differ in which of the three basicframes of reference (FoRs) they prefer when describingspatial relations: absolute, intrinsic, or relative. But how muchvariation is there with regard to the relative FoR, which isanchored in the observer and occurs as one of three variants?Is the reflection variant canonical, as assumed by manyscholars? And how are objects in a person’s back referred to:by turning towards the objects? Results from two studies, onewith speakers of Norwegian and Farsi, the other with speakersof German and Japanese, reveal that reflection is notcanonical, but that translation and even rotation are used aswell. In addition, turning towards objects arranged in aperson’s back is very rare; what people use instead is abackward projection strategy that goes without rotation.

Quantifying Infants' Statistical Word Segmentation: A Meta-Analysis

Theories of language acquisition and perceptual learning increasingly rely on statistical learning mechanisms. The current meta-analysis aims to clarify the robustness of this capacity in infancy within the word segmentation literature. Our analysis reveals a significant, small effect size for conceptual replications of Saffran, Aslin, & Newport (1996), and a nonsignificant effect across all studies that incorporate transitional probabilities to segment words. In both conceptual replications and the broader literature, however, statistical learning is moderated by whether stimuli are naturally produced or synthesized. These findings invite deeper questions about the complex factors that influence statistical learning, and the role of statistical learning in language acquisition.

Narrowing of the Cone-of-Direct Gaze Through Reinforcement Learning

The Cone of Direct Gaze (CoD) is described as the range ofeye gaze deviations over which an observer reports gaze asbeing directed towards them. The CoD has been found tonarrow with age across childhood (Mareschal et al. 2016). Weinvestigated whether reinforcement learning, so critical inshaping eye gaze responses in infancy, was able to accountfor the emergence of a CoD and its narrowing in childhood.To this end, we adapted Triesch et al.'s (2006) reinforcementlearning model by (1) defining a topology over objectlocations, and (2) introducing opponent non-linear rewardprofiles for looking at objects and caregivers. In Simulation 1we show that these modifications give rise to a functionalCoD in which there is reduced eye gaze following andincreased fixation on the caregiver for locations with a smallcaregiver eye gaze eccentricity. In Simulation 2 we show thatthe width of this effect reduces with learning, suggesting thatdevelopmental decreases in the CoD may be driven byreinforcement learning. In Simulation 3 we explore howchanges in model parameters can explain the CoD in highanxiety populations. Finally, the model provides one way ofunifying the developmental gaze-following and CoDliteratures, until now considered largely independent.

Functionally localized representations contain distributed information:insight from simulations of deep convolutional neural networks

Preferential activation to faces in the brain’s fusiform gyrus hasled to the proposed existence of a face module termed theFusiform Face Area (FFA) (Kanwisher et. al, 1997). However,arguments for distributed, topographical object-formrepresentations in FFA and across visual cortex have beenproposed to explain data showing that FFA activation patternscontain decodable information about non-face categories(Haxby et. al, 2001; Hanson & Schmidt, 2011). Using two deepconvolutional neural network models able to perform human-level object and facial recognition, respectively, wedemonstrate that both localized category representations(LCRs) and high-level face-specific representations allow forsimilar decoding accuracy between non-preferred visualcategories as between a preferred and non-preferred category.Our results suggest that neuroimaging of a cortical “module”optimized for face processing should yield significantdecodable information for non-face categories so long asrepresentations within the module are activated by non-facestimuli.

Inferential Role Semantics for Natural Language

Cognitive models have long been used to study linguistic phe-nomena spanning the domains of phonology, syntax, and se-mantics. Of these domains, semantics is somewhat unique inthat there is little clarity concerning what a model needs to beable to do in order to provide an account of how the mean-ings of complex linguistic expressions, such as sentences, areunderstood. To help address this problem, we introduce a tree-structured neural model that is trained to generate further sen-tences that follow from an input sentence. These further sen-tences chart out the “inferential role” of the input sentence,which we argue constitutes an important part of its meaning.The model is trained using the Stanford Natural Language In-ference (SNLI) dataset, and to evaluate its performance, we re-port entailment prediction accuracies on a set of test sentencesnot present in the training data. We also report the results of asimple study that compares human plausibility ratings for bothground-truth and model-generated entailments for a randomselection of sentences in this test set. Finally, we examine anumber of qualitative features of the model’s ability to gener-alize. Taken together, these analyses indicate that our modelis able to accurately account for important inferential relation-ships amongst linguistic expressions.

Split-Second Detection of Cooperativeness from Faces in the Trust Game

Economic interactions often imply to gauge the trustworthiness of others. Recent studies showed that when makingtrust decisions in economic games, people have some accuracy in detecting trustworthiness from the facial features of unknownpartners. Here we provide evidence that this face-based trustworthiness detection is a fast and intuitive process by testing itsperformance at split-second levels of exposure. Participants played a Trust game, in which they made decisions whether totrust another player based on their picture. In two studies, we manipulated the exposure time of the picture. We observed thattrustworthiness detection remained better than chance for exposure times as short as 100 ms, although it disappeared with anexposure time of 33ms. We discuss implications for ongoing debates on the use of facial inferences for social and economicdecisions.

Diversity in a Contrast Set Increases Generalization from a Single-Item Target

Four experiments explored the effect of diversity of contrasting negative evidence on inductive inferences drawnfrom a single-item target. In Experiments 1 and 2, we found that increasing the diversity of a contrast set led people to inferthat a target exemplar corresponded to a higher level category and led to greater generalization of a novel property associatedwith the target. Further, we demonstrated two boundary conditions in which the effect only occurred when the contrast setwas consistent with a higher level category that both united the contrast exemplars and distinguished them from the target(Experiment 4) and when contrast and target shared an obvious parent category (Experiment 5). Taken together, these findingsdemonstrate that increasing the diversity of a contrast increases generalization from a target, but only if the contrast set is drawnfrom a single category that excludes, but shares a common parent with, the target.

Causal learning from interventions and dynamics in continuous time

Event timing and interventions are important and intertwinedcues to causal structure, yet they have typically been studiedseparately. We bring them together for the first time in an ex-periment where participants learn causal structure by perform-ing interventions in continuous time. We contrast learning inacyclic and cyclic devices, with reliable and unreliable cause–effect delays. We show that successful learners use interven-tions to structure and simplify their interactions with the de-vices and that we can capture judgment patterns with heuristicsbased on online construction and testing of a single structural

Harmony in a non-harmonic language: word order learning in French children

Recent studies using artificial language learning have arguedthat the cross-linguistic frequency of harmonic word orderpatterns–in which heads are ordered consistently before or af-ter dependents across syntactic categories–reflects a cognitivebias (Culbertson, Smolensky, & Legendre, 2012; Culbertson& Newport, 2015a). These studies suggest that English speak-ing adults and children favor harmonic orders of nouns anddifferent nominal modifiers (adjectives, numerals). However,because they target English learners, whose native languageis harmonic in the nominal domain (Num-Adj-N), this pref-erence may be based on transfer rather than a universal biasfor harmony. We present new evidence from French-speakingchildren, whose native language is non-harmonic in this do-main (Num-N-Adj). Our results reveal clear effects of nativelanguage transfer, but also evidence that a harmonic pattern isfavored even in this population of learners.

How can I help? 24- to 48-month-olds provide helpspecific to the cause of others’ failed actions

When young children see others fail to achieve a goal, theyspontaneously help. But there are many reasons why someonemight fail, and consequently, many ways to help. In order tohelp effectively, we need to understand why someone is fail-ing, so we can address the cause. One important distinction iswhether the failure is due to the agent’s own actions or some-thing external to her in the world. Here we show that 24- to48-month-olds can use their past experience to reason aboutthe probable cause of another person’s failure and provide helpappropriate for that cause. Children’s help targeted the worldwhen their prior knowledge suggested that the source of fail-ure was external to the agent, and targeted the person’s actionswhen this source appeared to be internal to the agent.

Watching Non-Corresponding Gestures Helps Learners with High VisuospatialAbility to Learn about Movements with Dynamic Visualizations: An fNIRS Study

This study investigates whether making and observing(human) gestures facilitates learning about non-humanbiological movements and whether correspondence betweengesture and to-be-learned movement is superior to non-correspondence. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy wasused to address whether gestures activate the human mirror-neuron system (hMNS) and whether this activation mediatesthe facilitation of learning. During learning, participantsviewed the animations of the to-be-learned movements twice.Depending on the condition, the second viewing wassupplemented with either a self-gesturing instruction (Y/N)and/or a gesture video (corresponding/non-corresponding/no).Results showed that high-visuospatial-ability learners showedbetter learning outcomes with non-corresponding gestures,whereas those gestures were detrimental for low-visuospatial-ability learners. Furthermore, the activation of the inferior-parietal cortex (part of the hMNS) tended to predict betterlearning outcomes. Unexpectedly, making gestures did notinfluence learning, but cortical activation differed for learnerswho self-gestured depending on which gesture they observed.Results and implications are discussed.

Learning on Multi-Touch Devices: The influence of the distance betweeninformation in pop-ups and the hands of the users

Prior research indicated that information processing isinfluenced by the proximity of the hands to information:visuospatial processing is fostered near the hands, whereastextual processing might not be affected or even inhibitednear the hands. This study investigated how the proximity ofthe hands to digital information in pop-ups influenceslearning outcomes on multi-touch devices. Depending on thedistance between the information in the pop-ups and thehands of the users there were three conditions: (1) all pop-upsopened near the hands, (2) all pop-ups opened far from thehands, and (3) pop-ups with visuospatial information openednear the hands, whereas pop-ups with textual informationopened far from the hands (mixed condition). Results showedbetter learning outcomes when visuospatial pop-ups arepresented near the hands, whereas there was no difference inlearning outcomes between near and far presented textualpop-ups. Results and implications for multi-touch designs arediscussed.

A Flexible Mapping Scheme for Discrete and DimensionalEmotion Representations: Evidence from Textual Stimuli

While research on emotions has become one of the most pro-ductive areas at the intersection of cognitive science, artifi-cial intelligence and natural language processing, the diversityand incommensurability of emotion models seriously hampersprogress in the field. We here propose kNN regression as asimple, yet effective method for computationally mapping be-tween two major strands of emotion representations, namelydimensional and discrete emotion models. In a series of ma-chine learning experiments on data sets of textual stimuli wegather evidence that this approach reaches a human level ofreliability using a relatively small number of data points only.

Maintenance of Perceptual Information in Speech Perception

Acoustic and contextual cues to linguistic categories (e.g.,phonemes or words) tend to be temporally distributed acrossthe speech signal. Optimal cue integration thus requires main-tenance of subcategorical information over time. At the sametime, previous work suggests that finite sensory memory orprocessing capacity strongly limits how much subcategoricalinformation can be maintained (or for how long). We ar-gue that previous work might have over-interpreted the roleof these limitations. In two perception experiments, we findno limit in the ability to maintain subcategorical information.We also find that maintenance seems to be the default, neitherlimited to perceptually particularly ambiguous signals, nor alearned strategy specific to our experiment. In contrast, listen-ers’ decision for how long to delay categorization, we find, is afunction of perceptual ambiguity. It is therefore crucial to dis-tinguish between in-principle abilities (even when they reflectdefault processing), and decisions made within the bounds ofthose abilities.

Task-oriented Bayesian inference in interval timing: People use their prior reproduction experience to calibrate time reproduction

The estimation of duration has been shown to follow Bayesian inference, where people use their prior belief to calibrate the estimation. This explains timing biases such as the range bias where a duration is reproduced as longer when previously encountered durations were longer than shorter. However, it is unclear whether prior belief is based on previously perceived or reproduced durations. In 4 experiments, we show that the range bias occurs between short and long reproduction ranges but not between short and long perception ranges. Further analyses also show that the prior is updated by the most recent reproduced (but not perceived) duration. Together these results support a task-oriented Bayesian inference account of time reproduction, where people use the perceived duration and their past reproduction experience to make an inference about how much time to reproduce.

The most efficient sequence of study depends on the type of test

Previous research has shown that the sequence in whichconcepts are studied changes how well they are learned. In aseries of experiments featuring naturalistic concepts(psychology concepts) and naïve learners, we extend previousresearch by showing that the sequence of study changes therepresentation the learner creates of the study materials.Interleaved study leads to the creation of relatively interrelatedconcepts that are represented by contrast to each other andbased on discriminating properties. Blocked study, instead,leads to the creation of relatively isolated concepts that arerepresented in terms of their central and characteristicproperties. The relative benefits of these representationsdepend on whether the test of conceptual knowledge requirescontrastive or characteristic information. These results arguefor the integrated investigation of the benefits of differentsequences of study as depending on the characteristics of thestudy and testing situation as a whole.

s there an explicit learning bias?Students beliefs, behaviors and learning outcomes

Learning by doing refers to learning practices that involvecompleting activities as opposed to explicit learning (e.g.,reading). Although the benefits of learning by doing have beendescribed before, it is still relatively uncommon in instructionalpractice. We investigated how much students employ learningby doing in online courses, and whether it is associated withimproved learning outcomes. Spending more time completingactivities had a larger impact on learning outcomes thanspending more time reading, even in the case of mostlydeclarative content, such as in a Psychology course. Moreover,learning by doing is more efficient: grade improvements of 1standard-deviation require 10-20% less time in learning bydoing than reading. Finally, we contrast this evidence withstudents’ a priori intuitions on best study strategies for theironline course. Students overestimate the value of explicitlearning through reading, and underestimate the value of activelearning.

 A Model of Cognitive Control in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test:Integrating Schema Theory and Basal Ganglia Function

 We present a schema-based model of a classicneuropsychological task, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task(WCST), where competition between motor and cognitiveschemas is resolved using a variation of a neuroanatomicallydetailed model of the basal ganglia (Gurney et al., 2001). Weshow that the model achieves a good fit with existing data atthe group level, and correctly identifies two distinct cognitivemechanisms held to underlie two distinct types of error.However, at the individual level, the correlations amongstother error types produced by the model differ from thoseobserved in the human data. To address this, we clusterparticipant performance into distinct groups and show, byfitting each group separately, how the model can account forthe empirically observed correlations between error types.Methodologically, this demonstrates the importance ofmodelling participant performance at the sub-group orindividual level, rather than modelling group performance.We also discuss implications of the model for the WCSTperformance of elderly participants and Parkinson’s patients.

Distributional learning and lexical category acquisition:What makes words easy to categorize?

In this study, results of computational simulations on Englishchild-directed speech are presented to uncover what distribu-tional properties of words make it easier to group them intolexical categories. This analysis provides evidence that wordsare easier to categorize when (i) they are hard to predict giventhe contexts they occur in; (ii) they occur in few different con-texts; and (iii) their contextual distributions have a low entropy,meaning that they tend to occur more often in one of the con-texts they occur in. This profile fits that of content words, espe-cially nouns and verbs, which is consistent with developmentalevidence showing that children learning English start by form-ing a noun and a verb category. These results further charac-terize the role of distributional information in lexical categoryacquisition and confirm that it is a robust, reliable, and devel-opmentally plausible source to learn lexical categories.

Knowledge transfer in a probabilistic Language Of Thought

In many domains, people are able to transfer abstract knowl-edge about objects, events, or contexts that are superficiallydissimilar, enabling striking new insights and inferences. Weprovide evidence that this ability is naturally explained as theaddition of new primitive elements to a compositional mentalrepresentation, such as that in the probabilistic Language OfThought (LOT). We conducted a transfer-learning experimentin which participants learned about two sequences, one afterthe other. We show that participants’ ability to learn the secondsequence is affected the first sequence they saw. We test twoprobabilistic models to evaluate alternative theories of how al-gorithmic knowledge is transferred from the first to second se-quence: one model rationally updates the prior probability ofthe primitive operations in the LOT based on what was used inthe first sequence; the other stores previously likely hypothesesas new primitives. Both models perform better than baselinesin explaining behavior, with the human subjects appearing totransfer entire hypotheses when they can, and otherwise updat-ing the prior on primitives.

Cross-situational learning of novel anaphors

Word learning research has shown that learners constrain thehypothesis space for word meanings by using multiplesources of information, such as cross-situational regularitiesof word-context co-occurrences or syntactic cues, like thenumber of arguments. These studies typically focus on wordmeaning development where these cues can be helpful but notnecessary. As such, it sheds little light on the acquisition ofanaphors, which requires tracking syntactic dependenciesacross situations. To test whether or how learners track thisinformation, we conducted a novel anaphor learningexperiment with English and Japanese speakers, manipulatingcross-situational regularities in anaphors and their syntacticdependencies. Results show both English and Japanesespeakers closely track the frequency of interpretivepossibilities for novel anaphors. However, they demonstratedifficulties learning long-distance reflexives, which arecompatible with either local or non-local antecedents. Thissuggests that successful anaphor learning requires more thancross-situational regularities of interpretive possibilities.

Expected Utility in Romantic Relationships: Satisfaction as a Cue for Romantic Partnership Dissolution

The choice to enter and leave a romantic relationship can be framed as a decision-making problem based on expected utility of the partnership over time, akin to a forager deciding whether to stay in a particular patch based on the amount of resources it provides. We examined the temporal trajectory of three traits that may correspond to resources in romantic relationships—trust, love, and satisfaction—to determine whether they behave like depleting or replenishing patches from a foraging perspective. All three rise over time in intact relationships—suggesting replenishment—but plateau or fall in dissolved relationships—suggesting depletion. Survival analysis demonstrated that higher ratings of all three quality variables decreased the risk of romantic dissolution. The results suggest that these cues are lower in dissolved relationships, indicating individuals could potentially use them as cues for leaving an unsatisfactory relationship patch via aspiration-level cognitive mechanisms.

Analogical gestures foster understanding of causal systems

Sensitivity to the causal structure underlying phenomena iscritical to expert understanding. Fostering such understandingin learners is therefore a key goal in education. Wehypothesized that observing analogical gestures—whichrepresent relational information in visuospatial format—would lead learners to notice and reason about underlyingcausal patterns, such as positive and negative feedback.Participants watched brief video lectures about the humanbody and the plant kingdom, which were delivered along withgestures representing either: 1) visuospatial details (iconicgesture condition); or 2) relational structure (analogicalgesture condition). In a subsequent classification task, relativeto participants who saw iconic gestures, participants who sawanalogical gestures were more likely to sort the phenomenadescribed in the videos—as well as novel phenomena—bytheir causal structure (e.g., positive feedback). The resultssuggest that analogical gestures can be harnessed to fostercausal understanding.

Characterizing spatial construction processes:Toward computational tools to understand cognition

Spatial construction—creating or copying spatialarrangements—is a hallmark of human spatial cognition.Spatial construction appears early in development, predictslater spatial and mathematical skills, and is used throughoutlife. Despite its importance, we know little about the cognitiveprocesses underlying skilled construction. Construction tasksare highly complex but analyses have tended to focus onbroad-stroke measures of end-goal accuracy. In this paper weintroduce a novel behavioral coding formalism to characterizean individual’s entire construction process, examine manyindividuals’ processes in aggregate, and summarize patternsthat emerge. The results show high consistency at certainpoints occurring throughout the construction, but also indicateflexibility in the interim paths that lead to and diverge fromthese points. Our approach offers a new method that can moreprecisely describe the behavioral patterns observed duringconstruction in order to reveal the underlying cognitiveprocesses engaged, and capture individual differences inbuilding expertise.

Mathematical invariants in people’s probabilistic reasoning

Recent research has identified three invariants or identities thatappear to hold in people’s probabilistic decision making: theaddition law identity, the Bayes rule identity, and the QQidentity (Costello and Watts, 2014, Fisher and Wolfe, 2014,Costello and Watts, 2016b, Wang and Busemeyer, 2013, Wanget al., 2014). Each of these identities represent specific agree-ment with the requirements of normative probability theory;strikingly, these identities seem to hold in people’s probabilityjudgments despite the presence of strong and systematic bi-ases against the requirements of normative probability theoryin those very same judgments. We assess the degree to whichtwo formal models of probabilistic reasoning (the ‘probabilitytheory plus noise’ model and the ‘quantum probability’ model)can explain these identities and biases in probabilistic reason-ing.

Experts are better than novices when imagining wines, but not odors in general

Olfactory imagery is disputed to exist in novices, but is reported to be easier for smell experts. It plays an importantrole in wine expertise. Previous research shows experts’ superior cognitive abilities do not transfer beyond their domain ofexpertise. This leads to two questions: do wine experts have more vivid imagery for the multisensory experience of wine? Andhow general is wine experts’ olfactory imagery? Wine experts and novices completed a questionnaire measuring the vividnessof imagery for the color, smell, and flavor of wine. In addition, all participants completed a questionnaire on general smellimagery. Wine experts were better than novices at imagining wines in all modalities, but not better at imagining smells ingeneral. Novices reported the strongest imagery for the appearance of wine, but experts showed no difference between thesenses. So mental imagery becomes more vivid with expertise; but only for imagery directly expertise related.

The elusive oddness of or-introduction

The inference of or-introduction, p, therefore p or q, is fundamental in classical logic and probability theory. Yet traditional research in the psychology of reasoning found that people did not endorse this inference as highly as other one- premise valid inferences. A radical response to this finding is to claim that or-introduction is in fact invalid. This response is found in the recent revision of mental model theory (MMT). We argue that this revision of the theory leads to a number of logical problems and counterintuitive consequences for valid inferences, and present an experiment extending recent studies showing that people readily accept or-introduction under probabilistic instructions. We argue for a pragmatic explanation of why the inference is sometimes considered odd. The inference is not odd when people reason from their degrees of belief.

A Dynamic Tradeoff Model of Intertemporal Choice

The delay discounting perspective, which assumes an alternative-wise processing of attribute information, has long dominated research on intertemporal choice. Recent studies, however, have suggested that intertemporal choice is based on attribute-wise comparison. This line of research culminated in the tradeoff model (Scholten & Read, 2010; Scholten, Read, & Sanborn, 2014), which can accommodate most established behavioral regularities in intertemporal choice. One drawback of the tradeoff model, however, is that it is static, providing no account of the dynamic process leading to a choice. Here we develop a dynamic tradeoff model that can qualitatively account for empirical findings in intertemporal choice regarding not only choices but also response times. The dynamic model also outperforms the original, static tradeoff model when quantitatively fitting choices from representative data sets, and even outperforms the best-performing dynamic model derived from Decision Field Theory in Dai and Busemeyer (2014) when fitting both choices and response times.

What is Learning? A Definition for Cognitive Science

Many intuitive notions of “learning” do not support thediverse kinds of learning across different situations andlearners. In this paper I offer a functional definition oflearning from a cognitive science perspective, which attemptsto account for the presence of learning in different physicalsubstrates. The definition is that a particular event should beconsidered a good example of “learning” to the degree towhich the following characteristics describe it: 1) a systemundergoes change to its informational state or processing 2)the change is for the purpose of more effective future action,3) the change is in response what the system experiences, and4) the system executes the change, rather than some outsideforce. Episodes are better examples of learning according tohow many of these characteristics they have. I discussbenefits and limitations of this characterization.

Re-representation in comparison and similarity

Re-representation is a crucial component of structure mappingtheory, allowing individuals to notice structural commonalitiesbetween situations that do not initially have identical relationalrepresentations. Despite its theoretical importance, however, thisconcept has been the subject of very little empirical work. In twoexperiments, we find that a case’s participation in one comparisonsystematically changes its perceived similarity to new cases, in apattern consistent with re-representation. Additional work rules outalternative explanations based on relational priming.

Comprehenders Rationally Adapt Semantic Predictions to the Statistics of the Local Environment: a Bayesian Model of Trial-by-Trial N400 Amplitudes

When semantic information is activated by a context prior to new bottom-up input (i.e. when a word is predicted), semantic processing of that incoming word is typically facilitated, attenuating the amplitude of the N400 event related potential (ERP) – a direct neural measure of semantic processing. This N400 modulation is observed even when the context is just a single semantically related “prime” word. This so-called “N400 semantic priming effect” is sensitive to the probability of seeing a related prime-target pair within experimental blocks, suggesting that participants may be adapting the strength of their predictions to the predictive validity of their broader experimental environment. We formalize this adaptation using an optimal Bayesian learner, and link this model to N400 amplitudes using an information-theoretic measure, surprisal. We found that this model could account for the N400 amplitudes evoked by words (whether related or unrelated) as adaptation unfolds across individual trials. These findings suggest that comprehenders may rationally adapt their semantic predictions to the statistical structure of their broader environment, with implications for the functional significance of the N400 component and the predictive nature of language processing.

The Role of Imagination in Exemplar Generation: The Effects of Conflict and Explanation

Structured imagination refers to reliance upon prior knowledge when generating novel examples of a provided category. Yet studies supporting this tenet use experimental designs where the stimuli themselves cue exemplars based on culturally relevant items. The present study combined exemplar generation with abstract stimuli as a means of attenuating instructional bias. Participants were shown a group of abstract shapes identified as a single category and instructed to generate another member of this category. We additionally examined whether the introduction of a cognitive conflict (by including an anomalous category member) and self-explanation during generation affected the level of imaginative responses. Contrary to expectations, the presentation of a conflicting category member did not result in more imaginative responses when compared to more homogenous stimuli sets. However, a significantly greater degree of imaginative responses was observed from participants who were required to explain their thinking prior to and whilst constructing their exemplars.

A hierarchical Bayesian model of “memory for when”based on experience sampling data

Participants wore a smartphone, which collected GPS, audio,accelerometry and image data, in a pouch around their necksfor a period of two weeks. After a retention interval of oneweek, they were asked to judge the specific day on whicheach of a selection of images was taken. To account forpeople’s judgements, we proposed a mixture model of fourprocesses - uniform guessing, a signal detection process basedon decaying memory strength, a week confusion process anda event confusion process in which the sensor streams wereused to calculate the similarity of events. A model selectionexercise testing all possible subsets of the processes favoureda model that included only the event confusion model. GPSsimilarities were found to be the most significant predictors,followed by audio and accelerometry similarities and thenimage similarities.

Refixations gather new visual information rationally

The standard model is that word identification in reading isa holistic process, most efficient when words are centered inthe visual field. In contrast, rational models of reading predictword identification to be a constructive process, where readersefficiently gather visual information about a word, and maycombine visual information about different parts of the wordobtained across multiple fixations to identify it. We tease apartthese accounts by arguing that rational models should predictthat the most efficient place in a word to make a second fixa-tion (refixation) depends on the visual information the readerhas already obtained. We present evidence supporting this pre-diction from an eye movement corpus. Computational modelsimulations confirm that a rational model predicts this find-ing, but a model implementing holistic identification does not.These results suggest that refixations can be well understoodas rationally gathering visual information, and that word iden-tification works constructively.

A rational analysis of curiosity

We present a rational analysis of curiosity, proposing that peo-ple’s curiosity is driven by seeking stimuli that maximize theirability to make appropriate responses in the future. This per-spective offers a way to unify previous theories of curiosityinto a single framework. Experimental results confirm ourmodel’s predictions, showing how the relationship between cu-riosity and confidence can change significantly depending onthe nature of the environment.

Refining the distributional hypothesis:A role for time and context in semantic representation

Distributional models of semantics assume that the meaningof a given word is a function of the contexts in which itoccurs. In line with this, prior research suggests that a word’ssemantic representation can be manipulated – pushed towarda target meaning, for example – by situating that word indistributional contexts frequented by the target. Left open toquestion is the role that order plays in the distributionalconstruction of meaning. Learning occurs in time, and it canproduce asymmetric outcomes depending on the order inwhich information is presented. Discriminative learningmodels predict that systematically manipulating a word’spreceding context should more strongly influence its meaningthan should varying what follows. We find support for thishypothesis in three experiments in which we manipulatedsubjects’ contextual experience with novel and marginallyfamiliar words, while varying the locus of manipulation.

Cute Little Puppies and Nice Cold Beers:An Information Theoretic Analysis of Prenominal Adjectives

A central goal of typological research is to characterizelinguistic features in terms of both their functional role andtheir fit to social and cognitive systems. One longstandingpuzzle concerns why certain languages employ grammaticalgender. In an information theoretic analysis of German nounclassification, Dye et al. (2017) enumerated a number ofimportant processing advantages gender confers. Yet thisraises a further puzzle: If gender systems are so beneficial toprocessing, what does this mean for languages that make dowithout them? Here, we compare the communicative functionof gender marking in German (a deterministic system) to thatof prenominal adjectives in English (a probabilistic one),finding that despite their differences, both systems act toefficiently smooth information over discourse, making nounsmore equally predictable in context. We examine whyevolutionary pressures may favor one system over another,and discuss the implications for compositional accounts ofmeaning and Gricean principles of communication.

Vanishing the mirror effect:The influence of prior history & list composition on recognition memory

In the study of recognition memory, a mirror effect iscommonly observed for word frequency, with low frequencyitems yielding both a higher hit rate and lower false alarm ratethan high frequency items. The finding that LF itemsconsistently outperform HF items in recognition was oncetermed the “frequency paradox”, as LF items are less wellrepresented in memory. However, recognition is known to beinfluenced both by ‘context noise’—the prior contexts inwhich an item has appeared—and ‘item noise’—interferencefrom other items present within the list context. In a typicalrecognition list, HF items will suffer more interference thanLF items. To illustrate this principle, we deliberatelymanipulated both the contexts in which critical items hadbeen encountered prior to study, and the confusability oftargets and distractors. Our results suggest that when noisesources are balanced, the mirror effect disappears.

Creating words from iterated vocal imitation

We report the results of a large-scale (N=1571) experiment toinvestigate whether spoken words can emerge from the processof repeated imitation. Participants played a version of the chil-dren’s game “Telephone”. The first generation was asked toimitate recognizable environmental sounds (e.g., glass break-ing, water splashing); subsequent generations imitated the im-itators for a total of 8 generations. We then examined whetherthe vocal imitations became more stable and word-like, re-tained a resemblance to the original sound, and became moresuitable as learned category labels. The results showed (1) theimitations became progressively more word-like, (2) even af-ter 8 generations, they could be matched above chance to theenvironmental sound that motivated them, and (3) imitationsfrom later generations were more effective as learned cate-gory labels. These results show how repeated imitation cancreate progressively more word-like forms while retaining asemblance of iconicity.

A Model of Event Knowledge

We present a connectionist model of event knowledge that istrained on examples of sequences of activities that are notexplicitly labeled as events. The model learns co-occurrencepatterns among the components of activities as they occur inthe moment (entities, actions, and contexts), and also learns topredict sequential patterns of activities. In so doing, the modeldisplays behaviors that in humans have been characterized asexemplifying inferencing of unmentioned event components,the prediction of upcoming components (which may or maynot ever happen or be mentioned), reconstructive memory,and the ability to flexibly accommodate novel variations frompreviously encountered experiences. All of these behaviorsemerge from what the model learns.

The role of intentionality in causal attribution is culturally mediated: evidence from Chinese, Mayan, and Spanish populations

Speakers of Mandarin, Spanish, and Yucatec Maya watched videos of two actors involved in a causal chain initiated by one of them. After watching each video, participants divided 10 tokens into piles indicating their assignment of responsibility for the resulting event. There was a significant interaction between intentionality and population: causer and causee intentionality made a significant difference only for the Spanish and Yucatec participants, but not for the Chinese participants. This is in line with previous findings suggesting that internal dispositions play a lesser role in responsibility attribution in societies in which attention to individual agency is far more common than attention to group agency.

Explaining Enculturated Cognition

Many of our cognitive capacities are shaped by enculturation. Enculturation is the temporally extended transformative acquisition of cognitive practices such as reading, writing, and mathematics. They are embodied and normatively constrained ways to interact with epistemic resources (e.g., writing systems, number systems). Enculturation is associated with significant changes of the organization and connectivity of the brain and of the functional profiles of embodied actions and motor programs. Furthermore, it has a socio-culturally structured dimension, because it relies on cumulative cultural evolution and on the socially distributed acquisition of cognitive norms governing the engagement with epistemic resources. This paper argues that we need distinct, yet complementary levels of explanation and corresponding temporal scales. This leads to explanatory pluralism about enculturated cognition, which is the view that we need multiple perspectives and explanatory strategies to account for the complexity of enculturation.

A Cognitive-Pharmacokinetic Computational Model of the Effect of Toluene onPerformance

We developed a cognitive-pharmacokinetic computational(CPC) model to understand how pharmacoactive substances,such as caffeine and toluene, modulate cognition. In this in-tegrated model, dynamic physiological mechanisms are sim-ulated to predict concentrations of the solvent toluene in thebrain, which modulates specific cognitive systems in a dose-response fashion over multiple hours. We used our CPC modelto reanalyze the results from prior research that documented anincrease in reaction time following exposure to toluene in sev-eral laboratory tasks with no change in accuracy. Our analysisprovides tentative evidence that toluene affects motor execu-tion, rather than attention or declarative memory.

Thinking about the future: The role of spatial metaphors for time

People often use spatial language to talk about time, and thisis known to both reflect and shape how they think about it.Despite much research on the spatial grounding of temporallanguage and thought, little attention has been given to howspatial metaphors influence reasoning about real events,especially those in the future. In a large online study(N=2362), we framed a discussion of climate change usingspatial metaphors that varied on reference-frame (ego- vs.time-moving), speed of movement (fast vs. slow), and timehorizon (near, medium, or far future). We found thatdescribing climate change as approaching (time-movingframe) – versus something we approach – made the issueseem more serious, but also more tractable, at least when therate of motion was fast (e.g., “it’s rapidly approaching”).These findings offer novel insights into the relationshipbetween spatial metaphors and temporal reasoning and howwe communicate about uncertain future events.

Early Colour Word Learning in British Infants

Colour word learning has traditionally been viewed as a diffi-cult task. Previous accounts have focussed on infants’ ability toshow an adult-like understanding of colour terms. Here we ex-amine whether infants understand colour terms at a basic level,using two different methods: first, evidence from parental re-ports that British infants can comprehend colour terms early,second from experimental data using eye-tracking. These find-ing show that colour word learning is a process that beginsmuch earlier than previously thought, and develops slowly asinfants learn where the boundaries of each term are located.Due to their abstract properties, colour words present a uniqueopportunity to assess category learning in infants, as well asthe mechanisms that control word learning in general.

Word Identification Under Multimodal Uncertainty

Identifying the visual referent of a spoken word – that a partic-ular insect is referred to by the word “bee” – requires both theability to process and integrate multimodal input and the abil-ity to reason under uncertainty. How do these tasks interactwith one another? We introduce a task that allows us to ex-amine how adults identify words under joint uncertainty in theauditory and visual modalities. We propose an ideal observermodel of the task which provides an optimal baseline. Modelpredictions are tested in two experiments where word recogni-tion is made under two kinds of uncertainty: category ambigu-ity and distorting noise. In both cases, the ideal observer modelexplains much of the variance in human judgments. But whenone modality had noise added to it, human perceivers system-atically preferred the unperturbed modality to a greater extentthan the ideal observer model did.

Could both be right? Children’s and adults’ sensitivity to subjectivity in language

While some word meanings, like “spotted,” depend on in-tersubjectively accessible properties of the world, others like“pretty” invoke speakers’ subjective beliefs. We explored chil-dren and adults’ sensitivity to the subjectivity of a range ofadjectives, including words like “spotted” and “pretty,” butalso words like “tall,” which are evaluated relative to a stan-dard. Participants saw two speakers who had independentlyexperienced sets of exemplars of a novel object kind disagreeabout whether a critical exemplar was, e.g., “tall,” “pretty,” and“spotted.” In Experiments 1 and 3, speakers had seen distinctsets of exemplars, while in Experiments 2 and 4, the sets wereidentical. Adults always judged disagreements over words like“pretty” as faultless—indicating that both speakers “could beright”—and permitted less faultless disagreement for ones like“tall” when the speakers had experienced identical sets of ex-emplars. Strikingly, children did not respond in an adult-likemanner until age 8 or 9, but their explanations for speakers’conflicting assertions suggested some sensitivity to the kindsof knowledge relevant for evaluating different adjectives.

Word Embedding Distance Does not Predict Word Reading Time

It has been claimed that larger semantic distance between thewords of a sentence, as quantified by a distributional seman-tics model, increases both N400 size and word-reading time.The current study shows that the reading-time effect disap-pears when word surprisal is factored out, suggesting that theearlier findings were caused by a confound between semanticdistance and surprisal. This absence of a behavioural effectof semantic distance (in the presence of a strong neurophysi-ological effect) may be due to methodological differences be-tween eye-tracking and EEG experiments, but it can also beinterpreted as evidence that eye movements are optimized forreading efficiency.

Is Conflict Detection in Reasoning Domain General?

A great deal of reasoning research indicates that individualsare often biased by intuitive heuristics. However,contemporary results indicate that individuals seem sensitiveto their biases; they seem to detect conflict with reasoningnorms. One of the key remaining questions is whether thisconflict sensitivity is domain general. To address thisquestion, we administered a battery of five classical reasoningtasks to a large sample of subjects and assessed their conflictdetection efficiency on each task by measuring their responseconfidence. Results indicate that conflict detection is, in mostsenses, not domain general, though there are compellingexceptions.

Sequential effects in prediction

We studied a simple binary prediction task and discovered that,when making predictions, humans display sequential effectssimilar to those in reaction time. Moreover, we found that thereare considerable individual differences in sequential effects inprediction, again similarly to reaction time studies. We discussour results in light of the view that sequential effects are thetrace of an attempt at detecting a pattern in the sequence, aswell as the possible influence of randomness perception in ourresults. We conclude that the same pattern detection mech-anism is likely to underlie sequential effects in reaction timeand prediction.

Empirical tests of large-scale collaborative recall

Much of our knowledge is transmitted socially rather thanthrough firsthand experience. Even our memories depend onrecollections of those around us. Surprisingly, when people re-call memories with others, they do not reach the potential num-ber of items they could have recalled alone. This phenomenonis called collaborative inhibition. Recently, Luhmann and Ra-jaram (2015) analyzed the dynamics of collaborative inhibitionat scale with an agent-based model, extrapolating from previ-ous small-scale laboratory experiments. We tested their modelagainst human data collected in a large-scale experiment andfound that participants demonstrate non-monotonicities not ev-ident in these predictions. We next analyzed memory transmis-sion beyond directly interacting agents by placing agents intonetworks. Contrary to model predictions, we observed highsimilarity only within directly interacting pairs. By comparingbehavior to model predictions in large-scale experiments, wereveal unexpected results that motivate future work in eluci-dating the algorithms underlying collaborative memory.

Faulty Towers: A hypothetical simulation model of physical support

In this paper we introduce the hypothetical simulation model(HSM) of physical support. The HSM predicts that peoplejudge physical support by mentally simulating what wouldhappen if the object of interest were removed. Two experi-ments test the model by asking participants to evaluate the ex-tent to which one brick in a tower is responsible for the restof the bricks staying on a table. The results of both experi-ments show a very close correspondence between hypotheticalsimulations and responsibility judgments. We compare threeversions of the HSM which differ in how they model people’suncertainty about what would happen. Participants’ selectionsof which bricks would fall are best explained by assumingthat hypothetical interventions only lead to local changes whileleaving the rest of the scene unchanged.

Gaze Shifts between Text and Illustrations are Negatively Related to ReadingFluency in Beginning Readers

Learning to read is often considered the most important skill taughtin school because reading is a gateway to other learning. Manychildren struggle to acquire this fundamental skill. Suboptimaldesign of books for beginning readers may contribute to thedifficulties children experience as close proximity between textand illustrations could promote attentional competition hamperingliteracy skills. The present work utilized eye-tracking technologyto examine how beginning readers allocate attention and whetherthese patterns are related to fluency (Experiment 1) andcomprehension (Experiment 2). Results suggest when readingbooks in which text and illustrations are in close proximity,children frequently shift attention away from the text. This patternof attention was negatively associated with fluency, but notassociated with comprehension. This line of research aims toprovide theoretical insights about design principles for readingmaterials that can be employed to optimize instructional materialsand promote literacy development in young children.

A Cognitive Model of Strategic Deliberation and Decision Making

We study game theoretic decision making using a bidirectional evidence accumulation model. Our model represents both preferences for the strategies available to the decision maker, as well as beliefs regarding the opponent’s choices. Through sequential sampling and accumulation, the model is able to intelligently reason through two-player strategic games, while also generating specific violations of Nash equilibrium typically observed in these games. The main ingredients of accumulator models, stochastic sampling and dynamic accumulation, play a critical role in explaining these behavioral patterns as well as generating novel predictions.

How Can Memory-Augmented Neural Networks Pass a False-Belief Task?

A question-answering system needs to be able to reason aboutunobserved causes in order to answer questions of the sort thatpeople face in everyday conversations. Recent neural networkmodels that incorporate explicit memory and attention mecha-nisms have taken steps towards this capability. However, thesemodels have not been tested in scenarios for which reasoningabout the unobservable mental states of other agents is nec-essary to answer a question. We propose a new set of tasksinspired by the well-known false-belief test to examine howa recent question-answering model performs in situations thatrequire reasoning about latent mental states. We find that themodel is only successful when the training and test data bearsubstantial similarity, as it memorizes how to answer specificquestions and cannot reason about the causal relationship be-tween actions and latent mental states. We introduce an ex-tension to the model that explicitly simulates the mental rep-resentations of different participants in a reasoning task, andshow that this capacity increases the model’s performance onour theory of mind test.

Evidence for a facilitatory effect of multi-word units on child word learning

Previous studies have suggested that children possess cognitiverepresentations of multi-word units (MWUs) and that MWUscan facilitate the acquisition of smaller units contained withinthem. We propose that the formation of MWU representationsprecedes and facilitates the formation of single-word represen-tations in children. Using different computational methods,we extract MWUs from two large corpora of English child-directed speech. In subsequent regression analyses, we use ageof first production of individual words as the dependent and thenumber of MWUs within which each word appears as an in-dependent variable. We find that early-learned words appearwithin many MWUs – an effect which is neither reducible tofrequency or other common co-variates, nor to the number ofcontext words contained in the MWUs. Our findings supportaccounts wherein children acquire linguistic patterns of vary-ing sizes, moving gradually from the discovery of MWUs tothe acquisition of small-grained linguistic representations.

The nature of quantities influences the representation of arithmetic problems: evidence from drawings and solving procedures in children and adults

When solving arithmetic problems, semantic factors influence the representations built (Gamo, Sander & Richard, 2010). In order to specify such interpretative processes, we created structurally isomorphic word problems that could be solved with two distinct algorithms. We tested whether a distinction between cardinal and ordinal quantities would lead solvers, due to their daily-life knowledge, to build different representations, influencing their strategies as well as the nature of their drawings. We compared 5th grade children and adults in order to assess the validity of this hypothesis with participants of varying arithmetic proficiency. The results confirmed that the distinction between cardinal and ordinal situations led to different solving strategies and to different drawings among both age groups. This study supports the ontological distinction of cardinal versus ordinal quantities and calls for the consideration of the role of daily-life semantics when accounting for arithmetic problem solving processes.

Does Mandarin Spatial Metaphor for Time Influence Chinese Deaf Signers’ Spatio-Temporal Reasoning?

In Mandarin Chinese, the space-time word “前/qian” is used to express both the spatial concept of front/forward and the temporal concept of early/before (e.g., “前天/qian- tian”, literally front day, meaning the day before yesterday). This is consistent with the fact that Mandarin speakers can gesture to the front of the body to refer to a past event, and more generally can have past-in-front space-time mappings. In Chinese Sign Languages, however, the spatial front/forward and the temporal early/before are signed differently as the sign for spatial front is only used for the spatial concept of forward, and the sign for before/past is directed to the back. In this study we investigate whether the Mandarin sagittal spatial metaphors for time influence Chinese deaf signers’ spatio-temporal reasoning. In two experiments, we found that Chinese deaf signers with higher Mandarin proficiency were more likely to interpret the Mandarin word “前/qian” as the temporal conception of past (Study 1), and to perform past-in-front space-time mappings (Study 2) as opposed to signers with lower Mandarin proficiency. The findings of the study not only provide within-culture evidence for the influence of language on thought, but also demonstrate that even cross- modal space-time metaphors can have an impact on deaf- signers’ spatio-temporal reasoning.

Language and Spatial Memory in Japanese and English

Demonstratives are among the most frequent words in all languages, but demonstrative systems vary considerablybetween languages. In two experiments, we tested demonstrative use and the influence of demonstratives on spatial memory inJapanese and English – languages with purportedly very different demonstrative systems. Participants engaged in a ‘memorygame’, tapping their use of demonstratives to describe objects located on a sagittal plane (Experiment 1) and the influenceof demonstratives on memory for object location (Experiment 2). In addition to distance from speaker, the experiments alsomanipulated the position of a conspecific (next to or opposite participants). Distance and position of conspecific both affecteddemonstrative choice and memory in Japanese, with similar effects in English even though English does not explicitly encodethe position of a conspecific. We discuss possible universals underlying demonstrative systems and the influence of languageon memory.

A Computational Model for the Dynamical Learning of Event Taxonomies

We present a computational model that can learn event tax-onomies online from the continuous sensorimotor informationflow perceived by an agent while interacting with its environ-ment. Our model implements two fundamental learning bi-ases. First, it learns probabilistic event models as temporal sen-sorimotor forward models and event transition models, whichpredict event model transitions given particular perceptual cir-cumstances. Second, learning is based on the principle of min-imizing free energy, which is further biased towards the detec-tion of free energy transients. As a result, the algorithm formsconceptual structures that encode events and event boundaries.We show that event taxonomies can emerge when the algo-rithm is run on multiple levels of precision. Moreover, weshow that generally any type of forward model can be used,as long as it learns sufficiently fast. Finally, we show that thedeveloped structures can be used to hierarchically plan goal-directed behavior by means of active inference.

Reverse-engineering the process:Adults’ and preschoolers’ ability to infer the difficulty of novel tasks

The ability to reason about the difficulty of novel tasks is criti-cal for many real-world decisions. To decide whether to tacklea task or how to divide labor across people, we must estimatethe difficulty of the goal in the absence of prior experience.Here we examine adults’ and preschoolers’ inferences aboutthe difficulty of simple block-building tasks. Exp.1 first estab-lished that building time is a useful proxy for difficulty. Exp.2asked participants to view the initial and final states of variousblock-building tasks and judge their relative difficulty. Whileadults were near-ceiling on all trials, children showed varyinglevels of performance depending on the nature of the dimen-sions that varied across structures. Exp. 3 replicated the pat-tern. These results suggest that children can reverse-engineerthe process of goal-directed actions to infer the relative diffi-culty of novel tasks, although their ability to incorporate morenuanced factors may continue to develop.

Categorization, Information Selection and Stimulus Uncertainty

Although a common assumption in models of perceptual dis-crimination, most models of categorization do not explicitlyaccount for uncertainty in stimulus measurement. Such un-certainty may arise from inherent perceptual noise or externalmeasurement noise (e.g., a medical test that gives variable re-sults). In this paper we explore how people decide to gatherinformation from various stimulus properties when each sam-ple or measurement is noisy. The participant’s goal is to cor-rectly classify the given item. Across two experiments we findsupport for the idea that people take category structure intoaccount when selecting information for a classification deci-sion. In addition, we find some evidence that people are alsosensitive to their own perceptual uncertainty when selectinginformation.

Perceptual contrast and response assimilation in sequential categorization without feedback

Sequential categorization of perceptual stimuli typically shows contrast from one trial to the next. Using familiar categories of animals and faces, contrast effects were dissociated from assimilation effects. Two independent main effects were observed: contrast to the preceding stimulus, and assimilation to the previous response. It is argued that contrast and assimilation may reflect different processes in categorization.

Radical Embodied Cognition, Affordances, and the (Hard) Problemof Consciousness

Tony Chemero advances the radical thesis thatcognition and consciousness is actually the samething. He draws this conclusion from his understandingof cognition as an extended process. I question thisconclusion because this view expands cognition beyondbeing the sort of natural kind to which one can tiephenomenal experience. Moreover, because cognition hasbeen radically inflated, despite Chemero’s claim to thecontrary, embodied cognition does not solve any of thehard problems associated with consciousness.

Convention-formation in iterated reference games

What cognitive mechanisms support the emergence of linguis-tic conventions from repeated interaction? We present re-sults from a large-scale, multi-player replication of the clas-sic tangrams task, focusing on three foundational propertiesof conventions: arbitrariness, stability, and reduction of ut-terance length over time. These results motivate a theory ofconvention-formation where agents, though initially uncertainabout word meanings in context, assume others are using lan-guage with such knowledge. Thus, agents may learn aboutmeanings by reasoning about a knowledgeable, informativepartner; if all agents engage in such a process, they success-fully coordinate their beliefs, giving rise to a conventionalcommunication system. We formalize this theory in a compu-tational model of language understanding as social inferenceand demonstrate that it produces all three properties in a sim-plified domain.

Sampling frames, Bayesian inference and inductive reasoning

We outline and test a Bayesian model of the effects of evidencesampling on property induction. Our model assumes thatpeople are sensitive to the effects of different sampling framesapplied to sampled evidence. Two studies tested the model bycomparing property generalization following exposure tosamples selected because they belong to the same taxonomiccategory or because they share a salient property. Both studiesfound that category-based sampling led to broadergeneralization than property-based sampling. In line withmodel predictions, these differences were attenuated when amixture of positive and negative evidence was presented(Experiment 1) and when category-property relations wereprobabilistic rather than deterministic (Experiment 2).

The adaptive evolution of early human symbolic behavior

Dating back as far as 100 ka, the Blombos ochre and the Diepkloof ostrich egg engravings are considered among theearliest fossilized evidence of human symbolic behavior. Of special interest to this study is the temporal trajectory spanningmore than 30 thousand years from earlier simpler parallel line patterns to later complex cross-hatchings suggesting adaptivecompositional development. Through a series of three psychophysical experiments we test the hypotheses that the line engrav-ings at each site evolved to become 1) more salient to the human perceptual system, 2) more discriminable from each other, and3) increasingly associated with symbolic intent. Our findings suggest that just as instrumental tools have been found to undergocumulative refinements in adaptation to their function, the ochre and egg shell engravings evolved adaptively to become morefit for their cognitive function as signs.

Children’s social referencing reflects sensitivity to graded uncertainty

The ability to monitor epistemic uncertainty is critical for self-directed learning. However, we still know little about youngchildren’s ability to detect uncertainty in their mental repre-sentations. Here we asked whether a spontaneous informationgathering behavior – social referencing – is driven by uncer-tainty during early childhood. Children ages 2-5 completed aword-learning task in which they were presented with one ortwo objects, heard a label, and were asked to put the labeledobject in a bucket. Referential ambiguity was manipulatedthrough the number of objects present and their familiarity. InExperiment 1, when there were two novel objects and a novellabel, the referent was ambiguous; when there were two famil-iar objects, or only one novel or familiar object, the referentwas known or could be inferred. In Experiment 2, there wereeither two novel objects, two familiar objects, or one familiarand one novel object; in the latter case the referent could be in-ferred by excluding the familiar object. To further manipulatethe availability of referential cues, the experimenter gazed ateither the target or the center of the table while labeling the ob-ject. In both experiments, children looked at the experimentermore often while making their response when the referent wasambiguous. In Experiment 2, children also looked at the ex-perimenter more when there was one familiar and one novelobject, but only when the experimenter’s gaze during label-ing was uninformative. These results suggest that children’ssocial referencing is a sensitive index of graded epistemic un-certainty.

Investigating the Explore/Exploit Trade-off in Adult Causal Inferences

We explore how adults learn counterintuitive causal relation-ships, and whether they discover hypotheses by revising theirbeliefs incrementally. We examined how adults learned a noveland unusual causal rule when presented with data that initiallyappeared to conform to a simpler, more salient rule. Adultswatched a video of several blocks placed sequentially on ablicket detector, and were then asked to determine the under-lying causal structure. In the near condition the true rule wascomplex, but could be found by making incremental improve-ments to the simple and salient initial hypothesis. The distantcondition was governed by a simpler rule, but to adopt that ruleparticipants had to set aside their initial beliefs, rather thanrevising them incrementally. Adults performed better in thenear condition, despite this rule being more complex, provid-ing some of the first evidence for an explore-exploit trade-offin inference, analogous to the trade-off in active learning.

Modeling transfer of high-order uncertain information

Complex uncertainty expressions such as probably likely andcertainly possible naturally occur in everyday conversations.However, they received much less attention in the literaturethan simple ones. We propose a probabilistic model of the useand interpretation of complex uncertainty expressions based onthe assumption that their predominant function is to communi-cate factual information about the world, and that further layersof uncertainty are pragmatically inferred. We collected empir-ical data on the use and interpretation of these expressions anduse it for detailed model criticism.

A Priming Model of Category-based Feature Inference

Categorization has a large impact on how people perceive theworld, especially when used to make inferences about uncer-tain features of new objects. While making these inferences,people tend to draw information from only one possible cate-gorization of a new object; in addition, people are sensitive topre-existing correlations between features. Here, we explainthese trends of feature inference using a priming-based cogni-tive process model, and show that our model is distinguishedin that it can explain not only these two main trends, but alsocases where people seem to reverse the first trend and base in-ferences on information from multiple categories.

Quantifying the impact of active choice in word learning

Past theoretical studies on word learning have offeredsimple sampling models as a means of explaining realword learning, with a particular goal of addressing thespeed of word learning: people learn tens of thousandsof words within their first 18 years. The present studyrevisits past theoretical claims by considering a more re-alistic word frequency distribution in which a large num-ber of words are sampled with extremely small probabil-ities (e.g., according to Zipf’s law). Our new mathemati-cal analysis of a recently-proposed simple learning modelsuggests that the model is unable to account for wordlearning in feasible time when the distribution of wordfrequency is Zipfian (i.e., power-law distributed). Toameliorate the difficulty of learning real-world word fre-quency distributions, we consider a type of active, self-directed learning in which the learner can influence theconstruction of contexts from which they learn words.We show that active learners who choose optimal learn-ing situations can learn words hundreds of times fasterthan passive learners faced with randomly-sampled situ-ations. Thus, in agreement with past empirical studies,we find theoretical support for the idea that statisticalstructure in real-world situations–potentially structuredfor learning by both a self-directed learner, and by abeneficent teacher–is a potential remedy for the patho-logical case of learning words with Zipf-distributed fre-quency.

Reference Systems in Spatial Memory for Vertical Locations

Three experiments investigated the frame of reference used in memory to represent vertical spatial layouts perceiv-able from a single viewpoint. We tested for the selection of three different reference systems: the body orientation, the visualvertical of the surrounding room, and the direction of gravity. Participants learned and retrieved differently colored objects ona vertical board with body and room orientations varying in relation to gravity and each other systematically. Across all threeexperiments participants were quicker or more accurate in memory recall when they saw the vertical spatial layout in the sameorientation in relation to their body vertical as during learning, irrespective of the direction of gravity or visual room upright.These results indicate that spatial long-term memories for small-scale vertical relations are mainly defined in an egocentricreference system with respect to the body vertical despite the availability of alternative highly salient allocentric referencedirections.

Teaching by Intervention: Working Backwards,Undoing Mistakes, or Correcting Mistakes?

When teaching, people often intentionally intervene on alearner while it is acting. For instance, a dog owner mightmove the dog so it eats out of the right bowl, or a coach mightintervene while a tennis player is practicing to teach a skill.How do people teach by intervention? And how do thesestrategies interact with learning mechanisms? Here, we ex-amine one global and two local strategies: working backwardsfrom the end-goal of a task (backwards chaining), placing alearner in a previous state when an incorrect action was taken(undoing), or placing a learner in the state they would be in ifthey had taken the correct action (correcting). Depending onhow the learner interprets an intervention, different teachingstrategies result in better learning. We also examine how peo-ple teach by intervention in an interactive experiment and finda bias for using local strategies like undoing.

Computational and behavioral investigations of the SOB-CS removal mechanismin working memory

SOB-CS is an interference-based computational model ofworking memory that explains findings from simple and com-plex span experiments. According to the model’s mechanismof interference by superposition, high similarity between mem-ory items and subsequently processed distractors is beneficialbecause the more a distractor is similar to an item, the morethey share similar units, leading to less distortion of the mem-ory item. When time allows, SOB-CS removes interfering dis-tractors from memory by unbinding them from their context.The combination of these two mechanisms leads to the predic-tion that when free time is long enough to remove the distrac-tors entirely, similarity between items and distractors shouldno longer be beneficial to memory performance. The aim ofthe present study was to test this prediction. Adult participantsperformed a complex-span task in which the free time follow-ing each distractor and the similarity between items and dis-tractors were varied. As predicted by the model, we observeda positive effect of the similarity between items and distrac-tors, and a negative effect of pace on the mean working mem-ory performance. However, we did not observe the predictedinteraction. An analysis of the errors produced during recallshowed that longer free time reduced the tendency of distrac-tors to intrude in recall much less than the model predicted.The SOB-CS model accounted well for the data after a sub-stantial reduction of the removal-rate parameter.

Spatial Cognition and Science: The Role of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Spatial Skillsfrom Seven to Eleven Years

The current study investigated the relationship betweenchildren’s spatial ability and their scientific knowledge, skills andunderstanding. Children aged 7-11 years (N=123) completed abattery of five spatial tasks, based on a model of spatial ability inwhich skills fall along two dimensions: intrinsic-extrinsic; static-dynamic. Participants also answered science questions fromstandardised assessments, grouped into conceptual topic areas.Spatial scaling (extrinsic static spatial ability) and mental folding(intrinsic dynamic spatial ability) each emerged as predictors oftotal science scores, with mental folding accounting for morevariance than spatial scaling. Mental folding predicted bothphysics and biology scores, whereas spatial scaling accounted foradditional variance only in biology scores. The embeddedfigures task (intrinsic static spatial ability) predicted chemistryscores. The pattern was consistent across the age range. Thesefindings provide novel evidence for the differential role ofdistinct aspects of spatial ability in relation to children’s scienceperformance.

Moving together: in the body or the mind?

When people move together, as they dance, march or flirt, itincreases affiliation between them. But what about ‘movingtogether’ produces affiliation: the movements themselves, orthe social context of moving ‘together’? We instructed pairs ofparticipants to listen to music and move their arms or legsaccording to shapes appearing on screen. They either carriedout the same movements, or when one moved their arms theother moved their legs. They either saw shapes on one laptop,or each had their own laptop. Surprisingly, participants did notlike each other more if they carried out the same movements,but affiliation did increase if they danced looking at the samescreen. Rather than their movements, instructions, intentionsor perceptual experiences, here it is the social context of theactions that produces affiliation, a surprising finding that is noteasily accounted for by the dominant theories of mimicry andbehavioural synchrony.

Modeling Sources of Uncertainty in Spoken Word Learning

In order to successfully learn the meaning of words such asbin or pin, language learners must not only perceive relevantdifferences in the speech signal but also learn mappings fromwords to referents. Prior work in native (Stager & Werker,1997) and second (Pajak, Creel, & Levy, 2016) language ac-quisition has found that the ability to perceptually discrimi-nate between words does not guarantee successful word learn-ing. Learners fail to utilize knowledge that they can otherwiseuse in speech perception. To explore possible mechanisms ac-counting for this phenomenon, we developed a probabilisticmodel that infers both a word’s phonetic form and its asso-ciated referent. By analyzing different versions of the modelfitted to experimental results from Pajak et al. (2016), we ar-gue that a mechanism for spoken word learning needs to incor-porate both perceptual uncertainty as well as additional, task-specific sources of uncertainty.

Knowledge of Cross-Linguistic Semantic Diversity Reduces Essentialist Beliefsabout Categories

The words of different languages partition the world in strikingly different ways. Yet many people are unaware ofsuch differences, believing that some of the words of their native language pick out discrete categories based in nature. Weinvestigated whether knowledge of cross-linguistic semantic diversity—putatively inherent to bilingualism—can reduce suchessentialist beliefs. In three experiments, we found (a) that bilinguals were less likely than monolinguals to judge membershipfor animal categories in essentialist terms, (b) that explicit exposure to cross-linguistic semantic diversity, independent ofbilingualism, yielded similar effects, and (c) that this manipulation reduced essentialist beliefs about social categories as well.Together, our findings suggest that learning about how languages differ in their semantic systems—a form of metalinguisticknowledge—can lead people to think about categories more flexibly. Implications for research on language and thought, andfor ameliorating the negative consequences of social essentialism, are discussed.

Decisions based on verbal probabilities:Decision bias or decision by belief sampling?

We examined decisions based on verbal probability phrases,such as "small chance," "likely," or "doubtful" (we call thesephrases verbal probabilities). Verbal probabilities havecommunicative functions called directionality and can becategorized into positive (e.g., "likely" or "probable") ornegative (e.g., “unlikely,” “doubtful”) phrases in terms oftheir directionality. Previous studies have shown that thedirectionality of phrases affects decisions. Although suchdecisions seem biased, we argue that they are not. Wehypothesize that since a speaker has the option to choose thedirectionality used during communication, the selecteddirectionality becomes relevant information to a decisionmaker, and is taken into account in making decisions. Wemodeled these processes using the Decision by BeliefSampling (DbBS) model. We found that the observed datacould be well explained by our hypothesis, and that the DbBSmodel could be one of the best potential models for decisionsbased on verbal probability information.

Semantic diversity, frequency and learning to read: A mini-mega study withchildren

Children who read more tend to be better readers than children who read less. Reading exposure captures not onlythe number of times words are experienced but also the breadth of the contexts words appear in. Using a large children’s corpusof written language, we quantified the former as word frequency and the latter as Semantic Diversity (SemD) (Hoffman etal., 2013). SemD was indexed using Latent Semantic Analysis to calculate the degree of semantic dissimilarity between thecontexts in which each appeared. We selected 300 words that varied in SemD for a visual lexical decision and naming task with9-year-old children (N=114). Results showed that both frequency and SemD were associated with performance, independentlyaccounting for variation in speed and accuracy. Those words high in frequency and high in SemD were read more efficiently.These findings show that factors beyond frequency are important in determining children’s word reading.

Testing Statistical Learning Implicitly:A Novel Chunk-based Measure of Statistical Learning

Attempts to connect individual differences in statisticallearning with broader aspects of cognition have receivedconsiderable attention, but have yielded mixed results. Apossible explanation is that statistical learning is typicallytested using the two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) task.As a meta-cognitive task relying on explicit familiarityjudgments, 2AFC may not accurately capture implicitlyformed statistical computations. In this paper, we adapt theclassic serial-recall memory paradigm to implicitly teststatistical learning in a statistically-induced chunking recall(SICR) task. We hypothesized that artificial languageexposure would lead subjects to chunk recurring statisticalpatterns, facilitating recall of words from the input.Experiment 1 demonstrates that SICR offers more fine-grained insights into individual differences in statisticallearning than 2AFC. Experiment 2 shows that SICR hashigher test-retest reliability than that reported for 2AFC. Thus,SICR offers a more sensitive measure of individualdifferences, suggesting that basic chunking abilities mayexplain statistical learning.

Minimal covariation data support future one-shot inferences about unobservableproperties of novel agents

When we reason about others’ behavior, there are often manyequally-plausible explanations. If Bob climbs a tree to get anapple, we may be unsure if Bob found climbing difficult butreally wanted an apple; if he found climbing easy and was notparticularly excited about the apple; or if he found climbingintrinsically fun and just got the apple because it was conve-nient. Past research suggests that we solve this problem byobtaining repeated observations about the agent and about theworld. Here we argue that, beyond allowing us to sharpen ourinferences about agents and the world, covariation data alsoenables us to do one-shot inferences about novel agents. Weshow that given minimal covariation data, people can infer ob-jective and subjective properties of a new agent from a singleevent. We show that a model that assumes that agents maxi-mize utilities matches participant judgments with quantitativeprecision.

The Semantics and Pragmatics of Logical Connectives: Adults’ and Children’sInterpretations of And and Or in a Guessing Game

The development of the ubiquitous logical connectives and andor provides a window into the role of semantics and pragmat-ics in children’s linguistic development. Previous research hassuggested that adults and children might differ in their interpre-tation of or in two ways. First, unlike adults, children mightinterpret or as logical conjunction, akin to and. Second, chil-dren might interpret or as inclusive disjunction while adultsinterpret it as exclusive. We report experimental studies thatprobe interpretations of and and or in adults and children us-ing truth value judgements as well as children’s spontaneouslinguistic feedback. Both truth judgements and linguistic feed-back showed that four-year-olds do not interpret or as and.While children’s truth judgments suggested that they did notderive exclusivity implicatures, however, their corrective feed-back showed signs of sensitivity to the implicature, suggestingthat the truth value judgement task could have underestimatedchildren’s pragmatic competence. More generally, four-year-olds’ interpretation of logical connectives may not be as differ-ent from adults as previously supposed.

Maintaining Credibility When Communicating Uncertainty: The Role of Communication Format

Research into risk communication has commonly highlighted the disparity between the meaning intended by the communicator and what is understood by the recipient. Such miscommunications will have implications for perceived trust and expertise of the communicator, but it is not known whether this differs according to the communication format. We examined the effect of using verbal, numerical and mixed communication formats on perceptions of credibility and correctness, as well as whether they influenced a decision to evacuate, both before and after an ‘erroneous’ prediction (i.e. an ‘unlikely’ event occurs, or a ‘likely’ event does not occur). We observed no effect of communication format on any of the measures pre-outcome, but found the numerical format was perceived as less incorrect, as well as more credible than the other formats after an ‘erroneous’ prediction, but only when low probability expressions were used. Our findings suggest numbers should be used in consequential risk communications.

A modeling link between cognitive and biological homeostasis

The problem of stability has long been a limiting factor in de-veloping neural networks that can grow in size and complex-ity. Outside of particular, narrow parameter ranges, changesin activity can easily result in total loss of control. Humancognition must have reliable means of acting to stay withinthe stable ranges of sensitivity and activation. Learning is onesuch mechanism, and population dynamics are another. Here,we focus on another, often overlooked stability mechanism:cellular homeostasis through metabolism dynamics. We ran avisual change detection experiment designed to strain networkstability while minimizing any learnable patterns. We fit thedata using models with and without cellular energy levels as afactor, finding that the model influenced by its past history ofenergy use was a closer fit to the human data.

Viewers’ Sensitivity to Abstract Event Structure

Bounded and unbounded events differ in whether they include an inherent endpoint (Bach, 1986). Even though this distinction can be important for the way events are identified and processed, the literature on event cognition has not focused on such abstract aspects of event structure. In the present study, we asked whether viewers are sensitive to the distinction between bounded and unbounded events in a category learning task. Our results show that people were more successful in forming the category of bounded events than that of unbounded events. We discuss implications of this finding for event cognition.

Promoting Children’s Relational Understanding of Equivalence

Deep understanding of mathematical equivalence is criticalfor later mathematical understandings. However, researchstudies and national test results have repeatedly demonstratedthat many students fail to develop adequate understanding ofequivalence. Recent work from McNeil and colleaguesproposes that this failure is partly due to the format oftraditional instruction and practice with highly similarproblems. Specifically, the change-resistance account(McNeil & Alibali, 2005) proposes that students struggle withequivalence because they have developed overgeneralized“rules” that affect how they process and approach mathproblems, (e.g., the operators are always on the left side, theequal sign means to “do something” or “give the answer”)and fail to see equations having two separate sides that arebeing related to one another. Extensive practice withproblems in a similar format (e.g., those that present allarithmetic operations on the left side of the equal sign)encourages students to develop ineffective mental models ofproblem types. We replicate and extend prior work that bringscognitive science research to the classroom. Our findingsindicate that applying research-based design principles toarithmetic practice improves student understanding ofmathematical equivalence enough to support transfer to novelproblem types.

Opponent Uses of Simplicity and Complexity in Causal Explanation

People often prefer simpler explanations because they havehigher prior probability. However, simpler explanations arenot always normatively superior because they often do notfit the data as well as complex explanations. How dopeople negotiate this trade-off between prior probability(favoring simplicity) and goodness-of-fit (favoringcomplexity)? Here, we argue that people use opponentheuristics—relying on simplicity as a cue to priorprobability but complexity as a cue to goodness-of-fit(Study 1). We also examine factors that lead one or theother heuristic to predominate in a given context. Study 2finds that people have a stronger simplicity preference indeterministic rather than stochastic contexts, while Study 3finds that people have a stronger simplicity preference forphysical rather than social causal systems. Together, weargue that these cues and contextual moderators act aspowerful constraints that help to specify otherwise ill-defined hypothesis comparison problems.

Principles Used to Evaluate Mathematical Explanations

Mathematics is critical for making sense of the world. Yet,little is known about how people evaluate mathematicalexplanations. Here, we use an explanatory reasoning taskto investigate the intuitive structure of mathematics. Weshow that people evaluate arithmetic explanations bybuilding mental proofs over the conceptual structure ofintuitive arithmetic, evaluating those proofs using criteriasimilar to those of professional mathematicians.Specifically, we find that people prefer explanationsconsistent with the conceptual order of the operations(“9÷3=3 because 3 ́3=9” rather than “3 ́3=9 because9÷3=3”), and corresponding to simpler proofs (“9÷3=3because 3 ́3=9” rather than “9÷3=3 because 3+3+3=9”).Implications for mathematics cognition and education arediscussed.

Statistical and Mechanistic Information in Evaluating Causal Claims

People use a variety of strategies for evaluating causalclaims, including mechanistic strategies (seeking a step-by-step explanation for how a cause would bring about itseffect) and statistical strategies (examining patterns of co-occurrence). Two studies examine factors leading one orthe other of these strategies to predominate. First, generalcausal claims (e.g., “Smoking causes cancer”) areevaluated predominantly using statistical evidence,whereas statistics is less preferred for specific claims (e.g.,“Smoking caused Jack’s cancer”). Second, social andbiological causal claims are evaluated primarily throughstatistical evidence, whereas statistical evidence is deemedless relevant for evaluating physical causal claims. Weargue for a pluralistic view of causal learning on which amultiplicity of causal concepts lead to distinct strategies forlearning about causation.

Touch Screen Text Entry as Cognitively Bounded Rationality

Typing on a smartphone is an everyday activity that involves various cognitive and behavioural processes. This papermodels touch screen text entry as cognitively bounded rationality. The model aims to maximise error-free text throughput, whilebeing constrained by its architecture and task environment. Empirical data are used to calibrate the model, which demonstratesadequate fit. The model is used to explore how strategic choices under given constraints affect text entry performance. Thepreliminary model presented here serves as a confirmation that touch screen text entry can be modelled as cognitively boundedrationality. Future extensions by integration into richer cognitive architectures are outlined.

Using Measurement Models to Understand Eyewitness Identification

Much research effort has been expended improving police lineup procedures used in collecting eyewitness identification evidence. Sequential presentation of lineup members, in contrast to simultaneous presentation, has been posited to increase witness accuracy, though analyses based in Signal Detection Theory (SDT) have challenged these claims. A possible way to clarify the effect of presentation format on witness accuracy is to develop SDT-based measurement models, which characterise decision performance in terms of psychologically-relevant parameters, particularly discrimin- ability and response bias. A model of the sequential lineup task was developed with a “first-above-criterion” decision rule, alongside a simultaneous model with a “maximum familiarity” decision rule. These models were fit to a corpus of data comparing simultaneous and sequential lineup performance. Results showed no difference in discriminability between the procedures and more conservative responding for the sequential lineup. Future work will examine criterion setting in the sequential lineup and model alternative decision rules.

A Biologically Constrained Model of Semantic Memory Search

The semantic fluency task has been used to understand the ef-fects of semantic relationships on human memory search. Avariety of computational models have been proposed that ex-plain human behavioral data, yet it remains unclear how mil-lions of spiking neurons work in unison to realize the cogni-tive processes involved in memory search. In this paper, wepresent a biologically constrained neural network model thatperforms the task in a fashion similar to humans. The modelreproduces experimentally observed response timing effects,as well as similarity trends within and across semantic cate-gories derived from responses. Three different sources of theassociation data have been tested by embedding associationsin neural connections, with free association norms providingthe best match.

Mechanisms of overharvesting in patch foraging

Serial stay-or-search problems are ubiquitous across manydomains, including employment, internet search, mate search,and animal foraging. For instance, in patch foragingproblems, animals must decide whether to stick with adepleting reward vs search for a new source. The optimalstrategy in patch foraging problems, described by theMarginal Value Theorem (MVT; Charnov, 1976), is to leavethe depleting patch when the local reward rate within a patchmatches the overall long-run reward rate. Many species ofanimals, ranging from birds to rodents, monkeys, andhumans, adhere to this policy in important respects, but tendto overharvest, or stick with the depleting resource too long.Here we attempt to determine the cognitive biases thatunderlie overharvesting in one of these species (the rat). Wecharacterized rat behavior in response to two basicmanipulations in patch foraging tasks: to travel time betweenpatches and depletion rate, and two novel manipulations tothe foraging environment: the size of reward and length ofdelays, and placement of delays (pre- vs. post-reward). Inresponse to the basic manipulations, rats qualitativelyfollowed predictions of MVT, but stayed in patches for longerthan is predicted. In the latter two manipulations, rats deviatedfrom predictions of MVT, exhibiting changes in behavior notpredicted by MVT. We formally tested whether four separatecognitive biases – subjective costs, decreasing marginal utilityfor reward discounting of future reward, and ignoring post-reward delays – could explain overharvesting in the formertwo manipulations and deviations from MVT in the latter two.All of the biases tested explained overharvesting behavior inthe former contexts, but only one bias – in which rats ignorepost-reward delays – also explained deviations from MVTdue to larger rewards with longer delays and due tointroduction of a pre-reward delay. Our results show thatmultiple biases can explain certain aspects of overharvestingbehavior, and, while foraging behavior may be the result ofthe use of multiple biases, inaccurate estimation of post-reward delays likely contributes to overharvesting.

Language-users choose short words in predictive contextsin an artificial language task

Zipf (1935) observed that word length is inversely proportionalto word frequency in the lexicon. He hypothesised that thiscross-linguistically universal feature was due to the Principleof Least Effort: language-users align form-meaning mappingsin such a way that the lexicon is optimally coded for efficientinformation transfer. However, word frequency is not the onlyreliable predictor of word length: Piantadosi, Tily, and Gib-son (2011) show that a word’s predictability in context is infact more strongly correlated with word length than word fre-quency. Here, we present an artificial language learning studyaimed at investigating the mechanisms that could give rise tosuch a distribution at the level of the lexicon. We find thatparticipants are more likely to use an ambiguous short form inpredictive contexts, and distinct long forms in surprising con-texts, only when they are subject to the competing pressures tocommunicate accurately and efficiently. These results supportthe hypothesis that language-users are driven by a least-effortprinciple to restructure their input in order to align word lengthwith information content, and this mechanism could thereforeexplain the global pattern observed at the level of the lexicon.

Preparatory Effects of Problem Posing on Learning from Instruction

A randomized-controlled study compared the preparatory effects of problem-posing on learning from subsequentinstruction. Students engaged in problem-posing either with solution generation (where they generated problems and solutionsto a novel situation) or problem-posing without solution generation (where they generated only problems) prior to learning anovel math concept. Problem-posing with solution generation prior to instruction resulted in significantly better conceptualknowledge, without any significant difference in procedural knowledge and transfer. These findings suggest that althoughsolution generation prior to instruction plays a critical role in the development of conceptual understanding, and generatingproblems can further enhance transfer.

Comparing Individual and Collaborative Problem Solving in EnvironmentalSearch

Collaborative spatial problem solving is an important yet not thoroughly examined task. Participants navigatedindividually and in dyads through virtual cities of varying complexity. They only saw the environment part visible from theircurrent location from a bird’s eye view map perspective. We recorded missed target locations, overall trajectory length andsearch time per person until self-indicating whole coverage. Our results show a general increase in missed locations, trajectorylength, and search time with the complexity of the environment. These increases differed due to individual and collaborativesearch. For complex, but not for simple environments individual participants navigated shorter distances, finished earlier, butalso missed more target locations than when searching the same environments in collaboration. These results indicate that incomplex environments collaborative search is less error prone than individual search, but takes longer. Such initial findings willconstrain future theorizing about collaborative spatial problem solving.

Iterated Teaching Can Optimise Language Functionality

Experimental studies of the cultural evolution of languagehave focused on how constraints on learning andcommunication drive emergence of linguistic structure. Yetlanguage is typically transmitted by experts who adjust theinput in ways that facilitates learning by novices, e.g. throughchild-directed speech. Using iterated language learning ofbinary auditory sequences, we explored how language changeis affected by experts’ intention to teach the language tonovices. Comparison between teaching chains and simpletransmission chains revealed that teaching was associatedwith a greater rate of innovation which led to emergence ofmore expressive languages consisting of shorter signals. Thisis the first study to show that during cultural transmission,teaching can modify, and potentially optimise, functionalcharacteristics of language.

Dynamic Effects of Conceptual Combination on Semantic Network Structure

The generative capacity of language entails an ability to flexibly combine concepts with each other. Conceptual combination can occur either by using an attribute of one concept to describe another (attributive combination) or by forming some relation between two concepts to create a new one (relational combination). Prior research has addressed whether common or distinct processes support these two putatively different types of combinations. We turn the question around and ask whether the consequences of these combination types on our conceptual system might differ, by comparing semantic memory networks before and after participants perform either attributive or relational conceptual combinations. We find a general effect on the semantic networks: the structure of network decreases after participants conceptually combine some of the concepts in the network. However, the relational combination manipulation has a greater effect. Furthermore, only the relational combination manipulation leads to an increase in the network’s connectivity.

The provenance of modal inference

People reason about possibilities routinely, and reasoners caninfer “modal” conclusions, i.e., conclusions that concern whatis possible or necessary, from premises that make no mentionof modality. For instance, given that Cullen was born in NewYork or Kentucky, it is intuitive to infer that it’s possible thatCullen was born in New York, and a recent set of studies onmodal reasoning bear out these intuitions (Hinterecker,Knauff, & Johnson-Laird, 2016). What explains the tendencyto make modal inferences? Conventional logic does not applyto modal reasoning, and so logicians invented manyalternative systems of modal logic to capture valid modalinferences. But, none of those systems can explain theinference above. We posit a novel theory based on the ideathat reasoners build mental models, i.e., iconic simulations ofpossibilities, when they reason about sentential connectivessuch as and, if, and or (Johnson-Laird, 2006). The theoryposits that reasoners represent a set of conjunctivepossibilities to capture the meanings of compound assertions.It is implemented in a new computational process model ofsentential reasoning that can draw modal conclusions fromnon-modal premises. We describe the theory andcomputational model, and show how its performance matchesreasoners’ inferences in two studies by Hinterecker et al.(2016). We conclude by discussing the model-based theory inlight of alternative accounts of reasoning.

Numerical and Non-numerical Magnitude Estimation

Despite a heated debate regarding a cognitive mechanism of magnitude representation, little has been done to di-rectly compare numerical and non-numerical estimation and provide a unified account of the two processes. In the current study,we examined estimation of numerical and non-numerical quantities on a continuum using various psychophysical functions.Inconsistent with the proportion reasoning and measurement skills accounts, estimates of both numerical and non-numericalquantities were better predicted by the logarithmic-linear model than by cyclic power models. Furthermore, individual dif-ferences in the degree of logarithmic compression was highly correlated over tasks, whereas bias measures from competingmodels did not show such associations. These findings suggest that estimation of both numerical and non-numerical magnitudeis processed via shared representation systems that are logarithmically or linearly constructed.

Variables Involved in Selective Sustained Attention Development: Advances inMeasurement

Selective sustained attention (SSA) is an importantcognitive process that enables everyday functioning and taskperformance by allowing us to: 1) choose components of ourenvironment to process at the exclusion of others and 2)maintain focus on those components over time. AlthoughSSA is known to undergo rapid and marked changes duringthe preschool and early primary school children years, therehas been a paucity of behavioral data on these years ofdevelopment due to a lack of child-appropriate testingparadigms. TrackIt is a paradigm that was recently developedto fill the previously existing measurement gap for SSA inthese years. In this study, we analyzed errors that children(aged 3-7) make when performing TrackIt, to betterunderstand what factors drive improvement in theirperformance over age. In addition, we manipulatedparameters within TrackIt to place varying levels of demandon children’s SSA, and measured behavioral performanceover age, with the goal of measuring and characterizingdevelopmental trends during these years. Since TrackIt is stilla recent paradigm, our results also help suggest appropriateparameter settings for calibrating the task to different agegroups.

Constructing Social Preferences From Anticipated Judgments:When Impartial Inequity is Fair and Why?

Successful and repeated cooperation requires fairly sharingthe spoils of joint endeavors. Fair distribution is often doneaccording to preferences for equitable outcomes even thoughstrictly equitable outcomes can lead to inefficient waste. In ad-dition to preferences about the outcome itself, decision makersare also sensitive to the attributions others might make aboutthem as a result of their choice. We develop a novel mathemat-ical model where decision makers turn their capacity to inferlatent desires and beliefs from the behavior of others (theory-of-mind) towards themselves, anticipating the judgments oth-ers will make about them. Using this model we can construct apreference to be seen as impartial and integrate it with prefer-ences for equitable and efficient outcomes. We test this modelin two studies where the anticipated attribution of impartialityis ambiguous: when one agent is more deserving than the otherand when unbiased procedures for distribution are made avail-able. This model explains both participants’ judgments aboutthe partiality of others and their hypothetical decisions. Ourmodel argues that people avoid inequity not only because theyfind it inherently undesirable, they also want to avoid beingjudged as partial.

Explaining Guides Learners Towards Perfect Patterns, Not Perfect Prediction

When learners explain to themselves as they encounter new in-formation, they recruit a suite of processes that influence sub-sequent learning. One consequence is that learners are morelikely to discover exceptionless rules that underlie what theyare trying to explain. Here we investigate what it is about ex-ceptionless rules that satisfies the demands of explanation. Areexceptions unwelcome because they lower predictive accuracy,or because they challenge some other explanatory ideal, suchas simplicity and breadth? To compare these alternatives, weintroduce a causally rich property explanation task in whichexceptions to a general rule are either arbitrary or predictable(i.e., exceptions share a common feature that supports a “ruleplus exception” structure). If predictive accuracy is sufficientto satisfy the demands of explanation, the introduction of a ruleplus exception that supports perfect prediction should blockthe discovery of a more subtle but exceptionless rule. Acrosstwo experiments, we find that effects of explanation go beyondattaining perfect prediction.

Selective Information Sampling and the In-Group Heterogeneity Effect

People often perceive their in-groups as more heterogeneousthan their out-groups. We propose an information samplingexplanation for this in-group heterogeneity effect. We analyzea model in which an agent forms beliefs and attitudes aboutsocial groups from her experience. Consistent with robust evi-dence from the social sciences, we assume that people are morelikely to interact again with in-group members than with out-group members. This implies that people obtain larger sam-ples of information about in-groups than about out-groups. Be-cause estimators of variability tend to be right-skewed, but lessso when sample size is large, sampled in-group variability willtend to be higher than sampled out-group variability. This im-plies that even agents that process information correctly – evenif they are naive intuitive statisticians – will be subject to thein-group heterogeneity effect. Our sampling mechanism com-plements existing explanations that rely on how informationabout in-group and out-group members is processed.

Exploring the decision dynamics of risky intertemporal choice

Previous research on the effects of probability and delay ondecision-making has focused on examining each dimensionseparately, and hence little is known about when these dimen-sions are combined into a single choice option. Importantly,we know little about the psychological processes underlyingchoice behavior with rewards that are both delayed and proba-bilistic. Using a process-tracing experimental design, we mon-itored information acquisition patterns and processing strate-gies. We found that probability and delay are processed se-quentially and evaluations of risky delayed prospects are de-pendent on the sequence of information acquisition. Amongchoice strategies, directly comparing the values of each dimen-sion (i.e., dimension-wise processing) appears to be most fa-vored by participants. Our results provide insights into the psy-chological plausibility of existing computational models andmake suggestions for the development of a process model forrisky intertemporal choice.

Consistent Probabilistic Simulation UnderlyingHuman Judgment in Substance Dynamics

A growing body of evidence supports the hypothesis that hu-mans infer future states of perceived physical situations bypropagating noisy representations forward in time using ratio-nal (approximate) physics. In the present study, we examinewhether humans are able to predict (1) the resting geometryof sand pouring from a funnel and (2) the dynamics of threesubstances—liquid, sand, and rigid balls—flowing past obsta-cles into two basins. Participants’ judgments in each experi-ment are consistent with simulation results from the intuitivesubstance engine (ISE) model, which employs a Material PointMethod (MPM) simulator with noisy inputs. The ISE outper-forms ground-truth physical models in each situation, as wellas two data-driven models. The results reported herein expandon previous work proposing human use of mental simulation inphysical reasoning and demonstrate human proficiency in pre-dicting the dynamics of sand, a substance that is less commonin daily life than liquid or rigid objects.

Synchronization Assessment for Collective Behavior

Team cognition can be defined as the ability that humans haveto coordinate with others through a complex environment.Sports offer exquisite examples of this dynamic interplayrequiring decision making and other perceptual-cognitiveskills to adjust individual decisions to the team self-organization and vice-versa. Considering players of a team asperiodic phase oscillators, synchrony analyses can be used tomodel the coordination of a team. Nonetheless, a mainlimitation of current models is that collective behavior iscontext independent. In other words, players of a team can behighly synchronized without this corresponding to ameaningful coordination dynamics relevant to the context ofthe game. Considering these issues, the aim of this study wasto develop a method of analysis sensitive to the context forevidence-based measures of team cognition.

More Siblings Means Lower Input Quality in Early Language Development

Previous research has suggested that first-born infants acquire words faster than their later-born peers (Berglund etal., 2005), but may have some disadvantages in other aspects of syntactic and socio-communicative development (e.g. Hoff,2006). Here we analyzed infants’ early lexical development alongside their caregiver input from 6-18 months, in relation to howmany siblings they have. We find that having more siblings (rather than being first- or later-born) has a gradient and negativerelationship with infants’ language development. This affect appears to be manifested in caregiver input: across three differentmeasures of input quality/quantity, disadvantages were found for infants with more siblings. Having a larger number of siblingsdiminished the quality of the input and led to slower overall lexical development. Implications for language development andlearning within dyadic and multi-member contexts are discussed.

Uncovering visual priors in spatial memory using serial reproduction

Visual memory can be understood as an inferential process thatcombines noisy information about the world with knowledgedrawn from experience. Biases can arise during encoding ofinformation from the outside world into internal representa-tions, or during retrieval. In this work, we use the methodof serial reproduction, in which information is passed along achain of participants who try to recreate what they observed.We apply this method to the study of visual perception in thecontext of spatial memory biases for the remembered positionof dots inside different geometric shapes. We present the re-sults of non-parametric kernel density estimation of the end re-sult of serial reproduction to model visual biases. We confirmprevious findings, and show that memory biases revealed withour method are often more intricate and complex than whathad previously been reported, suggesting that serial reproduc-tion can be effective for studying perceptual priors.

A core-affect model of decision making in simple and complex tasks

When it comes to decision making, the dominant viewsuggests that engaging in a detailed analytical thought processis more beneficial than deciding based on one’s feelings.However, there seems to be a tradeoff, as the complexity andamount of elements on which to base the decision increases,decisions based on affect seem to be more accurate thandecisions based on a thorough analytical process in specificcontexts. In those last cases, an affective modulation ofmemory may help to make better decisions in complex tasksthat exceed human’s limited cognitive capacities. Some dualprocess accounts, ‘‘deliberation-without-attention’’hypothesis (Dijksterhuis et al., 2006), oppose a cognitive (i.e.,conscious) route to an affective (i.e., unconscious) route.Since most dual process accounts suggest one type of processis better than the other, the interaction and integration ofaffective and more conscious analytical processes in decisionmaking have been understudied. To address this issue, wepropose an explanation of the dynamics and interaction ofcognitive (i.e., explicit) and affective (i.e., implicit) encodingand retrieval of elements in memory, using a unified theorybased on core affect (Russell, 2003), in the shape of acognitive model in the ACT-R cognitive architecture.

Generic and Universal Generalisations:Contextualising the ‘Generic Overgeneralisation’ Effect

In this study, we focused on the Generic Overgeneralisation(GOG) effect (Leslie, Khemlani, and Glucksberg 2011) andtested the relevance of context and an explanation based onquantifier domain restriction for the pattern of judgement dataobserved. Participants judged generic majority characteristicstatements like tigers have stripes or statements withuniversal quantifiers that have different sensitivity to context(‘all’, ‘all the’, ‘each’) preceded by one of three levels ofcontext: a) neutral, where the information in the context doesnot interact with the truth value of the critical statement, b)contradictory, where it presents an exception which shouldrule out a universally quantified statement, and c) supportive.Our results suggest that proponents of the generics-as-defaultview ruled out context prematurely and that in fact context isa viable alternative explanation for much of the so-calledGOG effect.

Children’s semantic and world knowledge overrides fictional information during anticipatory linguistic processing

Using real-time eye-movement measures, we asked how a fantastical discourse context competes with stored representations of semantic and world knowledge to influence children's and adults' moment-by-moment interpretation of a story. Seven-year- olds were less effective at bypassing stored semantic and world knowledge during real-time interpretation than adults. Nevertheless, an effect of discourse context on comprehension was still apparent.

The impact of descriptions and incentives on the simultaneous underweighting and overestimation of rare events

We replicate and extend work demonstrating that choice and probability estimation can be dissociated through the coexistence of contradictory reactions to rare events. In the context of experience-based risky choice, we find the simultaneous underweighting of rare events in choice and their overestimation in probability judgement. This tendency persisted in the presence of accurate descriptions of rare event occurrence (Experiment 1), but was attenuated by incentivizing accurate probability estimates (Experiment 2). The implications of these results for popular models of risky choice are briefly discussed.

An automatic method for discovering rational heuristics for risky choice

What is the optimal way to make a decision given that yourtime is limited and your cognitive resources are bounded? Toanswer this question, we formalized the bounded optimal de-cision process as the solution to a meta-level Markov deci-sion process whose actions are costly computations. We ap-proximated the optimal solution and evaluated its predictionsagainst human choice behavior in the Mouselab paradigm,which is widely used to study decision strategies. Our compu-tational method rediscovered well-known heuristic strategiesand the conditions under which they are used, as well as novelheuristics. A Mouselab experiment confirmed our model’smain predictions. These findings are a proof-of-concept thatoptimal cognitive strategies can be automatically derived as therational use of finite time and bounded cognitive resources.

Sequential Effects in the Garner Tasks

The distinction between integral and separable dimensions isof central importance to understanding how humans integrateinformation from multiple stimulus sources. One approach tocharacterizing stimulus integrality is through a set of speededcategorization tasks most closely associated with the work ofWendell Garner. These tasks demonstrate that integral dimen-sions result in marked speed up or slow down in respondingwhen there is correlated or irrelevant variation, respectively,compared with a baseline task. Little, Wang & Nosofsky(2016) recently found that the slow down or interference canbe largely explained by a reduction in the number of direct rep-etitions in a modified Garner filtering task. In this paper, weexamine a large sample of subjects tested on either separable orintegral dimensions to determine the extent of and individualdifferences in the overall and sequential effects in the standardGarner tasks.

Error-Based Learning: A Mechanism for Linking Verbs to Syntax

Children and adults are guided by verb-specific syntactic like-lihoods, or verb bias, in language comprehension and produc-tion. Recent reports showed that verb bias can be altered bynew linguistic experience. We investigated the mechanismsunderlying this verb bias learning or adaptation. Specifically,we asked whether verb bias learning, like abstract syntacticpriming, is driven by error-based implicit learning. We reportthree experiments in which we altered the biases of familiardative verbs in children’s and adults’ sentence production, viatraining trials that induced participants to produce each verbconsistently in either double-object or prepositional-object da-tive structures. Participants’ syntactic choices in later test trialsreflected the expected adaptation of verb bias to the trainingexperience. In addition, the magnitude of the training effectvaried with the likelihood of each sentence structure and withpre-existing verb bias: Unexpected verb-structure combina-tions resulted in larger training effects, suggesting the opera-tion of error-based implicit learning.

Repeated Interactions Can Lead to More Iconic Signals

Previous research has shown that repeated interactions cancause iconicity in signals to reduce. However, data from sev-eral recent studies has shown the opposite trend: an increase iniconicity as the result of repeated interactions. Here, we dis-cuss whether signals may become less or more iconic as a re-sult of the modality used to produce them. We review severalrecent experimental results before presenting new data frommulti-modal signals, where visual input creates audio feed-back. Our results show that the growth in iconicity presentin the audio information may come at a cost to iconicity inthe visual information. Our results have implications for howwe think about and measure iconicity in artificial signallingexperiments. Further, we discuss how iconicity in real worldspeech may stem from auditory, kinetic or visual information,but iconicity in these different modalities may conflict

What’s worth the effort: Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from thecosts of actions

Infants understand that people act in order to achieve their goals, but how can they tell what goals people findworthwhile? Here, we explore the thesis that human infants solve this problem by building a mental model of action planning,taking into account the costs of acting and the rewards actions bring. Consistent with this thesis, we found that 10-month-oldinfants, after viewing an agent approach two objects equally often, inferred that the agent preferred the object whose attainmentrequired a costlier action. Infants’ responses generalized across changes in perceptual variables that distinguished one actionfrom another (e.g. path length, angle of incline), suggesting that an abstract cost metric based on force or effort supported theirjudgments. These findings suggest that infants’ knowledge about agents may be expressed as a generative model for actionplanning, which can then be inverted to identify the probable hidden causes for observed actions.

Why Does Higher Working Memory Capacity Help You Learn?

Algorithms for approximate Bayesian inference, such asMonte Carlo methods, provide one source of models of howpeople may deal with uncertainty in spite of limited cognitiveresources. Here, we model learning as a process of sequentialsampling, or ‘particle filtering’, and suggest that an individ-ual’s working memory capacity (WMC) may be usefully mod-elled in terms of the number of samples, or ‘particles’, that areavailable for inference. The model qualitatively captures twodistinct effects reported recently, namely that individuals withhigher WMC are better able to (i) learn novel categories, and(ii) flexibly switch between different categorization strategies.

Social Network Limits Language Complexity

Natural languages vary widely in the degree to which they make use of hierarchical composition in their grammars,in particular, the degree to which syntactic versus morphologi- cal means of composition are utilized. Languages historicallyspoken in small communities develop much deeper levels of morphological embedding than those spoken by larger groups, anobservation confirmed by a statistical analysis of the World Atlas of Language Structures. However, beyond population alone,social networks change in topological structure as they grow, and it may be the pattern of connectivity rather than numberof speakers driving these differences. To examine mechanistically this connection between social and linguistic structure, wepropose an agent-based model of grammatical change using complex network methods. We identify global transitivity asa physical parameter of social networks critical for developing morphological structure, and hubs associated with scale-freenetworks as inhibitory, encouraging syntactic composition instead.

Learning induced illusions: Statistical learning creates false memories

The cognitive system readily extracts regularities in terms of object co-occurrences over space and time through statistical learning. However, how does learning such relationships influence the memory representations of individual objects? Here we used a false memory paradigm to examine the impact of statistical learning on memory representations of individual objects. Observers were exposed to a temporal sequence (Experiment 1) or spatial arrays (Experiment 2) of objects which contained object pairs (e.g., A-B). In a subsequent recognition phase, observers viewed a sequence or an array containing only one member of the original pair, and judged whether either the presented object or the missing object in the original pair was present. We found that statistical learning not only sharpened the detection of the presented object, but also induced a false memory of the missing object. This reveals a novel consequence of statistical learning: learning of regularities can create illusory memories.

An information-seeking account of eye movements during spoken and signedlanguage comprehension

Language comprehension in grounded contexts involves in-tegrating visual and linguistic information through decisionsabout visual fixation. But when the visual signal also con-tains information about the language source – as in the caseof written text or sign language – how do we decide where tolook? Here, we hypothesize that eye movements during lan-guage comprehension represent an adaptive response. Usingtwo case studies, we show that, compared to English-learners,young signers delayed their gaze shifts away from a languagesource, were more accurate with these shifts, and produced asmaller proportion of nonlanguage-driven shifts (E1). Next,we present a well-controlled, confirmatory experiment, show-ing that English-speaking adults produced fewer nonlanguage-driven shifts when processing printed text compared to spokenlanguage (E2). Together, these data suggest that people adaptto the value of seeking different information in order to in-crease the chance of rapid and accurate language understand-ing.

Preschoolers appropriately allocate roles based on relative ability in acooperative interaction

In cooperative activities, all parties have a shared goalbut may not have the same set of skills. The currentstudy considers whether preschoolers are sensitive toprobable differences in individuals’ competence whenallocating roles. We found that 3.5- to 5.5-year-olds userelative competence, as indexed by the age of theirintended partner, to determine who should do the harderand easier of two tasks in a cooperative interaction. Asecond experiment demonstrated that children allocateroles differently in a competitive context. Youngchildren infer differences in others’ ability and candivide labor efficiently to achieve their goals.

Intuitive psychophysics: Children’s exploratory play quantitatively tracks thediscriminability of alternative hypotheses

Studies suggest that children’s exploratory behavior is sensitive to uncertainty; however, few have approached thiswith sufficient precision to model quantitatively. Across three experiments, children (mean age=70 months) were asked to shakea box to identify which of two sets of marbles, differing in numerosity, were hidden inside. The sets’ numerosities varied in theirdiscriminability indices – the degree to which listeners can distinguish the sets based on the acoustic information generated.The time children spent shaking the box varied systematically with the discriminability of the alternative hypotheses they wereasked to distinguish, even though they heard only one set for each contrast. This suggests that children represent the uncertaintyin their own perceptual discrimination abilities (an ability we refer to as an intuitive psychophysics) and their exploratorybehavior is precisely calibrated to their degree of uncertainty about alternative hypotheses that might explain unobserved causesof perceptual data.

Leveraging Response Consistency within Individuals to Improve Group Accuracyfor Rank-Ordering Problems

Averaging the estimates of a number of individuals has beenshown to produce an estimate that is generally more accuratethan those of the individuals themselves. Similarly, averagingresponses from a single individual can also lead to a moreaccurate answer. How can we best combine estimates withinand between individuals to create an accurate group estimate?We report empirical results from a general knowledge rank-ordering experiment and demonstrate that individuals thatprovide more consistent answers across repeated elicitationsare also more accurate. We develop a consistency weightingheuristic and show that repeated elicitations within anindividual can be used to improve group accuracy. We alsodevelop a Thurstonian cognitive model which assumes adirect link between the process that explains the accuracy ofan individual and response consistency and show how themodel can infer accurate group answers.

A Rational Constructivist Account of the Characteristic-to-Defining Shift

A widely observed phenomenon in children’s word-extensionsand generalizations is the characteristic-to-defining shift,whereby young children initially generalize words based ontypical properties and gradually transition into generalizingwords using abstract, logical information. In this paper, wepropose a statistically principled model of conceptual devel-opment grounded in the trade-off between simplicity and fit tothe data. We run our model based on informant-provided fam-ily trees and the real-life characteristic features of people onthose trees. We demonstrate that the characteristic-to-definingshift does not necessarily depend on discrete change in rep-resentation or processes. Instead, the shift could fall out nat-urally from statistical inference over conceptual hypotheses.Our model finds that the shift occurs even when abstract logicalrelations are present from the outset of learning as long as char-acteristic features are informative but imperfect in their abilityto capture the underlying concept to be learned—a property ofour elicited features.

An incremental information-theoretic buffer supports sentence processing

People have the capability to process text three times fasterthan they would naturally read it, yet many current theories ofsentence processing rely on natural reading times as a proxyfor processing difficulty. How can people read material soquickly in spite of information processing limitations sug-gested by sentence processing theories? One possibility is thatsurprisal effects on reading time, the hallmark of processingdifficulty under sentence processing theories, might arise fromperceptual processing, implying no relation between surprisaland sentence processing difficulty. In this paper, we conducteda novel self-paced rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) ex-periment, which controlled perceptual processes to probe forsentence processing related surprisal effects. We further testedhow readers might compensate for information processing lim-its during RSVP. We find support for sentence processing re-lated surprisal effects, the pattern of which is consistent with aFirst-In, First-Out (FIFO) buffer model.

Different processes for reading words learned before and after onset of literacy

Learning to read has a substantial effect on the representationsof spoken and meaning forms of words. In this paper weassess literacy effects beyond representational changes,focusing on adaptations to the architecture of the readingsystem that maps between these representations. We present aconnectionist model of reading that predicted distinctprocessing of pre- and post-literacy acquired words. Forreading for meaning, words learned prior to literacy wereprocessed more indirectly via phonological representations,whereas for post-literacy acquired words, processing wasmore direct along the orthography to semantics pathway. Thismore computationally intensive route was prioritised becauseindirect phonology to semantics mappings were unavailable.Such an effect was less apparent for naming, because learningdirect orthography to phonology mappings is lesscomputationally intensive. These results were confirmed in ananalysis of naming and lexical decision behavioural data. Theeffect of literacy onset remains an observable artefact in adultreading.

Multiple variable cues in the environment promote accurate and robust wordlearning

Learning how words refer to aspects of the environment is acomplex task, but one that is supported by numerous cueswithin the environment which constrain the possibilities formatching words to their intended referents. In this paper wetested the predictions of a computational model of multiplecue integration for word learning, that predicted variation inthe presence of cues provides an optimal learning situation. Ina cross-situational learning task with adult participants, wevaried the reliability of presence of distributional, prosodic,and gestural cues. We found that the best learning occurredwhen cues were often present, but not always. The effect ofvariability increased the salience of individual cues for thelearner, but resulted in robust learning that was not vulnerableto individual cues’ presence or absence. Thus, variability ofmultiple cues in the language-learning environment providedthe optimal circumstances for word learning.

The ecological rationality of children’s option generation and decision making

In everyday life, before deciding what to do, one has to think about what could be done. We investigate option generation from a developmental perspective, testing the predictions of the Take-The-First-heuristic (TTF). Moreover, we examine the influence of time limitation on decision-making processes. Using soccer as a testbed, 6- to 13-year-old children (N = 97) were tested in a video-based option-generation paradigm. Children’s performance was aligned with predictions of TTF: Children generated a mean of 2.21 options, did so in a meaningful way and selected the first as final option in 74%. With shorter time, children generated fewer and higher quality options, selected better options and more often the first option as final decision. Further, with age, an increase of the number of options generated and an increase in quality of the final decisions emerged. This age effect was more pronounced with shorter time. Implications for real-life decision-making are discussed.

Multitasking Capability Versus Learning Efficiencyin Neural Network Architectures

One of the most salient and well-recognized features of humangoal-directed behavior is our limited ability to conduct mul-tiple demanding tasks at once. Previous work has identifiedoverlap between task processing pathways as a limiting fac-tor for multitasking performance in neural architectures. Thisraises an important question: insofar as shared representationbetween tasks introduces the risk of cross-talk and thereby lim-itations in multitasking, why would the brain prefer shared taskrepresentations over separate representations across tasks? Weseek to answer this question by introducing formal considera-tions and neural network simulations in which we contrast themultitasking limitations that shared task representations incurwith their benefits for task learning. Our results suggest thatneural network architectures face a fundamental tradeoff be-tween learning efficiency and multitasking performance in en-vironments with shared structure between tasks.

Analogical Inferences in Causal Systems

Analogical and causal reasoning theories both seek to explainpatterns of inductive inference. Researchers have claimed thatreasoning scenarios incorporating aspects of both analogicalcomparison and causal thinking necessitate a new model of in-ductive inference (Holyoak, Lee, & Lu, 2010; Lee & Holyoak,2008). This paper takes an opposing position, arguing that fea-tures of analogical models make correct claims about infer-ence patterns found among causal analogies, including analo-gies with both generative and preventative relations. Experi-ment 1 demonstrates that analogical inferences for these kindsof causal systems can be explained by alignment of relationalstructure, including higher-order relations. Experiment 2 fur-ther demonstrates that inferences strengthened by matchinghigher-order relations are not guided by the transfer of prob-abilistic information about a cause from base to target. Weconclude that causal analogies behave like analogies in gen-eral—analogical mapping provides candidate inferences whichcan then be reasoned about in the target.

Individual Differences in Transfer Mediated by Conceptual Priming

Research in analogical transfer suggests a simple type of transfer that occurs due to the activation of key relational concepts. Analysis on mental structured representations indicates that this transfer may act differently depending upon structural and perceptual features of the priming task. Two hundred eight participants were assigned to three experimental groups where they received a structure-priming, tested once and afterwards they received a perceptual-priming and tested again. As predicted, the effect of structure-priming was found across conditions whereas the effect of perceptual- priming (a six-second animation) was detected only in subjects with high levels of cognitive reflectiveness. These individual differences are interpreted as evidence that only highly reflective subjects were able to process visuospatial cues in the animation and to extract their structural features, hence activating relational concepts that influenced their interpretations of subsequent tasks.

When extremists win: On the behavior of iterated learning chains when priors areheterogeneous

How does the process of information transmission affect thecultural products that emerge from that process? This questionis often studied experimentally and computationally via iter-ated learning, in which participants learn from previous partic-ipants in a chain. Much research in this area builds on math-ematical analyses suggesting that iterated learning chains con-verge to people’s priors. We present three simulation studiessuggesting that when the population of learners is heteroge-neous, the behavior of the chain is systematically distorted bythe learners with the most extreme biases. We discuss implica-tions for the use of iterated learning as a methodological tooland for the processes that might have shaped cultural productsin the real world.

Calculating Probabilities Simplifies Word Learning

Children can use the statistical regularities of their environ-ment to learn word meanings, a mechanism known as cross-situational learning. We take a computational approach to in-vestigate how the information present during each observationin a cross-situational framework can affect the overall acqui-sition of word meanings. We do so by formulating variousin-the-moment learning mechanisms that are sensitive to dif-ferent statistics of the environment, such as counts and con-ditional probabilities. Each mechanism introduces a uniquesource of competition or mutual exclusivity bias to the model;the mechanism that maximally uses the model’s knowledge ofword meanings performs the best. Moreover, the gap betweenthis mechanism and others is amplified in more challenginglearning scenarios, such as learning from few examples. Key-words: cross-situational word learning; computational model-ing; word learning biases

Evaluating Vector-Space Models of Word Representation, or,The Unreasonable Effectiveness of Counting Words Near Other Words

Vector-space models of semantics represent words ascontinuously-valued vectors and measure similarity based onthe distance or angle between those vectors. Such representa-tions have become increasingly popular due to the recent de-velopment of methods that allow them to be efficiently esti-mated from very large amounts of data. However, the ideaof relating similarity to distance in a spatial representationhas been criticized by cognitive scientists, as human similar-ity judgments have many properties that are inconsistent withthe geometric constraints that a distance metric must obey. Weshow that two popular vector-space models, Word2Vec andGloVe, are unable to capture certain critical aspects of humanword association data as a consequence of these constraints.However, a probabilistic topic model estimated from a rela-tively small curated corpus qualitatively reproduces the asym-metric patterns seen in the human data. We also demonstratethat a simple co-occurrence frequency performs similarly toreduced-dimensionality vector-space models on medium-sizecorpora, at least for relatively frequent words.

Listeners integrate speech, gesture, and discourse structure to interpret thetemporal structure of complex events

Human communication has a remarkable capacity todescribe events that occurred elsewhere and at othertimes. In particular, when describing complex narratives,speakers must communicate temporal structure using amixture of words (e.g., “after”), gestures (e.g., pointingrightward for a later event), and discourse structure (e.g.,mentioning earlier events first). How do listenersintegrate these sources of temporal information to makesense of complex narratives? In two experiments, wesystematically manipulated gesture, speech, and order-of-mention to investigate their respective impacts oncomprehension of temporal structure. Gesture had asignificant effect on interpretations of temporal order.This influence of gesture, however, was weaker than theinfluence of both speech and order-of-mention. Indeed,in some cases, order-of-mention trumped explicitdescriptions in speech; for instance, if ‘earlier’ eventswere mentioned second, they were sometimes thought tohave occurred second. Listeners integrate multiplesources of information to interpret what happened when.

Dynamics of Affordance Actualization

The actualization of affordances can often be accomplished in numerous, equifinal ways. For instance, an individual could discard an item in a rubbish bin by walking over and dropping it, or by throwing it from a distance. The aim of the current study was to investigate the behavioral dynamics associated with such metastability using a ball-to-bin transportation task. Using time-interval between sequential ball-presentation as a control parameter, participants transported balls from a pickup location to a drop-off bin 9m away. A high degree of variability in task-actualization was expected and found, and the Cusp Catatrophe model was used to understand how this behavioral variability emerged as a function of hard (time interval) and soft (e.g. motivation) task dynamic constraints. Simulations demonstrated that this two parameter state manifold could capture the wide range of participant behaviors, and explain how these behaviors naturally emerge in an under-constrained task context.

Broadening the Scope of Recognition Memory

Within the literature of psychological and decision sciences, there is a critical difference in the way recognition is defined and studied experimentally. To address this difference, the current experiment examines and attempts to disentangle the influence of two recognition judgment sources (from within an experiment and from an individual’s prior life experiences) upon two different recognition judgments. By presenting participants with a set of related stimuli that vary naturally in environmental occurrence and by manipulating exposure within an experimental context, this experiment allows for a broader and more ecologically valid assessment of recognition memory. Contrasting with the typical word- frequency effect, the results reveal an overall bias to judge high-frequency items as studied on an episodic recognition test. Additionally, the results underscore the role of context by showing that a single study exposure increases the probability that individuals will judge stimuli as presented outside the laboratory.

A Computational Logic Approach to Human Syllogistic Reasoning

A recent meta-analysis (Khemlani & Johnson-Laird, 2012)about psychological experiments of syllogistic reasoningdemonstrates that the conclusions drawn by human reasonersstrongly deviate from conclusions of classical logic. Moreover,none of the current cognitive theories predictions fit reliablythe empirical data. In this paper, we show how humansyllogistic reasoning can be modeled under a new cognitivetheory, the Weak Completion Semantics. Our analysis basedon computational logics identifies seven principles necessaryto draw the inferences. Hence, this work contributes to acomputational foundation of cognitive reasoning processes.

Speakers’ gestures predict the meaning and perception of iconicity in signs

Sign languages stand out in that there is high prevalence ofconventionalised linguistic forms that map directly to theirreferent (i.e., iconic). Hearing adults show low performancewhen asked to guess the meaning of iconic signs suggestingthat their iconic features are largely inaccessible to them.However, it has not been investigated whether speakers’gestures, which also share the property of iconicity, mayassist non-signers in guessing the meaning of signs. Resultsfrom a pantomime generation task (Study 1) show thatspeakers’ gestures exhibit a high degree of systematicity, andshare different degrees of form overlap with signs (full,partial, and no overlap). Study 2 shows that signs with fulland partial overlap are more accurately guessed and areassigned higher iconicity ratings than signs with no overlap.Deaf and hearing adults converge in their iconic depictionsfor some concepts due to the shared conceptual knowledgeand manual-visual modality.

A Formal Approach to Modeling the Cost of Cognitive Control

This paper introduces a formal method to model the level of de-mand on control when executing cognitive processes. The costof cognitive control is parsed into an intensity cost which en-capsulates how much additional input information is requiredso as to get the specified response, and an interaction costwhich encapsulates the level of interference between individ-ual processes in a network. We develop a formal relationshipbetween the probability of successful execution of desired pro-cesses and the control signals (additive control biases). Thisrelationship is also used to specify optimal control policies toachieve a desired probability of activation for processes. Weobserve that there are boundary cases when finding such con-trol policies which leads us to introduce the interaction cost.We show that the interaction cost is influenced by the relativestrengths of individual processes, as well as the directionalityof the underlying competition between processes.

Optimization of American English, Spanish, and Mandarin Chinese over time forefficient communication

Frequent words tend to be short, and many researchers haveproposed that this relationship reflects a tendency towards ef-ficient communication. Recent work has sought to formalizethis observation in the context of information theory, which es-tablishes a limit on communicative efficiency called the chan-nel capacity. In this paper, I first show that the compositionalstructure of natural language prevents natural language com-munication from getting close to the channel capacity, but thata different limit, which incorporates probability in context,may be achievable. Next, I present two corpus studies in threetypologically-diverse languages that provide evidence that lan-guages change over time towards the achievable limit. Theseresults suggest that natural language optimizes for efficiencyover time, and does so in a way that is appopriate for compo-sitional codes.

Relational Concept Learning via Guided Interactive Discovery

A key goal in both education and higher-order cognitionresearch is to understand how relational concepts are bestlearned. In the current work, we present a novel approach forlearning complex relational categories – a low-support,interactive discovery interface. The platform, which allowslearners to make modifications to exemplars and see thecorresponding effects on membership, holds the potential toaugment relational learning by facilitating self-directed,alignably-different comparisons that explore what the learnerdoes not yet understand. We compared interactive learning to anidentification learning task. Participants were assessed on theirability to generalize category knowledge to novel exemplarsfrom the same domain. Although identification learners wereprovided with seven times as many positive examples of thecategory during training, interactive learners demonstratedenhanced generalization accuracy and knowledge of specificmembership constraints. Moreover, the data suggest thatidentification learners tended to overgeneralize categoryknowledge to non-members – a problem that interactive learnersexhibited to a significantly lesser degree. Overall, the resultsshow interactive training to be a powerful tool forsupplementing relational category learning, with particularutility for refining category knowledge. We conclude withimplications of these findings and promising future directions.

The Use of Iconic Words in Early Child-Parent Interactions

This paper examines the use of iconic words in earlyconversations between children and caregivers. Thelongitudinal data include a span of six observations of 35children-parent dyads in the same semi-structured activity.Our findings show that children’s speech initially has a highproportion of iconic words, and over time, these wordsbecome diluted by an increase of arbitrary words. Parents’speech is also initially high in iconic words, with a decreasein the proportion of iconic words over time – in this casedriven by the use of fewer iconic words. The level anddevelopment of iconicity are related to individual differencesin the children’s cognitive skills. Our findings fit with thehypothesis that iconicity facilitates early word learning andmay play an important role in learning to produce new words

Evidence for the size principle in semantic and perceptual domains

Shepard’s Universal Law of Generalization offered a com-pelling case for the first physics-like law in cognitive sciencethat should hold for all intelligent agents in the universe. Shep-ard’s account is based on a rational Bayesian model of general-ization, providing an answer to the question of why such a lawshould emerge. Extending this account to explain how humansuse multiple examples to make better generalizations requiresan additional assumption, called the size principle: hypothesesthat pick out fewer objects should make a larger contributionto generalization. The degree to which this principle warrantssimilarly law-like status is far from conclusive. Typically, eval-uating this principle has not been straightforward, requiringadditional assumptions. We present a new method for evaluat-ing the size principle that is more direct, and apply this methodto a diverse array of datasets. Our results provide support forthe broad applicability of the size principle.

Modeling the Ellsberg Paradox by Argument Strength

We present a formal measure of argument strength, whichcombines the ideas that conclusions of strong arguments are (i)highly probable and (ii) their uncertainty is relatively precise.Likewise, arguments are weak when their conclusion proba-bility is low or when it is highly imprecise. We show howthe proposed measure provides a new model of the Ellsbergparadox. Moreover, we further substantiate the psychologi-cal plausibility of our approach by an experiment (N = 60).The data show that the proposed measure predicts human in-ferences in the original Ellsberg task and in corresponding ar-gument strength tasks. Finally, we report qualitative data takenfrom structured interviews on folk psychological conceptionson what argument strength means.

Causation and norms of proper functioning: Counterfactuals are (still) relevant

Causal judgments are well-known to be sensitive to violationsof both prescriptive moral and descriptive statistical norms.There is ongoing discussion as to whether both effects arebest explained through changes in the relevance of counter-factual possibilities, or if moral norm violations should be in-dependently explained through a potential polysemy whereby‘cause’ may simply mean ‘is morally responsible for’. Insupport of the latter view, recent work has pointed out thatmoral norm violations affect judgments of agents, but not inan-imate objects, and that these effects are moderated by agents’knowledge states. We advance this debate by demonstratingthat judgments of counterfactual relevance exhibit preciselythe same patterns, and that judgments of inanimate objects areactually highly sensitive to whether the object violated a pre-scriptive norm by malfunctioning. The latter finding is difficultto account for through polysemy, but is predicted by changes inthe relevance of counterfactual alternatives. Finally, we showthat direct (non-moral) interventions on the the relevance ofcounterfactual alternatives affect causal judgments in preciselythe same way as functional and moral norm violations.

Assessing the Linguistic Productivity of Unsupervised Deep Neural Networks

Increasingly, cognitive scientists have demonstrated interest inapplying tools from deep learning. One use for deep learning isin language acquisition where it is useful to know if a linguisticphenomenon can be learned through domain-general means.To assess whether unsupervised deep learning is appropriate,we first pose a smaller question: Can unsupervised neural net-works apply linguistic rules productively, using them in novelsituations? We draw from the literature on determiner/nounproductivity by training an unsupervised, autoencoder networkmeasuring its ability to combine nouns with determiners. Oursimple autoencoder creates combinations it has not previouslyencountered and produces a degree of overlap matching adults.While this preliminary work does not provide conclusive evi-dence for productivity, it warrants further investigation withmore complex models. Further, this work helps lay the foun-dations for future collaboration between the deep learning andcognitive science communities.

Opinion Cascades and Echo-Chambers in Online Networks: A Proof of Concept Agent-Based Model

In online networks, the polarization of opinions (e.g., regarding presidential elections or referenda) has been associated with the creation of “echo-chambers” of like- minded peers, secluded from those of contrary viewpoints. Previous work has commonly attributed such phenomena to self-regarding preferences (e.g., confirmation bias), individual differences, and the pre-dispositions of users, with clusters forming over repeated interactions. The present work provides a proof of concept Agent-Based Model that demonstrates online networks are susceptible to echo-chambers from a single opinion cascade, due to the spatiotemporal order induced by lateral transmission. This susceptibility is found to vary as a function of degree of interconnectivity and opinion strength. Critically, such effects are found despite globally proportionate levels of opinions, equally rational agents (i.e. absent conformity, confirmation bias or pre-disposition architecture), and prior to cyclical interactions. The assumptions and implications of this work, including the value of Agent-Based Modelling to cognitive psychology, are discussed.

Make-or-break: chasing risky goals or settling for safe rewards?

Humans regularly invest time towards activities characterized by dramatic success or failure outcomes, where criti-cally, the outcome is uncertain ex-ante. How should people allocate time between such make-or-break activities and other safealternatives, where rewards are more predictable (e.g., linear) functions of time? We present a formal framework for studyingtime allocation between these two types of activities, and explore (optimal) behavior in both one-shot and dynamic versions ofthe problem. In the one-shot version, we illustrate the striking discontinuous relation between peoples skill and optimal timeallocation to the make-or-break task. In the dynamic version, we formulate both fully rational and boundedly rational strategies,both defined by a giving up threshold, which adaptively dictates when one should cease pursuit of the make- or-break goal.Comparing strategies across environments, we investigate the cost of sidestepping the computational burden of full rationality.

Inferential Pitfalls in Decoding Neural Representations

A key challenge for cognitive neuroscience is to decipher therepresentational schemes of the brain. A recent class of decodingalgorithms for fMRI data, stimulus-feature-based encodingmodels, is becoming increasingly popular for inferring thedimensions of neural representational spaces from stimulus-feature spaces. We argue that such inferences are not always valid,because decoding can occur even if the neural representationalspace and the stimulus-feature space use different representationalschemes. This can happen when there is a systematic mappingbetween them. In a simulation, we successfully decoded the binaryrepresentation of numbers from their decimal features. Sincebinary and decimal number systems use different representations,we cannot conclude that the binary representation encodes decimalfeatures. The same argument applies to the decoding of neuralpatterns from stimulus-feature spaces and we urge caution ininferring the nature of the neural code from such methods. Wediscuss ways to overcome these inferential limitations.

The Relational Luring Effect: False Recognition via Relational Similarity

We present evidence for a novel relational luring effect (RLE) in recognition memory. Participants performeda continuous associative recognition task in which they had to discriminate between new, old and recombined word pairs.Participants made more false alarms and responded more slowly to lures (TABLE CLOTH) that were relationally similarto studied pairs (FLOOR CARPET). RTs and false alarms for lures increased linearly as the number of previously studieddifferent exemplars of the relation increased (e.g., 0 to 4 previous exemplars). The RLE effect was stronger for relations thatwere represented by exemplars that were more typical of the relation. These results suggest that semantic relations exist asindependent representations in LTM, and that during associative recognition these representations can be a spurious source offamiliarity. The RLE has implications for models of semantic and episodic memory, unitization in associative recognition,analogical reasoning, and constructive memory research.

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance

We found an unexpected positive effect of target-to-distractorsimilarity (TD) in a visual search task, despite overwhelmingevidence in the literature that TD similarity hurts visual searchperformance. Participants with no prior knowledge of Chineseperformed 12 hour-long sessions over 4 weeks, where they hadto find a briefly presented target character among a set ofdistractors. At the beginning of the experiment, TD similarityhurt performance, but the effect reversed during the firstsession and remained positive throughout the remainingsessions. We present a simple connectionist model thataccounts for that reversal of TD similarity effects on visualsearch and we discuss possible theoretical explanations.

Timing Time: Why Early Vision is Cognitively Impenetrable

Newen and Vetter (2016) argue that cognitive penetration (CP) ofperceptual experience is the most possible account of the evidence.They target both the weak impenetrability thesis that only some earlyvisual processes are cognitively impenetrable (CI), and the strongimpenetrability thesis that all perceptual processes are CI. Since Iagree that perceptual processing as a whole is CP, I will concentrateon their arguments against the weak CI thesis. In attacking weak CI,the authors take aim at Raftopoulos’ arguments supporting the CI ofearly vision. Their main argument comes from studies that, Newenand Vetter think, show that early vision is CP by demonstrating theexistence of cognitive effects on early vision. I examine the sameempirical evidence that Newen and Vatter discuss and argue thissame evidence strongly supports the view that early vision is CI.

The Wason Selection Task: A Meta-Analysis

In Wason’s selection task, participants select whichever offour cards could provide evidence about the truth or falsity ofa conditional rule. As our meta-analysis of hundreds of ex-periments corroborates, participants tend to overlook one ofthe cards that could falsify the rule. 15 distinct theories aimto explain this phenomenon and others, but many of thempresuppose that cards are selected independently of one an-other. We show that this assumption is false: Shannon’s en-tropy for selections is reliably redundant in comparison withthose of 10,000 simulated experiments using the same fourindividual probabilities for each real experiment. This resultrules out those theories presupposing independent selections.Of the remaining theories, only two predict the frequenciesof selections, one (due to Johnson-Laird & Wason, 1970a)provides a better fit to the experimental data than the other(due to Klauer et al., 2007). We discuss the implications ofthese results.

Mental Algorithms in the Historical Emergence of Word Meanings

Words frequently acquire new senses, but the mental processthat underlies the historical emergence of these senses is oftenopaque. Many have suggested that word meanings develop innon-arbitrary ways, but no attempt has been made to formalizethese proposals and test them against historical data at scale.We propose that word meaning extension should reflect a drivetowards cognitive economy. We test this proposal by exploringa family of computational models that predict the evolution ofword senses, evaluated against a large digitized lexicon thatdates back 1000 years in English language history. Our find-ings suggest that word meanings not only extend in predictableways, but also that they do so following an historical path thattends to minimize cognitive cost - through a process of nearest-neighbor chaining. Our work contributes a formal approach toreverse-engineering mental algorithms of the human lexicon.

A cognitive analysis of deception without lying

When the interests of interlocutors are not aligned, either partymay wish to avoid truthful disclosure. A sender wishing toconceal the truth from a receiver may lie by providing falseinformation, mislead by actively encouraging the receiver toreach a false conclusion, or simply be uninformative by provid-ing little or no relevant information. Lying entails moral andother hazards, such as detection and its consequences, and isthus often avoided. We focus here on the latter two strategies,arguably more pernicious and prevalent, but not without theirown drawbacks. We argue and show in two studies that whenchoosing between these options, senders consider the level ofsuspicion likely to be exercised on the part of the receiver andhow much truth must be revealed in order to mislead. Extend-ing Bayesian models of cooperative communication to includehigher level inference regarding the helpfulness of the senderleads to insight into the strategies employed in non-cooperativecontexts.

Connecting stimulus-driven attention to the properties of infant-directed speech —Is exaggerated intonation also more surprising?

The exaggerated intonation and special rhythmic properties ofinfant-directed speech (IDS) have been hypothesized to attractinfant’s attention to the speech stream. However, studiesinvestigating IDS in the context of models of attention arefew. A number of such models suggest that surprising ornovel perceptual inputs attract attention, where novelty can beoperationalized as the statistical predictability of the stimulusin a context. Since prosodic patterns such as F0 contours areaccessible to young infants who are also adept statisticallearners, the present paper investigates a hypothesis that pitchcontours in IDS are less predictable than those in adult-directed speech (ADS), thereby efficiently tapping into thebasic attentional mechanisms of the listeners. Results fromanalyses with naturalistic IDS and ADS speech show that IDShas lower overall predictability of intonation acrossneighboring syllables even when the F0 contours in bothspeaking styles are normalized to the same frequency range.

When does a ‘visual proof by induction’ serve a proof-like function in mathematics?

A proof by mathematical induction demonstrates that ageneral theorem is necessarily true for all natural numbers. Ithas been suggested that some theorems may also be provenby a ‘visual proof by induction’ (Brown, 2010), despite thefact that the image only displays particular cases of thegeneral theorem. In this study we examine the nature of theconclusions drawn from a visual proof by induction. We findthat, while most university-educated viewers demonstrate awillingness to generalize the statement to nearby cases notdepicted in the image, only viewers who have been trained informal proof strategies show significantly higher resistance tothe suggestion of large-magnitude counterexamples to thetheorem. We conclude that for most university-educatedadults without proof-training the image serves as the basis ofa standard inductive generalization and does not provide thedegree of certainty required for mathematical proof.

Interpreting Asymmetries in Speech Perception with Bayesian Inference

This paper proposes a Bayesian account of asymmetriesfound in speech perception: In many languages, listenersshow greater sensitivity if a non-coronal sound (/b/, /p/, /g/,/k/) is changed to coronal sounds (/d/, /t/) than vice versa. Thecurrently predominant explanation for these asymmetries isthat they reflect innate constraints from Universal Grammar.Alternatively, we propose that the asymmetries could simplyarise from optimal inference given the statistical properties ofdifferent speech categories of the listener’s native language.In the framework of Bayesian inference, we examined twostatistical parameters of coronal and non-coronal sounds:frequencies of occurrence and variance in articulation. In thelanguages in which perceptual asymmetries have been found,coronal sounds are either more frequent or more variable thannon-coronal sounds. Given such differences, an ideal observeris more likely to perceive a non-coronal speech signal as acoronal segment than vice versa. Thus, the perceptualasymmetries can be explained as a natural consequence ofprobabilistic inference. The coronal/non-coronal asymmetryis similar to asymmetries observed in many other cognitivedomains. Thus, we argue that it is more parsimonious toexplain this asymmetry as one of many similar asymmetriesfound in cognitive processing, rather than a linguistic-specific, innate constraint.

Quantitative Models of Human-Human Conversational Grounding Processes

Natural language dialogue between multiple participants re-quires conversational grounding, a process whereby interlocu-tors achieve a shared understanding. However, the mecha-nisms involved in the grounding process are under dispute.Two prominent models of dialogue between multiple partici-pants are: interactive alignment, a simpler model that relieson automatic priming processes within individuals, and in-terpersonal synergy, a more complicated model emphasizingcoordinated interaction across participants. Using recurrenceanalysis methods, Fusaroli and Tyl ́en (2016) simultaneouslyevaluated both models and showed that alignment is an insuf-ficient explanation for grounding or for the teams’ task per-formance. However, their task and resulting dialogues lackthe typical complexity of conversations or teamwork. Further-more, the interpersonal synergy model was not clearly differ-entiated from other coordination-focused models of groundingwith explicit foundations in strategy and intentionality (i.e., au-dience design, joint activity, perspective taking). Here we testrecurrence-based models in a collaborative task that stressedthe grounding process. Results support a coordination modelof dialogue over the alignment model as a predictor of perfor-mance. Content-based mediation analyses showed that the co-ordination recurrence model includes critical aspects of strate-gic design and is not purely interpersonal synergy.

Comprehenders Model the Nature of Noise in the Environment

Recent work suggests that language understanding is the result of rational inference over a noisy channel. Uponperceiving a sentence, listeners decode the speaker’s intended sentence from the prior probability that a speaker would say thatsentence and the probability that it would be corrupted to the perceived sentence by noise. Here we examine the listener’snoise model. Readers were asked to correct sentences if they thought they contained an error. We manipulated context suchthat participants corrected exposure sentences containing either deletion, insertion, swap, mixed, or no errors (e.g., swap: Abystander was rescued by the fireman in the time of nick.). Test sentences were syntactically licensed but implausible (e.g., Thebat swung the player). On test sentences, participants’ corrections differed by exposure condition. This suggests participantstrack the type of errors that have a higher likelihood and make inferences about the intentions of the speaker accordingly.

Is the strength of regularisation behaviour uniform across linguistic levels?

Human languages contain very little unconditioned variation.In contexts where language learners are exposed to input thatcontains inconsistencies, they tend to regularise it, either byeliminating competing variants, or conditioning variant use onthe context. In the present study we compare regularisationbehaviour across linguistic levels, looking at how adult learn-ers respond to variability in morphology and word order. Ourresults suggest similar strengths in regularisation between lin-guistic levels given input languages whose complexity is com-parable.

Converting Cascade-Correlation Neural Nets into Probabilistic Generative Models

Humans are not only adept in recognizing what class an in-put instance belongs to (i.e., classification task), but perhapsmore remarkably, they can imagine (i.e., generate) plausibleinstances of a desired class with ease, when prompted. Inspiredby this, we propose a framework which allows transformingCascade-Correlation Neural Networks (CCNNs) into proba-bilistic generative models, thereby enabling CCNNs to gen-erate samples from a category of interest. CCNNs are a well-known class of deterministic, discriminative NNs, which au-tonomously construct their topology, and have been successfulin accounting for a variety of psychological phenomena. Ourproposed framework is based on a Markov Chain Monte Carlo(MCMC) method, called the Metropolis-adjusted Langevin al-gorithm, which capitalizes on the gradient information of thetarget distribution to direct its explorations towards regionsof high probability, thereby achieving good mixing proper-ties. Through extensive simulations, we demonstrate the effi-cacy of our proposed framework. Importantly, our frameworkbridges computational, algorithmic, and implementational lev-els of analysis.

Mental Representations and Computational Modeling of Context-Specific HumanNorm Systems

Human behavior is frequently guided by social and moralnorms; in fact, no societies, no social groups could exist with-out norms. However, there are few cognitive science ap-proaches to this central phenomenon of norms. While therehas been some progress in developing formal representationsof norm systems (e.g., deontological approaches), we do notyet know basic properties of human norms: how they arerepresented, activated, and learned. Further, what computa-tional models can capture these properties, and what algo-rithms could learn them? In this paper we describe initial ex-periments on human norm representations in which the contextspecificity of norms features prominently. We then provide aformal representation of norms using Dempster-Shafer Theorythat allows a machine learning algorithm to learn norms un-der uncertainty from these human data, while preserving theircontext specificity.

Attractor Dynamics in Delay Discounting: A Call for Complexity

The outcomes of intertemporal choices indicate that people discount rewards by their delay. These outcomes are well described by discounting functions. However, to fully understand the decision process one needs models describing how the process of decision-making unfolds dynamically over time. Here, we validate a recently published attractor model that extends discounting functions through a description of the dynamics leading to a final choice outcome within and across trials. We focus on the decision dynamics across trials. We derive qualitative predictions for the inter-trial dynamics of sequences of decisions that are unique to this type of model. We test these predictions in a delay discounting game where we sequentially manipulated subjective values of options across all attribute dimensions. Results confirm the model’s predictions. We discuss future challenges on integrating attractor models towards a general attractor model of delay discounting to enhance our understanding of the processes underlying delay discounting decisions.

Strategic exploration in human adaptive control

How do people explore in order to gain rewards in uncer-tain dynamical systems? Within a reinforcement learningparadigm, control normally involves trading off between ex-ploration (i.e. trying out actions in order to gain more knowl-edge about the system) and exploitation (i.e. using currentknowledge of the system to maximize reward). We study anovel control task in which participants must steer a boat ona grid, aiming to follow a path of high reward whilst learninghow their actions affect the boat’s position. We find that partic-ipants explore strategically yet conservatively, exploring morewhen mistakes are less costly and practicing actions that willbe required later on.

How Does Instance-Based Inference About Event Frequencies Develop?An Analysis with a Computational Process Model

To make inferences about the frequency of events in theworld (e.g., the prevalence of diseases or the popularity ofconsumer products), people often exploit observations ofrelevant instances sampled from their personal social network.How does this ability to infer event frequencies by searchingand relying on personal instance knowledge develop fromchildhood to adulthood? To address this question, weconducted a study in which children (age 8–11 years) andadults (age 19–34 years) judged the relative frequencies offirst names in Germany. Based on the recalled instances of thenames in participants’ social networks, we modeled theirfrequency judgments and the underlying search process with aBayesian hierarchical latent-mixture approach encompassingdifferent computational models. We found developmentaldifferences in the inference strategies that children and adultsused. Whereas the judgments of most adults were bestdescribed by a noncompensatory strategy that assumes limitedand sequentially ordered search (social-circle model), thejudgments of most children were best described by acompensatory strategy that assumes exhaustive search andinformation aggregation (availability-by-recall). Our resultshighlight that already children use instance knowledge to inferevent frequencies but they appear to search more exhaustivelyfor instances than adults. One interpretation of these results isthat the ability to conduct ordered and focused search is acentral aspect in the development of noncompensatoryinstance-based inference.

Prior Expectations in Linguistic Learning: A Stochastic Model of IndividualDifferences

When learners are exposed to inconsistent input, do they reproduce the probabilities in the input (probability match-ing), or produce some variants disproportionately often (regularization)? Laboratory results and computational models ofartificial language learning both argue that the learning mechanism is basically probability matching, with regularization aris-ing from additional factors. However, these models were fit to aggregated experimental data, which can exhibit probabilitymatching even if all individuals regularize. To assess whether learning can be accurately characterized as basically probabilitymatching or systematizing at the individual level, we ran a large-scale experiment. We found substantial individual variation.The structure of this variation is not predicted by recent beta-binomial models. We introduce a new model, the Double ScalingSigmoid (DSS) model, fit its parameters on a by-participant basis, and show that it captures the patterns in the data. Priorexpectations in the DSS are abstract, and do not entirely represent previous experience.

Effects of Grammatical Gender on Object Description

Can grammatical gender influence how people conceptualize the referents of nouns? Using an implicit measure, we investigated whether such an effect could be found in a task where neither grammatical nor biological gender is highlighted. In the current study, conducted in English, speakers of French, German and Romanian with knowledge of English were asked to generate adjectives they associate with referents of nouns. Afterwards, the gender valence of the adjectives was measured. The results showed that participants generated more feminine adjectives for nouns with majority feminine translations compared to nouns with majority masculine translations. We found a stronger effect of grammatical gender for some semantic categories than for others. Significant effects of grammatical gender were present starting with the 2nd adjective generated by participants (effects were stronger for adjectives generated 2nd and 3rd by participants, as opposed to the 1st adjective).

Inferring Human Interaction from Motion Trajectories in Aerial Videos

People are adept at perceiving interactions from movementsof simple shapes but the underlying mechanism remains un-known. Previous studies have often used object movementsdefined by experimenters. The present study used aerial videosrecorded by drones in a real-life environment to generate de-contextualized motion stimuli. Motion trajectories of dis-played elements were the only visual input. We measuredhuman judgments of interactiveness between two moving el-ements, and the dynamic change of such judgments over time.A hierarchical model was developed to account for human per-formance in this task, which represents interactivity using la-tent variables, and learns the distribution of critical movementfeatures that signal potential interactivity. The model providesa good fit to human judgments and can also be generalized tothe original Heider-Simmel animations (1944). The model canalso synthesize decontextualized animations with controlleddegree of interactiveness, providing a viable tool for studyinganimacy and social perception.

Children’s spontaneous comparisons from 26 to 58 months predict performance inverbal and non-verbal analogy tests in 6th grade

Comparison supports the development of children’s analogicalreasoning. The evidence for this claim comes from labora-tory studies. We describe spontaneous comparisons producedby 24 typically developing children from 26 to 58 months.Children tend to express similarity before expressing differ-ence. They compare objects from the same category beforeobjects from different categories, make global comparisons be-fore specific comparisons, and specify perceptual features ofsimilarity/difference before non-perceptual features. We theninvestigate how a theoretically interesting subset of children’scomparisons – those expressing a specific feature of similar-ity or difference – relates to analogical reasoning as measuredby verbal and non-verbal tests in 6th grade. The number ofspecific comparisons children produce before 58 months pre-dicts their scores on both tests, controlling for vocabulary at54 months. The results provide naturalistic support for experi-mental findings on comparison development, and demonstratea strong relationship between children’s early comparisons andtheir later analogical reasoning.

Learning About Causal Systems Through Play

It is commonly believed that children are able to learn through play. Recent studies have found that children are able to learn causal rules through free play (Sim & Xu, in press). One such study found that children learned how to correctly activate machines, using either a block that was the same shape or the same color as the machine, when given five minutes to play with them. However, would children be able to learn a more complex causal rule through free play as well and would their performance be comparable to children who were didactically taught the same causal rule? In the current study, we show that children are able to learn more complex causal rules through free play. We also show that children perform significantly better when learning these rules through free play or by first engaging in free play and then observing, as opposed to solely through observation.

Conditionals, Individual Variation, and the Scorekeeping Task

In this manuscript we study individual variation in the interpretation of conditionals by establishing individual profiles of the participants based on their behavioral respon- ses and reflective attitudes. To investigate the participants’ reflective attitudes we introduce a new experimental paradigm called the Scorekeeping Task, and a Bayesian mixture model tailored to analyze the data. The goal is thereby to identify the participants who follow the Suppositional Theory of condi- tionals and Inferentialism and to investigate their performance on the uncertain and-to-if inference task.

A case for systematic sound symbolism in pragmatics: The role of the first phoneme in question prediction in context

Turn-taking in conversation is a cognitively demanding process that proceeds rapidly due to interlocutors utilizing a range of cues to aid prediction. In the present study we set out to test recent claims that content question words (also called wh-words) sound similar within languages as an adaptation to help listeners predict that a question is about to be asked. We test whether upcoming questions can be predicted based on the first phoneme of a turn and the prior context. We analyze the Switchboard corpus of English by means of a decision tree to test whether /w/ and /h/ are good statistical cues of upcoming questions in conversation. Based on the results, we perform a controlled experiment to test whether people really use these cues to recognize questions. In both studies we show that both the initial phoneme and the sequential context help predict questions. This contributes converging evidence that elements of languages adapt to pragmatic pressures applied during conversation.

Learning to See People Like People: Predicting Social Impressions of Faces

Humans make complex inferences on faces, ranging from ob-jective properties (gender, ethnicity, expression, age, identity,etc) to subjective judgments (facial attractiveness, trustworthi-ness, sociability, friendliness, etc). While the objective as-pects of face perception have been extensively studied, rela-tively fewer computational models have been developed forthe social impressions of faces. Bridging this gap, we de-velop a method to predict human impressions of faces in 40subjective social dimensions, using deep representations fromstate-of-the-art neural networks. We find that model perfor-mance grows as the human consensus on a face trait increases,and that model predictions outperform human groups in cor-relation with human averages. This illustrates the learnabilityof subjective social perception of faces, especially when thereis high human consensus. Our system can be used to decidewhich photographs from a personal collection will make thebest impression. The results are significant for the field of so-cial robotics, demonstrating that robots can learn the subjectivejudgments defining the underlying fabric of human interaction.

A Rational Approach to Stereotype Change

Existing theories of stereotype change have often made use ofcategorisation principles in order to provide qualitative expla-nations for both the revision and maintenance of stereotypicalbeliefs. The present paper examines the quantitative methodsunderlying these explanations, contrasting both rational andheuristic models of stereotype change using participant dataand model fits. In a comparison of three models each simulat-ing existing descriptions of stereotype change, both empiricaldata and model fits suggest that stereotypes are updated usingrational categorisation processes. This presents stereotype useas a more rational behaviour than may commonly be assumed,and provides new avenues of encouraging stereotype changeaccording to rational principles.

Population size, learning, and innovation determine linguistic complexity

There are a number of claims regarding why linguistic com-plexity varies, for example: i) different types of societalstructure (e.g. Wray & Grace, 2007), ii) population size (e.g.Lupyan & Dale, 2010), and iii) the proportion of child vs. adultlearners (e.g. Trudgill, 2011). This simple model of interact-ing agents, capable of learning and innovation, partially sup-ports all these accounts. However, several subtle points arise.Firstly, differences in the capacity or opportunity to learn deter-mine how much complexity can remain stable. Secondly, smallpopulations are susceptible to large amounts of drift and sub-sequent loss, unless innovation is frequent. Conversely, largepopulations remain resilient to change unless there is too muchinnovation, which leads to a collapse in complexity. Next, ifadult learners are prevalent, we can instead expect less sus-tained complexity in large populations. Finally, creolisationdoes not imply simplification in smaller populations.

A rational analysis of marketing strategies

Rational accounts of decision-making are incompatible withthe prevalence and success of ubiquitous marketing strategies.In this paper, we demonstrate, using computational experi-ments, how an ideal Bayesian observer model of preferencelearning is compatible with the manipulation of purchasingdecisions via a number of well-known marketing techniques.The ability of this model to predict the effects of both famil-iar and novel marketing interventions suggests it as a plausiblecandidate theory of consumer marketing. Simultaneously, byclarifying the logic underneath the interplay between environ-mental exposure and preference distortions seen in economicdecisions, this model rationalizes the seemingly irrational sus-ceptibility of consumers to marketing.

Spatial language promotes cross-domain associations in early childhood

Spatial language is often used metaphorically to describe other domains, including time (a long sound) and pitch (a high sound). How does experience with these metaphors shape the associations we make across disparate domains? Here, we tested 3- to 6-year-old English-speaking children and adults with a cross-domain matching task that assessed space-time and space-pitch mappings. We tested spatial relations that are expressed in English metaphors for time and pitch, as well as metaphors that are unfamiliar to English speakers, but expressed in other languages. Participants performed a perceptual matching task, in which they matched pictures and sounds, and a linguistic matching task, in which they matched pictures or sounds to verbal labels. Adults readily matched between space and time and between space and pitch, using relations expressed by both familiar and unfamiliar metaphors. Children showed an advantage for linguistic matching compared to perceptual matching, but their performance was similarly unaffected by metaphor familiarity. Together, these results suggest that spatial language promotes the development of cross-domain associations, and that experience with particular spatial metaphors is not required to produce this benefit.

Preemption in Singular Causation Judgments: A Computational Model

Causal queries about singular cases are ubiquitous, yet thequestion of how we assess whether a particular outcome wasactually caused by a specific potential cause turns out to bedifficult to answer. Relying on the causal power approach,Cheng and Novick (2005) proposed a model of causal attribu-tion intended to help answering this question. We challengethis model, both conceptually and empirically. The centralproblem of this model is that it treats the presence of sufficientcauses as necessarily causal in singular causation, and thus ne-glects that causes can be preempted in their efficacy. Also, themodel does not take into account that reasoners incorporateuncertainty about the underlying causal structure and strengthof causes when making causal inferences. We propose a newmeasure of causal attribution and embed it into our structure in-duction model of singular causation (SISC). Two experimentssupport the model.

Marbles in Inaction: Counterfactual Simulation and Causation by Omission

Consider the following causal explanation: The ball wentthrough the goal because the defender didn’t block it. Thereare at least two problems with citing omissions as causal ex-planations. First, how do we choose the relevant candidateomission (e.g. why the defender and not the goalkeeper). Sec-ond, how do we determine what would have happened in therelevant counterfactual situation (i.e. maybe the shot wouldstill have gone through the goal even if it had been blocked).In this paper, we extend the counterfactual simulation model(CSM) of causal judgment (Gerstenberg, Goodman, Lagnado,& Tenenbaum, 2014) to handle the second problem. In two ex-periments, we show how people’s causal model of the situationaffects their causal judgments via influencing what counterfac-tuals they consider. Omissions are considered causes to theextent that the outcome in the relevant counterfactual situationwould have been different from what it actually was.

A Two-Step Signal Detection Model of Belief Bias

When asked to assess the deductive validity of an argument, people are influenced by their prior knowledge of the content. Recently, two competing explanations for this belief bias effect have been proposed, each based on signal detection theory. Under a response bias explanation, people set more lenient decision criteria for believable than for unbelievable arguments. Alternatively, believable and unbelievable arguments may differ in subjective argument strength for both valid and invalid items. Two experiments tested these accounts by asking participants to assess the validity of categorical syllogisms and rate their confidence. Conclusion- believability was manipulated either within- or between- groups. A two-step signal detection model was applied to examine the effects on the relative location of the decision threshold and the distributions of argument strength. Equivalent belief bias effects were found when believability was manipulated within- and between-groups, supporting the view that the belief bias effect is due to response bias.

Rational use of prosody predicts projection in manner adverb utterances

Speakers can be taken to be committed to utterance con-tent even when that content is contributed in the scope ofan entailment-canceling operator, like negation (e.g., Chier-chia & McConnell-Ginet, 1990). We develop a probabilisticmodel of this phenomenon, called ‘projection’, that relies onthe prosodic realization of utterances. We synthesize exist-ing theoretical claims about prosody, information structure andprojection into a model that assumes a rational speaker (Frank& Goodman, 2012) who produces utterances with prosodicmelodies that can signal which utterance content she is com-mitted to. Predictions of the probabilistic model are comparedto the responses of an experiment designed to test the effect ofprosody on projection in manner adverb utterances. Key be-haviors of the model are borne out empirically, and the quan-titative fit is surprisingly good given that the model has onlyone free parameter. Our findings lend support to analyses ofprojection that are sensitive to the information structure of ut-terances (e.g., Simons, Beaver, Roberts, & Tonhauser, 2017).

A Common Neural Component for Finger Gnosis and Magnitude Comparison

Finger gnosis (the ability to identify which finger has beentouched) and magnitude comparison (the ability to determinewhich of two numbers is larger) are surprisingly correlated.We present a spiking neuron model of a common componentthat could be used in both tasks: an array of pointers. Weshow that if the model's single tuned parameter is set to matchhuman accuracy performance in one task, then it also matcheson the other task (with the exception of one data point). Thisprovides a novel explanation of the relation, and proposes acommon component that could be used across cognitive tasks.

Flexible integration of a navigable, clustered environment

The representation of navigable space, consisting of multiple interconnected spaces, yet is not well understood. Weexamined different levels of integration within memory (local, regional, global). Participants learned two distinctive regions ofa virtual environment that converged at a common transition-point. Subsequently, we tested their memory with a pointing task,varying body alignment during pointing, corridor distance to and regional belonging of the target. Pointing latency increasedwith increasing distance to the target and when pointing into the other region. Further, alignment with local, regional and globalreference frames were found to facilitate pointing latency. These findings suggest that participants memorized local corridors,clustered corridors into regions, and also formed global reference frames, thus, represented the environment on multiple levelsof integration. They are inconsistent with conceptions of spatial memory for navigable environments based either on exclusiverepresentation within a single reference frame or exclusive reliance on local reference frames.

The Semantic Spaces of Child-Directed Speech, Child Speech and Adult-directed Speech: a Manifold Perspective

Child-directed speech (CDS) is a talking style adopted by caregivers when they talk to toddlers (Snow, 1995). We consider the role of distributional semantic features of CDS in language acquisition. We view semantic structure as a manifold on which words lie. We compare the semantic structure of verbs in CDS to the semantic structure of child speech (CS) and adult-directed speech (ADS) by measuring how easy it is to align the manifolds. We find that it is easier to align verbs in CS to CDS than to align CS to ADS, suggesting that the semantic structure of CDS is reflected in child productions. We also find, by measuring verbs vertex degrees in a semantic graph, that a mixed initialized set of verbs with high degrees and medium degrees has the best performance among all alignments, suggesting that both semantic generality and diversity may be important for developing semantic representations.

From Abstract to Concrete - Evidence for designing learning platforms that adapt to user's proficiencies.

Digital-tablets distribute cognition through visual, auditoryand haptic interactivity. We designed a tutor-game thatexplored how narratives ((S)trong/(W)eak) and gestures((I)conic/(D)eictic) could be combined to situate embodiedlearning. Students played seven levels of a fractions gamedesigned to teach them how to create and compare fractions.One hundred thirty-one students (N=131, age x̄ =8.78 yrs,52.6% Female) were randomly assigned to one of four groups(SI, SD, WI, WD) in a 2x2 factorial experiment. Studentscompleted pre/post direct and transfer assessments and tutor-game log data was mined to explore characteristics ofstudents learning. Results revealed a significant interactionbetween narrative and gesture moderated by studentproficiency. In effect, students new to fractions performedbetter in an abstract environment using deictic (pointing)gestures. However, as students' proficiencies improved, theylearned better using iconically enactive gestures in strongnarrative with setting, characters and a plot. This hasimportant implications for designing adaptive learningplatforms and curricula for teaching fractions.

Supporting Low-Performing Students by Manipulating Self-efficacy in Digital Tutees

Educational software based on teachable agents has repeatedly proven to have positive effects on students’ learning outcomes. The strongest effects have been shown for low-performers. A number of mechanisms have been proposed to explore this outcome, in particular mechanisms that involve attributions of social agency to teachable agents. Our study examined whether an expression of high versus low self-efficacy in a teachable agent would affect low- performing students with respect to their learning outcomes and with respect to a potential change in their own self- efficacy. The learning domain was mathematics, specifically the base-ten system. Results were that the learning outcomes of low-performers who taught a low self-efficacy agent were significantly better than the learning outcomes of low- performers who taught a high self-efficacy agent. There were no effects from the manipulation of self-efficacy expressed by the teachable agent on changes of the low-performing students’ own self-efficacy.

The impact of practice frequency on learning and retention

The current study manipulated how frequently different prob-lems were practiced during a first day of practice, with themore frequent items being more closely spaced. Fitting thedata to a skill acquisition model, we find that greater spac-ing between items is associated with an increased probabilityof transitioning to more efficient phases of performance, butwith a shallower speedup within each phase. Three days aftertraining, we find that performance is predicted not by the prac-tice frequency during training, but rather by the phase of skillacquisition attained during training. Thus, it is type of pro-cessing achieved not the amount and spacing of practice, thatdetermines retention. Spacing, however, promotes learning bydriving changes in cognitive processing.

Warm (for winter): Comparison class understanding in vague language

Speakers often refer to context only implicitly when using lan-guage. The utterance “it’s warm outside” could signal it’swarm relative to other days of the year or just relative to thecurrent season (e.g., it’s warm for winter). Warm vaguely con-veys that the temperature is high relative to some contextualcomparison class, but little is known about how a listener de-cides upon such a standard of comparison. Here, we formalizehow world knowledge and listeners’ internal models of speechproduction can drive the resolution of a comparison class incontext. We introduce a Rational Speech Act model and de-rive two novel predictions from it, which we validate using aparaphrase experiment to measure listeners’ beliefs about thelikely comparison class used by a speaker. Our model makesquantitative predictions given prior world knowledge for thedomains in question. We triangulate this knowledge with afollow-up language task in the same domains, using Bayesiandata analysis to infer priors from both data sets

Neural and computational arguments for memory as a compressed supportedtimeline

It is well known that, all things being equal, the accuracy ofmammalian timing and memory decays gradually with the pas-sage of time. The gradual decay of temporal accuracy is alsoobserved in single-unit neural recordings. Here we review re-cent modeling work describing a specific mechanism for tim-ing and memory and relevant neural data. The model describesa neural mechanism that can give rise to a logarithmically com-pressed representation of the recent past. We examine the spe-cific predictions of the model, in particular that the elapse oftime is represented by sequentially activated cells which firefor a circumscribed period of time. Such cells, called timecells, have been observed in neural recordings from severalbrain regions in multiple species. As predicted by the model,the cells show accuracy that decreases with time.

Picturing time: Children’s preferences for visual representations of events

English-speaking adults recruit a left-to-right “mentaltimeline” (MTL) when thinking about time. The origins of theMTL are debated, with some arguing that it is a culturalconstruct and others arguing that it is rooted in innateassociations between time and space. Here we ask whetherpreschoolers, with limited experience with cultural practicesthought to shape the MTL, prefer conventional linearrepresentations of temporal events. English-speakingpreschoolers and adults were told stories and asked to choosewhich of two visual representations best illustrated the story.As expected, adults overwhelmingly preferred images thatwere linearly ordered from left-to-right. Five-year-olds alsopreferred left-to-right to right-to-left series, but were equallylikely choose left-to-right and top-to-bottom. By contrast,3-year-olds chose at random, apparently insensitive to thespatial ordering of event-denoting images. These resultssuggest that attention to the ordinal structure of visualrepresentations of time increases across early childhood, andthat adults’ preference for horizontal space-time mappingsresults from increased cultural conditioning.

Computational Exploration of Lexical Development in Down Syndrome

Research on lexical development in Down syndrome (DS) has emphasized a dissociation between language comprehension and production abilities, with production of words being relatively more impaired than comprehension. Current theories stress the role of associative learning on lexical development. However, there have been no attempts to explain the atypical lexical development in DS based on atypical associative learning. The long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) of synapses, underlying associative learning, are altered in DS. Here we present a neural network model that instantiates notions from neurophysiological studies to account for the disparities between lexical comprehension and production in DS. Our simulations show that an atypical LTP/LTD balance affects comprehension and production differently in an associative model of lexical development.

The temporal dynamics of base rate neglect: People may not be intuitivestatisticians after all

According to a classic view of reasoning, intuition is fast but fallible, while reflection is slow but reliable. Biases,therefore, emerge when a reasoner’s intuitions are wrong and they fail to notice. Recent evidence, however, suggests thatpeople may be aware when their intuitions are incorrect. A possible explanation reason for this is that both correct and incorrectresponses are cued in parallel, but the strongly-cued incorrect response is given unless people can inhibit it. We tested thisexplanation using base rate neglect problems, and recorded participants’ mouse cursor movements as they chose betweenpossible answers under time pressure. Descriptions affected both participants’ early movements and ultimate responses, andinterfered with their use of the base rates, while base rates rarely interfered with participants’ use of descriptions, and then onlyat a later point in time. Thus, despite suggestive findings elsewhere, our results support the classic of view reasoning.

Promoting Spontaneous Analogical Transfer by Idealizing TargetRepresentations

Recent results demonstrate that inducing an abstractrepresentation of target analogs at retrieval time aids access toanalogous situations with mismatching surface features (i.e.,the late abstraction principle). A limitation of currentimplementations of this principle is that they either require theexternal provision of target-specific information or demandvery high intellectual engagement. Experiment 1 demonstratedthat constructing an idealized situation model of a targetproblem increases the rate of correct solutions compared toconstructing either concrete simulations or no simulations.Experiment 2 confirmed that these results were based on anadvantage for accessing the base analog, and not merely on anadvantage of idealized simulations for understanding the targetproblem in its own terms. This target idealization strategy hasbroader applicability than prior interventions based on the lateabstraction principle, because it can be achieved by a greaterproportion of participants and without the need to receivetarget-specific information.

It’s all in your head: Effects of expertise on real-time accessto knowledge during written sentence processing

Real-time sentence processing involves connecting linguistic inputwith knowledge. Here, we ask how variability in semantic memory(specific domain knowledge) may influence semantic access in real-time sentence processing. We recorded EEG while participantsmore/less knowledgeable about the narrative world of Harry Potter(HP) read sentences. In Experiment 1, all participants showed N400predictability effects for general-knowledge sentences, but onlythose with high HP knowledge showed predictability effects forsentences about Harry Potter. This effect was driven by graded brainresponses to predictable endings as a function of knowledge.Experiment 2 revealed greater semantic activation (inferred fromN400 effects) for HP items participants reported knowing. High-knowledge participants also showed greater semantic activation foritems they reported not knowing/remembering. These findingssuggest that amount and/or functional organization of knowledgehas real-time consequences on written sentence processing andimplicate implicit/partial access to domain knowledge for expertswhen information is not explicitly recalled.

Inhibitory Control Supports Referential Context Use in Language Production and Comprehension

Using referential context in language (e.g., saying “blue pen” when two different-colored pens are visible) makes communication efficient. But it is still unclear which general cognitive processes support the use of context in conversation. Research on pragmatic use in language implicates working memory and inhibitory control; however, no studies have shown evidence of a shared cognitive mechanism in both production and comprehension within an individual. The current study asked a) whether referential context use is supported by the same cognitive mechanisms in production and comprehension, b) which processes are implicated, and c) whether the nature of the context itself affects processing. Participants completed a referential communication eye-tracking task in which a disambiguating adjective was either necessary or over-informative, as well as a cognitive test battery. The results implicated inhibitory control in both production and comprehension (although the comprehension results were more variable), suggesting a shared underlying cognitive mechanism across domains.

A Dynamic Process Model for Predicting Workload in an Air Traffic ControllerTask

We present a dynamic process model for workload, developedaccording to a conducted experiment, which recorded the pupildilation during an air traffic controller simulation. We describehow we built such a dynamic system based on the collecteddata. Logged events that happened in our simulation were usedas system input and the recorded pupil dilation as output. Af-terwards, we used the MATLAB system identification toolboxto identify the transfer function between input and output. Theidentified model is validated with a validation data set that hasbeen excluded from the identification process. Results showthat we are able to explain nearly 50% of the variance of therecorded pupil dilation data in the air traffic controller simu-lation. Moreover, the model explains some contrary results ofthe statistical analysis from our experiment.

The Dynamics of Selective Integration during Rapid Experiential Decisions

When making decisions humans often violate the principlesof rational choice theory. Recent experiments, involving rapidexperiential decisions, uncovered a mechanism that is respon-sible for various rationality violations. According to this se-lective gating mechanism, incoming value samples are accu-mulated across time, but prior to their accumulation they areweighted in proportion to their momentary rank-order. Here,using a data-driven approach, I present a dynamic extensionof this mechanism, which involves potentially asymmetric in-hibition between the inputs. As a result, and contrary to theprevious selective gating implementation, the vigour of gatingis modulated by the difference between two value samples (adistance effect) as well as by the absolute magnitude of thesamples (a magnitude effect). This extension offers a supe-rior explanation to existing and new data; and links high-leveldecision phenomena with computational principles previouslydescribed in theories of selective attention and visual search.

Cake or Hat? Words Change How Young Children Process Visual Objects

A large literature shows that language influences cognition.Yet, we know very little about when and how linguisticinfluences on cognition become important in development.Here we test the proposal that one pathway by whichlanguage affects cognition is by activating categoryinformation which influences visual processing, and that thisinfluence starts early. Across two experiments, we show thatcategory information affects visual processing and that wordscan activate category information in young children.

Looking for the Cat and Seeing the Dog:Using Visual Search to Study Semantic Knowledge in Children

Semantic knowledge influences various higher-ordercognitive processes; therefore, it is important to understandhow it changes with development. The Match-to-Sample taskis perhaps the most common paradigm for studying changesin semantic knowledge over development, yet this paradigmhas a number of limitations. Here we provide initial evidencevalidating a Visual Search paradigm as a measure of semanticknowledge in preschoolers, and discuss the potential of thisparadigm to address the limitations posed by the Match-to-Sample task to study semantic knowledge development.

Interactivity and Ego Depletion in Insight Problem Solving

In the triangle of coins problem coins are arranged tocreate a triangle pointing down and the solution involvesmoving a few coins to change its orientation. The taskecology can be designed such that participants can workon it in a low interactivity environment, maintaining amental representation of simulated moves, or in a highinteractivity environment, thinking with and through aphysical model of the problem. These task ecologiesinvolve working memory to a different degree: Problemsolving draws more on working memory the lower thedegree of physical interaction. Participants first engagedin a writing task that required vigilance to inhibitcommon word choices, a degree of self regulationdesigned to induce a so-called ego depletion;participants then worked on the ToC problem in either alow or high interactivity environment. Solution rateswere determined by level of interactivity; the precedingdepletion experience did not impact performance.

A computational model for decision tree search

How do people plan ahead in sequential decision-makingtasks? In this article, we compare computational models of hu-man behavior in a challenging variant of tic-tac-toe, to inves-tigate the cognitive processes underlying sequential planning.We validate the most successful model by predicting choicesduring games, two-alternative forced choices and board evalu-ations. We then use this model to study individual skill differ-ences, the effects of time pressure and the nature of expertise.Our findings suggest that people perform less tree search un-der time pressure, and that players search more as they improveduring learning.

Approximations of Predictive Entropy Correlate with Reading Times

The lexical frequency of an upcoming word affects read-ing times even when the upcoming word is masked fromreaders (Angele et al., 2015). One explanation for thisobservation is that readers may slow down if there is highuncertainty about upcoming material. In line with thishypothesis, this study finds a positive correlation be-tween predictive entropy and self-paced reading times.This study also demonstrates that such predictive en-tropy can be effectively approximated by the surprisalof upcoming observations and that this future surprisalestimate is more predictive of reading times when thegrammar is more granular, which would be prohibitivelyexpensive for predictive entropy. These results suggestreaders engage in fine-grained predictive estimations ofcertainty about upcoming lexical and syntactic material,that such predictions influence reading times, and thatestimating that uncertainty can be done less expensivelyand more robustly with information-theoretic surprisal.

The Development of Structural Thinking about Social Categories

Representations of social categories help us make sense of thesocial world, supporting predictions and explanations aboutgroups and individuals. Here we explore whether children andadults are able to understanding category-property associationsin structural terms, locating an object of explanation within alarger structure and identifying structural constraints that acton elements of the structure. We show that children as young3-4 years of age show signs of structural thinking, but that thiscapacity does not fully develop until after 7 years of age. Thesefindings introduce a viable alternative to internalist accounts ofsocial categories, such as psychological essentialism.

The statistical significance filter leads to overconfident expectations of replicability

We show that publishing results using the statistical signif-icance filter—publishing only when the p-value is less than0.05—leads to a vicious cycle of overoptimistic expectationof the replicability of results. First, we show analytically thatwhen true statistical power is relatively low, computing powerbased on statistically significant results will lead to overesti-mates of power. Then, we present a case study using 10 exper-imental comparisons drawn from a recently published meta-analysis in psycholinguistics (J ̈ager et al., 2017). We show thatthe statistically significant results yield an illusion of replica-bility. This illusion holds even if the researcher doesn’t con-duct any formal power analysis but just uses statistical signifi-cance to informally assess robustness (i.e., replicability) of re-sults.

Modelling dependency completion in sentence comprehensionas a Bayesian hierarchical mixture process:A case study involving Chinese relative clauses

We present a case-study demonstrating the usefulness ofBayesian hierarchical mixture modelling for investigating cog-nitive processes. In sentence comprehension, it is widely as-sumed that the distance between linguistic co-dependents af-fects the latency of dependency resolution: the longer thedistance, the longer the retrieval time (the distance-based ac-count). An alternative theory, direct-access, assumes that re-trieval times are a mixture of two distributions: one distribu-tion represents successful retrievals (these are independent ofdependency distance) and the other represents an initial failureto retrieve the correct dependent, followed by a reanalysis thatleads to successful retrieval. We implement both models asBayesian hierarchical models and show that the direct-accessmodel explains Chinese relative clause reading time data betterthan the distance account.

Interpreting actions by attributing compositional desires

We cannot see others’ mental states, so we infer them by watching how people behave. Bayesian inference in a model of rational action – called inverse planning – captures how humans infer desires from observable actions. These models represent desires as simple associations between agents and world states. In this paper we show that by representing desires as probabilistic programs, an inverse planning model can infer complex desires underlying complex behaviors—desires with temporal and logical structure, which can be fulfilled in different ways. Our model, which combines basic desires via logical primitives, is inspired by recent probabilistic grammar- based models of concept learning. Through an experiment where we vary behaviors parametrically, we show that our model predicts with high accuracy how people infer complex desires. Our work sheds light on the representations underlying mental states, and paves the way towards algorithms that can reason about others’ minds as we do.

Non-Symbolic Exact Quantity Representation in a Language Impaired Population

English-speakers whose access to number language isartificially compromised by verbal interference and the Pirahã(an Amazonian tribe without exact number words) appear torely on analog magnitude estimation for representing non-symbolic exact quantities greater than 3. Here, 16 participantswith aphasia performed the 5 counting tasks from theseprevious studies. Performance was poorest when targets werenot visible during response (70% correct, task 4; 71% correct,task 5) and best when targets were presented as subitizablegroups of 2 and 3 (98% correct, task 2). Western AphasiaBattery-Revised subtest scores correlated with taskperformance, suggesting diverse forms of languageimpairment may contribute to errors. Coefficients of variationfor tasks and significant correlations of target magnitude witherror rate (r 2=.88) and error size (r 2=.87) across tasks suggestparticipant use of analog magnitude estimation. Experimentsinvolving people with aphasia may further refine ourunderstanding of how language and thought interact.

Audiovisual integration is affected by performing a task jointly

Humans constantly receive sensory input from several sensorymodalities. Via the process of multisensory integration, this inputis often integrated into a unitary percept. Researchers haveinvestigated several factors that could affect the process ofmultisensory integration. However, in this field of research, socialfactors (i.e., whether a task is performed alone or jointly) havebeen widely neglected. Using an audiovisual crossmodalcongruency task we investigated whether social factors affectaudiovisual integration. Pairs of participants received congruent orincongruent audiovisual stimuli and were required to indicate theelevation of these stimuli. We found that the reaction time cost ofresponding to incongruent stimuli (relative to congruent stimuli)was reduced significantly when participants performed the taskjointly compared to when they performed the task alone. Theseresults extend earlier findings on visuotactile integration byshowing that audiovisual integration is also affected by socialfactors.

More than meets the eye: Early relational reasoning cannot be reduced to perceptual heuristics

The ability to represent same-different relations is a condition for abstract thought. However, there is mixed evidence for when this ability develops, both ontogenetically and phylogenetically. Apparent success in relational reasoning may be evidence for conceptual understanding or may be due to low-level, perceptual strategies. We introduce a method to discriminate these possibilities by pitting two conditions that are perceptually matched but conceptually different: in a “fused” condition, same and different objects are joined, creating single objects that have the same perceptual features as the pairs in the “relational” condition. However, the “fused” objects do not provide evidence for the relation. Using this method in a causal task provides evidence for genuine conceptual understanding. This novel technique offers a simple manipulation that may be applied to a variety of existing match-to-sample procedures used to assess same-different reasoning to include in future research with non-human animals, as well as human infants.

Simultaneous acquisition of vocabulary and grammar in an artificial language learning task

Learning syntax requires determining relations between the grammatical categories of words in the language, but learning those categories requires understanding the role of words in the syntax. In this study, we examined how this chicken and egg problem is resolved by learners of an artificial language comprising nouns, verbs, adjectives and case markers following syntactic rules. We found that the language could be acquired through cross-situational statistical correspondences with complex scenes and without explicit feedback, and that knowledge was maintained after 24 hours. Results also showed that verbs and word order were the first to be acquired, followed by nouns, adjectives and finally case markers. Interdependencies in learning were found for word order and verbs, and also for nouns, adjectives and case markers. Grammar and vocabulary can be acquired simultaneously, but with distinctive patterns of acquisition – grammar and the role of verbs first, then the vocabulary of other lexical categories.

Please Explain: Radical Enactivism and its Explanatory Debt

Radical Enactivism is a position in the philosophy of cognitive science that aims to displace representationalism, the dominant position in cognitive science for the last 50-60 years. To accomplish this aim, radical enactivism must provide an alternative explanation of cognition. Radical enactivism offers two alternative explanations of cognition. The first I call the dynamical explanation and the second I call the historical explanation. The mechanists have given us reasons for doubting that the first alternative makes for a good explanation. The historical explanation does not hit the right explanatory target without the introduction of a proximate mechanism, but the proximate mechanisms suggested by radical enactivism are associationist mechanisms, the limitations of which led to the initial widespread endorsement of representationalism. Therefore, radical enactivism cannot displace representationalism in cognitive science.

Learning to reinforcement learn

In recent years deep reinforcement learning (RL) systems have attained superhuman performance in a number ofchallenging task domains, but are constrained by a demand for large training sets. A critical present objective is thus to developdeep RL methods that can adapt rapidly to new tasks. In the present work we introduce a novel approach to this challenge,which we refer to as deep meta-reinforcement learning. Previous work has shown that recurrent networks can support meta-learning in a fully supervised context. We extend this approach to the RL setting. What emerges is a system that is trainedusing one RL algorithm, but whose recurrent dynamics implement a second, quite separate RL procedure. This second, learnedRL algorithm can differ from the original one in arbitrary ways and exploit structure in the training domain. We unpack thesepoints in five proof-of-concept experiments to examine key aspects of deep meta-RL.

The Learning of Subordinate Word Meanings

In three experiments, adults attempted to learn words with subordinate-level meanings (dalmatian) by sampling thereferent world cross-situationally. Xu & Tenenbaum, 2007 predicted that encountering three uses of a word, each referringto a dalmatian would evoke “suspicious coincidence” inferencing, leading to the subordinate meaning (dalmatian). Exp. 1found little evidence for this; cross-situational exposure led to a basic-level bias. This bias was unchanged even when thesample was increased to five subordinate exemplars (Exp. 2). Exp. 3 encouraged semantic contrast by simultaneously teachingeach subject a word for the subordinate-level and the basic-level category within the same semantic domain (dap=dalmatian;blit=dog). Participants now showed non-basic level learning, but more in line with mutual exclusivity: they may think “dap”means dalmatian but “blit” means all-dogs-except-dalmatians. We conclude that the basic-level interpretation is powerful andcannot be removed by the mere observation of exemplar items over multiple word instances.

Anticipatory Synchronization in Artificial Agents

By integrating theories and methodologies from a diverserange of scientific disciplines (e.g., physics, neuroscience,cognitive science, psychology and robotics engineering) thepresent work is aimed at harnessing self-organizedanticipatory synchronization in order to advance human-robotic interaction (HRI). This phenomenon is characterizedby the emergence of anticipatory behavior by one systemcoupled to the chaotic behavior of another, following theintroduction of short self-referential delays in the coordinatingsystem. The current set of studies involved the creation of anartificial agent based on a time-delayed, low-dimensionaldynamical model capable of behaving prospectively during aninteraction with a human actor performing complex,unpredictable behaviors. By achieving characteristics similarto those observed during natural human interaction andcoordination, the time-delayed modeling approachedadvocated here provides the potential for considerable futureadvancements in HRI.

Structure Learning in Motor Control:A Deep Reinforcement Learning Model

Motor adaptation displays a structure-learning effect: adapta-tion to a new perturbation occurs more quickly when the sub-ject has prior exposure to perturbations with related structure.Although this ‘learning-to-learn’ effect is well documented, itsunderlying computational mechanisms are poorly understood.We present a new model of motor structure learning, approach-ing it from the point of view of deep reinforcement learning.Previous work outside of motor control has shown how recur-rent neural networks can account for learning-to-learn effects.We leverage this insight to address motor learning, by import-ing it into the setting of model-based reinforcement learning.We apply the resulting processing architecture to empiricalfindings from a landmark study of structure learning in target-directed reaching (Braun et al., 2009), and discuss its implica-tions for a wider range of learning-to-learn phenomena.

Children’s intuitions about the structure of mental life

We investigated children’s understanding of mental life byanalyzing attributions of perceptual, cognitive, affective, andother capacities. 200 children (7-9y) and 200 adults evaluatedthe mental capacities of beetles or robots. By assessing whichcapacities traveled together when participants disagreed aboutthese controversial “edge cases,” we reconstructed the latentstructure underlying mental capacity judgments from thebottom up—a novel approach to elucidating conceptualstructure among children. For both children and adults, factoranalyses revealed a distinction between social-emotional,physiological, and perceptual-cognitive capacities, hinting atthree fundamental ways of explaining and predicting others’actions: as social partners, biological creatures, and goal-directed agents (each involving related forms of both“experience” and “agency”; Gray et al., 2007). Relative toadults, children attributed greater social-emotional capacitiesto beetles and robots, suggesting that intuitive ontologies ofmental life could be critical for making sense of children’sdeveloping understanding of the social world.

The Effect of expertise and biscriptalism on letter perception: The complexity benefit

Previous work has demonstrated that the visual complexity ofletter-shapes is processed differently by naïve and expertobservers. Specifically, fluent readers of the Arabic alphabetwere found to discriminate complex letters more readily thanless complex letters, whereas naïve observers exhibited theopposite effect. This “complexity benefit”, wherein complexletters confer a processing advantage to expert observers, isnot yet well understood. In a new study, we investigatewhether this effect generalizes across scripts, and whether itis unique to individuals with biscriptal experience (knowledgeof reading two different scripts). The results of the threeexperiments confirm that the complexity benefit ischaracteristic of expert monoscriptal and biscriptal readers,and that, furthermore, there may be a biscriptal advantage inprocessing visual complexity.

Examining Multiscale Movement Coordination in Collaborative Problem Solving

During collaborative problem solving (CPS), coordination occurs at different spatial and temporal scales. This multiscale coordination should, at least on some scales, play a functional role in facilitating effective collaboration outcomes. To evaluate this, we conducted a study of computer-based CPS with 42 dyads. We used cross-wavelet coherence as a way to examine the degree to which movement coordination is evident at a variety of scales and tested whether the observed coordination was greater than both the amount expected due to chance and due to task demands. We found that coordination at scales less than 2s was greater than expected due to chance and at most scales (except 16s, 1m, and 2m) was greater than expected due to task demands. Lastly, we evaluated whether the degree of coherence at scales less than 2s, and the form of coordination (in terms of relative phase), were predictive of CPS performance. We found that .25s and 1s scales were predictive of performance. When including relative phase, our results suggest that higher in-phase movement coordination at the 1s scale was the strongest predictor of CPS performance. We discuss these findings and detail their relevance for expanding our knowledge on how coordination facilitates CPS.

A Computational Model for Constructing Preferences for Multiple Choice Options

When choosing between multiple alternatives, people usuallydo not have ready-made preferences in their mind but ratherconstruct them on the go. The 2N-ary Choice Tree Model(Wollschlaeger & Diederich, 2012) proposes a preference con-struction process for N choice options from description, whichis based on attribute weights, differences between attribute val-ues, and noise. It is able to produce similarity, attraction,and compromise effects, which have become a benchmark formulti-alternative choice models, but also several other contextand reference point effects. Here, we present a new and math-ematically tractable version of the model – the Simple ChoiceTree Model – which also explains the above mentioned effectsand additionally accounts for the positive correlation betweenthe attraction and compromise effect, and the negative correla-tion between these two and the similarity effect as observed byBerkowitsch, Scheibehenne, and Rieskamp (2014).

Mapping the unknown: The spatially correlated multi-armed bandit

We introduce the spatially correlated multi-armed banditas a task coupling function learning with the exploration-exploitation trade-off. Participants interacted with bi-variatereward functions on a two-dimensional grid, with the goal ofeither gaining the largest average score or finding the largestpayoff. By providing an opportunity to learn the underly-ing reward function through spatial correlations, we modelto what extent people form beliefs about unexplored payoffsand how that guides search behavior. Participants adapted toassigned payoff conditions, performed better in smooth thanin rough environments, and—surprisingly—sometimes per-formed equally well in short as in long search horizons. Ourmodeling results indicate a preference for local search options,which when accounted for, still suggests participants werebest-described as forming local inferences about unexploredregions, combined with a search strategy that directly tradedoff between exploiting high expected rewards and exploring toreduce uncertainty about the spatial structure of rewards.

What do you really think? Children’s ability to infer others’ desires whenemotional expressions change between social and nonsocial contexts

We investigate children’s ability to use social display rules toinfer agents’ otherwise under-determined desires. InExperiment 1, seven-to-ten-year-olds saw a protagonistexpress one emotional reaction to an event in front of hersocial partner (the Social Context), and a different expressionbehind her social partner’s back (the Nonsocial Context).Children were able to use the expression in the Social Contextto infer the social partner’s desire and the expression in theNonsocial Context to infer the protagonist’s desire. Thisability increased between ages seven and ten (Experiment 1).When task demands were reduced (Experiment 2), seven-to-eight-year-olds, but not five-to-six-year-olds, succeeded.These results suggest that although it is not easy for observersto infer emotions masked by social display rules, changingemotional expressions between social and non-social contextsallow even children to recover not only the desire of theperson displaying the emotions, but also that of the audience.

Discovering Multicausality in the Development of Coordinated Behavior

Human interaction involves the organization of a collection ofsensorimotor systems across space and time. The study ofhow coordination develops in child-parent interaction hasprimarily focused on understanding the development ofspecific coordination patterns from individual modalities.However, less work has taken a systems view andinvestigated the development of coordination among multipleinterdependent behaviors. In the present work, we usedGranger causality as a mathematical model to constructdyadic causal networks of multimodal data collected from alongitudinal study of child-parent interaction. At a group-level, we observed increases in the number of causal links andin the strength of such links in dyadic interaction from 9-months to 12-months. At an individual-level, we observedhigh variability in the types of causal links that emergedacross developmental ages. We discuss these results in termsof a multicausality hypothesis for the development of humancoordination.

Unifying recommendation and active learning for human-algorithm interactions

The enormous scale of the available information and productson the Internet has necessitated the development of algorithmsthat intermediate between options and human users. These al-gorithms do not select information at random, but attempt toprovide the user with relevant information. In doing so, thealgorithms may incur potential negative consequences relatedto, for example, “filter bubbles.” Building from existing al-gorithms, we introduce a parametrized model that unifies andinterpolates between recommending relevant information andactive learning. In a concept learning paradigm, we illustratethe trade-offs of optimizing prediction and recommendation,show that there is a broad parameter region of stable perfor-mance that optimizes for both, identify a specific regime thatis most robust to human variability, and identify the cause ofthis optimized performance. We conclude by discussing im-plications for the cognitive science of concept learning and thepractice of machine learning in the real world.

A non-parametric Bayesian prior for causal inference of auditory streaming

Human perceptual grouping of sequential auditory cues hastraditionally been modeled using a mechanistic approach. Theproblem however is essentially one of source inference – aproblem that has recently been tackled using statisticalBayesian models in visual and auditory-visual modalities.Usually the models are restricted to performing inference overjust one or two possible sources, but human perceptualsystems have to deal with much more complex scenarios. Tocharacterize human perception we have developed a Bayesianinference model that allows an unlimited number of signalsources to be considered: it is general enough to allow anydiscrete sequential cues, from any modality. The model uses anon-parametric prior, hence increased complexity of thesignal does not necessitate more parameters. The model notonly determines the most likely number of sources, but alsospecifies the source that each signal is associated with. Themodel gives an excellent fit to data from an auditory streamsegregation experiment in which the pitch and presentationrate of pure tones determined the perceived number ofsources.

Perceived similarity mediates violations of independence in probabilisticjudgments

We outline a simple way of representing sets of non-normativejudgements that makes them look as similar as possible to nor-mative ones. This representation allows us to view certaintypes of non-normative judgments, such as conjunction falla-cies, as arising from a misestimation of the correlation betweenevents, that might arise when decision-makers have no priorinformation about the frequency of co-occurrence. We sug-gest that decision-makers use the perceived similarity betweenevents to make inferences about correlation, and we describethe results of an experiment showing that judged correlationand violations of independence in probabilistic judgments arestrongly influenced by the perceived similarity between events.

Causal and compositional generative models in online perception

From a quick glance or the touch of an object, our brains map sensory signals to scenes composed of rich anddetailed shapes and surfaces. Unlike the standard approaches to perception, we argue that this mapping draws on internalcausal and compositional models of the physical world and these internal models underlie the generalization capacity of humanperception. Here, we present a generative model of visual and multisensory perception in which the latent variables encodeintrinsic (e.g., shape) and extrinsic (e.g., occlusion) object properties. Latent variables are inputs to causal models that outputsense-specific signals. We present a recognition network that performs efficient inference in the generative model, computingat a speed similar to online perception. We show that our model, but not alternatives, can account for human performance in anoccluded face matching task and in a visual-to-haptic face matching task.

Individual Differences in Gaze Dynamics in Risky Decision-making

In risky decision-making, expected utility (EU) theory is widely used to examine people's risk attitude and choice behavior. However, it is unknown how risk attitude relates to attention and information search. In this paper, we explore the relationship between risk attitude (as measured by a variant of EU) and eye movement patterns (which serve as a proxy for attention and information search). Participants made choices between gambles presented perceptually as flickering grids in which monetary values were indicated by colors and probabilities by color proportions. To explore attention and information search patterns, we investigated eye movement patterns when faced with different gambles and correlated these patterns with the parameters of EU. We observed that people who are more risk-seeking (as determined by modeling) tend to look at risky options more often. These results bridge choice behaviors conceptualized by EU and information search strategies under risky decision-making revealed by eye movements.

Novel Evidence for the Bilingual Advantage: Effects of Language Control onExecutive Function in Balanced and Unbalanced Dual-Language Users

Bilinguals’ need to monitor and inhibit non-relevantlanguages over a relevant one confers advantage in cognitivecontrol. No studies have demonstrated that the dual-languagecontrol process directly contributes to the bilingual cognitiveadvantage. We utilized a novel language control manipulationparadigm where 83 English-Chinese bilingual adultscompleted a reading and comprehension task in either single-language (low-language-control) or dual-language (high-language-control) prior to performing nonverbal executivecontrol tasks (Stroop, task-switching, and n-back). Resultsshowed that language control had significant effects onsubsequent cognitive performance, depending on whether theparticipants were regular dual language users or not. In thedual-language condition, but not the single-languagecondition, participants who used both languages regularlydemonstrated a smaller mixing cost in task-switching and agreater sensitivity in n-back detection compared toparticipants who did not. This suggests that dual languagecontrol utilizes similar resources as executive function andfrequent dual language use enhances this resource.

Inconvenient samples: Modeling the effects of non-consent by coupling observational and experimental results

Biased sampling of participants presents a major limiting factor for the generalizability of findings from behavioral studies. This effect may be especially pronounced in developmental studies, where parents serve as both the primary environmental input and decide whether their child participates in a study. To estimate the effects of parental non-consent, we coupled naturalistic observations of parent-child interactions with a behavioral test. Results showed that one particular parenting practice, the tendency to use questions to teach, associated with both children’s behavior in the test and parents’ tendencies to participate. Exploiting these associations with a model-based multiple imputation, we estimated that the means of the consented and not-consented groups could differ as much as 0.2 standard deviations for five of the seven test measurements we used, and standard deviations are likely underestimated. These results suggest that ignoring the role of consent may lead to systematic biases when generalizing beyond lab samples.

Seeing Is Not Enough for Sustained Visual Attention

Sustained visual attention is crucial to many developmentaloutcomes. We demonstrate that, consistent with thedevelopmental systems view, sustained visual attentionemerges from and is tightly tied to sensory motor coordination.We examined whether changes in manual behavior altertoddlers’ eye gaze by giving one group of children heavy toysthat were hard to pick up, while giving another group ofchildren perceptually identical toys that were lighter, easy topick up and hold. We found a tight temporal coupling betweenthe dynamics of visual attention and the dynamics of manualactivities on objects, a relation that cannot be explained byinterest alone. In the Heavy condition, toddlers looked atobjects just as much as did toddlers in the Light condition butdid so through many brief glances, whereas in Light conditionlooks to the objects were longer and sustained. We discuss theimplication of hand-eye coordination in the development ofvisual attention.

Mindshaping the world can make mindreading tractable: Bridging the gap between philosophy and computational complexity analysis

It is often assumed that the socio-cultural context positively influences mindreading performances. Among the available theories, mindshaping is proposed to consist of cultural mechanisms that make the social domain homogeneous and, hence, easier to interpret. Proponents of the mindshaping hypothesis claim that homogeneity is responsible for the computational tractability of mindreading, which is otherwise intractable. In this paper, we examine this core claim of mindshaping and investigate how homogeneity influences mindreading tractability. By taking action understanding as a case-study for mindreading, we formally operationalize mindshaping homogeneity in different ways with the goal of bridging the gap between informal claims and formal (in)tractability results. The analysis shows that only specific combinations of homogeneity may lead to tractable mindreading, whilst others do not. Additionally, the analysis reveals the possibility of a yet undiscovered mindshaping mechanism.

Using mouse-tracking data to visualise decision landscapes

Computerised paradigms have enabled decision making re-searchers to gather rich data on human behaviour, includinginformation on motor execution of a decision, e.g., by track-ing mouse cursor trajectories. As the number and complexityof mouse-tracking studies rapidly increase, more sophisticatedmethodology is needed to analyse the decision trajectories.Here we present a new computational approach to generat-ing decision landscape visualisations based on mouse-trackingdata. Decision landscape is an analogue of energy potentialfield mathematically derived from velocity of mouse move-ment during a decision. Visualised as a 3D surface, it pro-vides a comprehensive overview of motor evolution of deci-sions. Employing the dynamical systems theory framework,we develop a new method for generating decision landscapesbased on arbitrary number of trajectories. The decision land-scape visualisation have potential to become a novel tool foranalysing mouse trajectories during decision execution, whichcan provide new insights into the dynamics of decision mak-ing.

Insomniacs Misidentify Angry Faces as Fearful Faces Because of Missing the Eyes: An Eye-Tracking Study

Insomniacs were found to have compromised perception of facial expressions. Through eye movement examinations, here we test the hypothesis that this effect is due to impaired visual attention functions for retrieving diagnostic features in facial expression judgments. 23 individuals with insomnia symptoms and 23 non-insomniac controls completed a task to categorize happy, sad, fearful, and angry faces. The insomniacs were less accurate to recognize angry faces and made more “fearful” mistakes than controls. A hidden Markov modeling approach for eye movement data analysis revealed that when recognizing angry faces, more insomniacs adopted an eye movement pattern focusing on the mouth while more controls adopted a pattern attending to both the eyes and the mouth. This result is consistent with previous findings that the primary diagnostic feature for recognizing angry faces is the eyes suggesting that impaired information selection through visual attention control may account for the compromised emotion perception in insomniac individuals.

Low Dimensional Representations in Multi-Cue Judgment

The study of multi-cue judgment investigates how decisionmakers integrate cues to predict the value of a criterionvariable. We consider a multi-cue judgment task in whichdecision makers have prior knowledge of inter-cuerelationships but are ignorant of how the cues correlate withthe criterion. In this setting, a naive judgment strategyprescribes an equal weight for each cue. However, we findthat many participants appear to use a weighting schemebased on a low-dimensional representation of the cue space.The use of such a representation is consistent with coreinsights in semantic memory research and has importantoptimality properties concerning judgment accuracy.

Talks: Publication-Based

Using single unit recordings in PDP and localist models to better understand how knowledge is coded in the cortex

There is long history of studies documenting that some neurons respond to images of objects, faces, and scenes in a highly selective manner. This includes neurons in the human hippocampus (e.g., the famous example of a neuron responding to images of the actress Jennifer Aniston) and neurons in high-level visual cortex in monkey (for reviews see Bowers, 2009; Ison, Quian Quiroga, & Fried, 2015). These findings have led to a growing interest in the claim that some neurons code for information in a localist (‘grandmother cell’) manner, as reflected in the many contributions to a recent special issue on this topic in the journal Language, Cognition, & Neuroscience (Bowers, 2017). By contrast, it is only recently that interest in characterizing the selectivity of single units in connectionist networks has gathered speed. Critically, these studies also show that networks learn highly selective representations under a number of conditions, as detailed below. In this talk I will summarize recent research in my lab that explores the conditions in which artificial networks learn selective codes, and research comparing the responses of selective neurons and localist representations used in cognitive models. These findings suggest when and why some neurons in cortex respond in a highly selective manner, and highlight the biological plausibility of localist models in psychology.

Interoception: The Forgotten Modality in Perceptual Grounding of Concepts

Concepts are the basis of the human cognitive system, andthe question of what constitutes the content of these mentalrepresentations has long occupied the cognitive sciences.Work in psychology, linguistics and cognitive neurosciencehas converged on the idea that we develop our conceptualrepresentations through our perception of and interactionwith our environment. To date, such research has typicallyrestricted consideration to the perceptual modalities ofvision, touch, sound, taste, and smell. However, there isanother major modality of perceptual information that isdistinct from these traditional five senses; that is,interoception, or sensations within the body. In this paper,we explore the role of interoception in the perceptualgrounding of concepts.Recently, modality-specific measures of the strength ofperceptual experience (Lynott & Connell 2009, 2013) haveproven themselves important predictors of human behaviourin a range of conceptual tasks including word recognitionand reading (Connell & Lynott, 2010, 2012, 2014a, 2014b,2015, 2016). In a megastudy of over 32,000 words fromacross the abstract-concrete spectrum, we asked people toprovide modality-specific ratings of perceptual strength forsix modalities: the usual five (auditory, haptic, gustatory,olfactory, visual) plus the new category of interoceptivestrength. We found that interoceptive information dominatesthe perceptual profile of a sizeable number of concepts (9%;e.g., hangover, eternal, remorse), less than the proportion ofconcepts dominated by vision (74%; e.g., book) or sound(12%; e.g., melody), but more than are dominated by touch(3%; e.g., silky), gustation (2%; e.g., candy), or olfaction(<1%; e.g., bleach). Using principal components analysis toexamine how interoception relates to the other perceptualmodalities, we found that it tends to be strongly loadedagainst visual and haptic strength (i.e., that which is sensedwithin the body can be neither seen nor touched) but isrelatively distinct from sound, taste, and smell.Finally, we tested whether interoceptive strength offersvaluable information to conceptual content by examining itsrole in semantic facilitation of word recognition. Maximumperceptual strength (i.e., strength in the dominant modality)has previously been shown to predict word recognitionperformance better than concreteness or imageability(Connell & Lynott, 2012). We therefore compared thepredictive ability of two different versions of maximumperceptual strength: the original measure based on fivetraditional modalities, and a new version based on sixmodalities including interoceptive strength. In a regressionanalysis of lexical decision and word naming performance,interoceptive information considerably improved theefficacy of maximum perceptual strength in predicting bothresponse time and accuracy (Bayes Factors ranged fromBF 10 = 3.303×10 7 to BF 10 = 3.059×10 16 ). That is,perceptually strong words were recognized more quicklyand accurately than perceptually weak words, andinteroceptive strength was a valuable component in thisperceptual facilitation. Overall, these findings suggest thatinteroception has comparable status to other modalities incontributing to the perceptual grounding of concepts.

A Unified Model of Entropy and the Value of Information

Notions of entropy and uncertainty are fundamental tomany domains, ranging from the philosophy of science tophysics. One important application is to quantify theexpected usefulness of possible experiments (or questions ortests). Many different entropy models could be used;different models do not in general lead to the sameconclusions about which tests (or experiments) are mostvaluable. It is often unclear whether this is due to differenttheoretical and practical goals or are merely due to historicalaccident. We introduce a unified two-parameter family ofentropy models that incorporates a great deal of entropies asspecial cases. This family of models offers insight intoheretofore perplexing psychological results, and generatespredictions for future research.

The role of learning mechanisms in understanding spoken words

Word meaning priming has become a key method to studyhow listeners (and readers) retune their lexical semanticrepresentations in response to their linguistic environment inorder to facilitate access to word meanings. We present asummary of recent findings using this method that help toconstrain our theories of how this important form of lexical-semantic learning occurs.

Far Transfer: Does it Exist?

Implementing interventions that are supposed to enhance students’general learning skill and overall cognitive ability is still acommon practice in education. The basic idea on which thisapproach relies is that improving domain-general skills providesbenefits for a broad range of domain-specific areas, such asacademic disciplines. Thus, it is assumed that there is far transfer –i.e., the generalization of a set of skills between domains looselyrelated to each other. In recent years, chess instruction, musicinstruction, and working memory training have been claimed to beable to train domain-general abilities (e.g., fluidreasoning/intelligence) which, in turn, generalize to other cognitiveand academic skills (e.g., mathematics). We tested these claims inthe population of healthy children via meta-analysis. The resultsshowed small to moderate overall far-transfer effects in all theoutcome measures of the three meta-analyses. However, the effectsizes were inversely related to the design quality (e.g., presence ofactive control groups), which casts doubts on the effectiveness ofthe three activities. We discuss the theoretical and practicalimplications of these findings for education and expertise andextend the debate to another type of training, video games training.

Posters: Papers

The effects of gesture restriction on spatial language in young and elderly adults

There is contradictory evidence on whether speech production gets impaired or enhanced when people are restrained from gesturing. There is also very little research on how this effect can change with aging. The present study sought evidence for these by asking young and elderly adults to describe two different routes on a map in spontaneous speech and when gestures were prohibited. We found that elderly adults produced more spatial language when they were restricted to use gestures compared to their spontaneous speech, whereas young adults produced comparable levels of spatial language in both conditions. Young and elderly adults used comparable levels of gestures in their spontaneous route descriptions. Yet, only young adults’ gesture use correlated positively with their spatial language production. Thus, the results of gesture prohibition on speech production are different for young and elderly adults.

The Effects of Duration Words and Spatial-Temporal Metaphors on Perceived Duration

Subjective duration estimates are positively related to the magnitude of various non-temporal stimuli (e.g. Xuan et al., 2007). Our study investigated whether temporal and spatial magnitude information conveyed by linguistic stimuli would affect perceived duration in a temporal reproduction task. We used time-related words referring to different exact durations (e.g. second; Experiment 1), and spatial-temporal metaphors (e.g. long), referring to indistinct temporal as well as spatial magnitudes (Experiment 2). In both experiments, participants over-reproduced the shorter target duration (2.4 s) and under- reproduced the longer target duration (4.8 s). In Experiment 1, participants under-reproduced the longer target duration more when they saw “week” in the training and “year” in the reproduction. Yet, we did not observe the same semantic magnitude effect in other word pairs either in Experiment 1 or 2. Overall, we did not find supporting evidence for magnitude information conveyed by language affecting subjective time estimates.

Action and actor gaze mismatch effects during spoken sentence processing

Eye tracking research on situated language comprehension hasshown that participants rely more on a recent event than on aplausible future event during spoken sentence comprehension.When people saw a recent action event and then they listenedto a German (NP1-Verb-Adv-NP2) past or futuric present tensesentence, they preferentially looked at the recent event targetover another plausible target object (that might be involvedin a future action) independent of tense. This preferential in-spection persisted even when future events and futuric presentsentences were much more frequent within the experiment,or when a gaze cue biased towards the future action target.The present experiments extend this line of research by intro-ducing incongruence (in Experiment 1 a past tense verb mis-matched the recently seen action and in Experiment 2 an actorgaze cue mismatched the past tense sentence condition). Canthe verb-action and the gaze-sentence mismatches eliminatethe recent-event inspection preference? Would participants re-call information in post-experimental memory tests better formatches (the futuric present tense condition) than mismatches(the past tense condition)? Results revealed inspection of therecent event target as participants processed the verb-actionmismatch (Exp 1) and actor gaze incongruence (Exp 2). How-ever, the gaze (but not the verb-action) incongruence elimi-nated the overall recent event preference in the NP2 region.The memory tests also showed some evidence for a reversal ofthe recent-event preference.

It’s Time: Quantifying the Relevant Timescales for Joint Attention

The study of the coordination of attention, a term called jointattention (JA), has resulted in a better understanding of thedynamics and development of communication. Despite theimportant insights gained from studying JA, there is littleconsensus regarding the specific components that are included inoperationalizing JA. The present work explored a parameter spaceof JA during a dyadic naturalistic toy play task between 9-month-old infants and their parents. We systematically measured thetemporal properties of two components commonly used tooperationalize JA: the duration of continuous alignment of parentand infant visual fixations and the flexibility of fluctuations ofattention. The results show that very brief bouts of JA areimportant predictors for vocabulary development. The results fromthis work provide new insights into the specific properties used tooperationalize JA and point to the importance of consideringmultiple timescales of behavior that make up JA.

The Role of Letter Frequency on Eye Movements in Sentential Pseudoword Reading

For a language learner, any new word is a pseudoword. Apseudoword is a string of of letters or phonemes that soundslike an existing word in a language, though it has no meaningin the lexicon. On the other hand, speakers are well aware ofpermissible phonemes, their frequencies and collocations intheir language due to the phonotactics inherent in thelanguage. For example, saktal is a pseudoword in Turkish,whereas szyan is not, due to Turkish phonotactics. This studyinvestigates the relationship between pseudoword letterformation and eye movement characteristics in reading. Inparticular, we examine the role of Turkish vowel harmony,middle-word consonant collocation, and word-initial andword-final consonants on eye movements with adult nativespeakers reading sentences that involve predesigned Turkishpseudowords. The results of an experiment with 34participants are indicative of the role of pseudowordformation on a set of eye movement parameters.

Children’s familiarity preference in self-directed study improvesrecognition memory

In both adults and school-age children, volitional controlover the presentation of stimuli during study leads to en-hanced recognition memory. Yet little is known abouthow very young learners choose to allocate their timeand attention during self-directed study. Using a recog-nition memory task, we investigate self-directed study inlow-income preschoolers, who are at an age when atten-tion, memory, and executive function skills rapidly de-velop and learning strategies emerge. By pre-exposingchildren to some items before self-directed study, weaimed to discover how familiarity modulates their studystrategies. We found that children showed a preferencefor studying pre-exposed items. Overall, items stud-ied longer led to increased recognition of those items attest. We also compared recognition task performanceand strategies with measures of cognitive control skills,finding that children’s selective attention skills supportrecognition performance. These findings may informboth theory and educational intervention.

From Words to Sentences & Back:Characterizing Context-dependent Meaning Representations in the Brain

Recent Machine Learning systems in vision and languageprocessing have drawn attention to single-word vector spaces,where concepts are represented by a set of basic features orattributes based on textual and perceptual input. However,such representations are still shallow and fall short fromsymbol grounding. In contrast, Grounded Cognition theoriessuch as CAR (Concept Attribute Representation; Binder etal., 2009) provide an intrinsic analysis of word meaning interms of sensory, motor, spatial, temporal, affective andsocial features, as well as a mapping to corresponding brainnetworks. Building on this theory, this research aims tounderstand an intriguing effect of grounding, i.e. how wordmeaning changes depending on context. CAR representationsof words are mapped to fMRI images of subjects readingdifferent sentences, and the contributions of each worddetermined through Multiple Linear Regression and theFGREP nonlinear neural network. As a result, the FGREPmodel in particular identifies significant changes on theCARs for the same word used in different sentences, thussupporting the hypothesis that context adapts the meaning ofwords in the brain. In future work, such context-modifiedword vectors could be used as representations for a naturallanguage processing system, making it more effective androbust.

The Influence of Pop-Culture on Misattribution of Memory

Social media platforms provide a source for transmittinginformation that can become widely accepted. However, inthis process of transmission, information becomes susceptibleto distortion. In this study, we assessed people’s semantic(i.e., prior expectations) and recognition memory for popculture content, as a function of confidence and perceivedinformation source. In Experiment 1, we investigatedsemantic memory for ubiquitous movie quotes (e.g., thefamous Star Wars quote “Luke I am your father”). Notablythis quote is incorrect, but we found that a majority ofparticipants accepted these lure quotes as true with highconfidence and indicated they had experienced the originalsource. In Experiment 2, participants viewed the originalmovie sources before a recognition test of the quotes. Wefound that while there was some improvement, people stillpreferred the lure quote with high confidence. We discuss thefindings in terms of the strength of people’s prior expectationswhen reconstructing events from memory.

A Computational Model for Reasoning About the Paper Folding TaskUsing Visual Mental Images

The paper folding task is commonly used for the evaluation ofnonverbal, spatial reasoning skills. In this paper, we presenta computational model that attempts to use visual-imagery-based representations and operations to solve this task. Themodel was tested against all problems from the standard pa-per folding task and achieved a perfect score, illustrating thatvisual-imagery-based representations and operations are suf-ficiently expressive to capture at least one successful solutionstrategy. Although the model does not closely resemble humancognitive processing, and thus should not be considered in itscurrent form to be a plausible psychological model of humantask performance, the assumptions made and their implicationsfor our understanding of human cognition on the paper foldingtask point to fruitful lines of future work towards this goal.

Legal HARKing: theoretical grounding in interaction research

In psychology, we tend to follow the general logic of fal-sificationism: we separate the ‘context of discovery’ (howwe come up with theories) from the ‘context of justification’(how we test them). However, when studying human interac-tion, separating these contexts can lead to theories with lowecological validity that do not generalize well to life outsidethe lab. We propose borrowing research practices from for-mal inductive methodologies during the process of discover-ing new regularities and analyzing natural data without beingled by theory. From the perspective of experimental psychol-ogy, this approach may appear similar to the ‘questionable re-search practice’ of HARKing (Hypothesizing After The Re-sults are Known). We argue that a carefully constructed formof HARKing can be used systematically and transparently dur-ing exploratory research and can lead to more robust and eco-logically valid theories.

Segmentation as Retention and Recognition: the R&R model

We present the Retention and Recognition model (R&R), aprobabilistic exemplar model that accounts for segmentationin Artificial Language Learning experiments. We show thatR&R provides an excellent fit to human responses in threesegmentation experiments with adults (Frank et al., 2010),outperforming existing models. Additionally, we analyze theresults of the simulations and propose alternative explanationsfor the experimental findings.

Phonological features in the bilingual lexicon: Insights from tonal accent inSwedish

Scandinavian languages like Swedish employ tonal accent as a lexical phonological feature, where suprasegmentalinformation can be the sole factor differentiating between words. Using cross-modal semantic fragment priming we testedthe following: (a) Do monolingual speakers of Swedish use tonal accent information during lexical access? (b) Do bilingualspeakers, who grew up with one tonal (Swedish) and one non-tonal language, treat this feature the same way as monolinguals?Our results show that for monolinguals, accent mispronunciations eliminate priming effects, implying that tone is used duringlexical access. For bilinguals, by contrast, mispronunciation sensitivity depends on both the accent type and its distributionacross the linguistic input, as well as on the lexical neighbourhood.

Semantic Networks Generated from Early Linguistic Input

Semantic networks generated from different word corporashow common structural characteristics, including high de-grees of clustering, short average path lengths, and scale freedegree distributions. Previous research has disagreed aboutwhether these features emerge from internally- or externally-driven properties (i.e. words already in the lexicon vs. regu-larities in the external world), mapping onto preferential at-tachment and preferential acquisition accounts, respectively(Steyvers & Tenenbaum, 2005; Hills, Maouene, Maouene,Sheya, & Smith, 2009). Such accounts suggest that inherentsemantic structure shapes new lexical growth. Here we ex-tend previous work by creating semantic networks using theSEEDLingS corpus, a newly collected corpus of linguistic in-put to infants. Using a recently developed LSA-like approach(GLoVe vectors), we confirm the presence of previously re-ported structural characteristics, but only in certain ranges ofsemantic similarity space. Our results confirm the robustnessof certain aspects of network organization, and provide novelevidence in support of preferential acquisition accounts.

Analogical Abstraction in Three-Month-Olds

This research tests whether analogical processing ability is present in 3-month-old infants. Infants are habituated to a series of analogous pairs, instantiating either same (e.g., AA, BB, etc.) or different (e.g., AB, CD, etc.), and then tested with further exemplars of the relations. If they can distinguish the familiar relation from the novel relation, even with new objects, this is evidence that for analogical abstraction across the study pairs. In Experiment 1, we did not find evidence of analogical abstraction when 3-month-olds were habituated to six pairs instantiating the relation. However, in Experiment 2, infants showed evidence of analogical abstraction after habituation to two alternating pairs (e.g., AA, BB, AA, BB...). Further, as with older groups, rendering individual objects salient disrupted relational learning. These results demonstrate that 3-month-old infants are capable of analogical comparison and abstraction. Our findings also place limits on the conditions under which these processes are likely to occur. We discuss implications for theories of relational learning.

A Preliminary P-Curve Meta-Analysis of Learned Categorical Perception Research

A preliminary meta-analysis using the p-curve method(Simonsohn, Nelson, & Simmons, 2014) was performed on asubset of the learned categorical perception literature toexplore the robustness of the phenomenon. Only studies usingnovel visual categories and behavioral measures wereincluded. The results strongly suggest that the phenomenon isrobust but that the studies are somewhat underpowered. Weargue that this is problematic because it renders bothstatistically significant and nonsignificant results verydifficult to interpret, which impedes progress inunderstanding the learned CP phenomenon, for example, whyexpansion vs. compression is observed, or boundary vs.dimensional effects. Fortunately, there is a clear solution:conduct studies with greater statistical power.

Reading Skill Test to Diagnose Basic Language Skills in Comparison to Machines

A reading skill test to diagnose basic language skills is introduced. The test is designed to measure six component skills relevant to reading in comparison with those of state-of- the-art natural language processing technologies. The results of the first large-scale experiments using the test are reported. Surprisingly, almost half of Japanese junior high school students do no better than machines in dependency analysis. More than half of 7th grade students do no better than making random choices on questions involving inferences and definition understanding.

Perception Meets Examination: Studying Deceptive Behaviors in VR

Students cheating on an exam in an academic setting createsan environment where one person (the student) must reasonabout the perception of another (the teacher). In exploring thestudent’s mindset, trends concerning how humans make deci-sions based on their understanding of another human’s inten-tions and knowledge can be uncovered. In this work, we studyhuman cheating behavior through simulated examinations invirtual reality, showing that the teacher’s animacy and orienta-tion plays a large part in the student’s reasoning of the teacher’sawareness. By utilizing a virtual classroom setting and accu-rately tracking a users behavior (through head tracking, eyemovement, etc.), we have also demonstrated how a novel vir-tual reality approach can be used for such experiments involv-ing human behavioral observations, which can be further ex-plored in other cognitive science research experiments.

Against the group actor assumption in joint action research

A central assumption in joint action research is that in order toexplain how individuals act as part of a group, we must firstexplain how the group comes into existence. This assump-tion has led to an unnecessarily narrow research programme:research has focussed largely on interpersonal coordinationmechanisms. I outline an alternative approach predicated ona dynamic conception of the ecosystem. On this view, thereis no need to assume that actors must first constitute a groupagent with their fellows before entering into coordinated ac-tion. Such coordination can be more efficiently explained byrecognizing that all actions perturb the structure of the ecosys-tem itself in a manner that can alter the action possibilitiesavailable to neighbouring actors. This move allows us to over-come entrenched debates over the nature of shared intention-ality, and to instead focus on practical interventions in multi-actor settings.

Towards Automated Classification of Emotional Facial Expressions

Emotional state influences nearly every aspect of human cog-nition. However, coding emotional state is a costly processthat relies on proprietary software or the subjective judgmentsof trained raters, highlighting the need for a reliable, automaticmethod of recognizing and labeling emotional expression. Wedemonstrate that machine learning methods can approach near-human levels for categorization of facial expression in natural-istic experiments. Our results show relative success of modelson highly controlled stimuli and relative failure on less con-trolled images, emphasizing the need for real-world data forapplication to real-world experiments. We then test the poten-tial of combining multiple freely available datasets to broadlycategorize faces that vary across age, race, gender and photo-graphic quality.

Reasons and the “Motivated Numeracy Effect”

Does the ability to reason well make one less likely to engage in motivated reasoning? Following a paradigm used by Kahan, Peters, Dawson, and Slovic (2013), this study aims to replicate, extend, and explain the surprising finding that those most likely to process politicized data in a biased manner are those who score highest on a measure of numerical proficiency. Although our study found general effects of motivated reasoning, we failed to replicate Kahan et al.’s “motivated numeracy effect”. However, our study did find that, when forced to consider competing statistical interpretations of the data before responding, highly numerate participants were more likely than less numerate ones to choose a correct but belief-contradicting interpretation of data. These results suggest that while numerate participants were biased when generating responses, they were not when evaluating reasons to justify their responses

Belief Updating and Argument Evaluation

Studies of how evidence affects beliefs sometimes show be-lief polarization in response to mixed evidence. However, thenature of the mental processes leading to change in opinion isup for debate. Different accounts of how people process evi-dence and then update their beliefs make different predictions,especially about one-sided evidence, which is rarely examined.We presented subjects with multiple text arguments regardingsocio-political topics as one-sided or mixed evidence. Partici-pants rated arguments differently according to their extant be-liefs, which is consistent with accounts of motivated reason-ing. They did not polarize afterward, instead showing evi-dence of belief updating according to Bayesian principles: be-lief change is sensitive to prior opinions and to the directionand quality of the evidence presented. These data support re-thinking some of the mental processes underlying incorpora-tion of evidence into a personal belief structure.

The impact of the Digital Age in Moral Judgments

Nowadays, several of the situations in which we have to makedecisions are in digital form. In a first experiment (N=1010)we showed that people’s moral judgments depend on theDigital Context (Smartphone vs. PC) in which a dilemma ispresented, becoming more utilitarian (vs. deontological) whenusing Smartphones. To provide additional evidence, we ran asecond (N=250) and a third experiment (N=300), where weintroduced time constraints and we manipulated timeinstructions. Our results provide an extended perspective onDual-Process Models of Moral Judgment, as we showed thatthe use of smartphones, often assumed to be hurried whichwould be consistent with gut-feeling decision-making,increased the likelihood of utilitarian responses and decreaseddeontological ones. This is the first study to look at theimpact of the digital age on moral judgments and the resultspresented have consequences for understanding moral choicein our increasingly virtualized world

Exploring Functions of Working Memory Related to Fluid Intelligence:Coordination and Relational Integration

Two hypothesized functions of working memory –coordination (ability to maintain unrelated storage loads duringprocessing) and integration (ability to integrate multipleelements into a relation) – were explored and compared to fluidintelligence. In Experiment 1, 130 participants completed amodified Latin-Square Task (LST) which experimentallyadded or reduced storage load. Results suggested that pureintegration (with no storage load) could predict Gf, but nodifference was found between coordination and integration.Experiment 2 employed the Arithmetic Chain Task (ACT),again with modifications to storage load. Results supportreplication of LST findings, though a distinction was foundbetween coordination and integration when storage materialcould not be easily rehearsed. Findings from both experimentssupport a distinction between coordination and integrationtasks in understanding the WM-Gf association.

How Order of Label Presentation Impacts Semantic Processing: an ERP Study

In this study, we wanted to investigate whether the processing of semantic information is easier when mapping names to pictures or is it the other way around. In order to test this hypothesis, we ran a behavioural and an ERP (Event Related Potential) study, with specific interest in the N400 component as an indicator of semantic processing. We compared three groups of participants who did a match/mismatch task with the only difference being that the labels would appear before, after or simultaneously with the pictures. Not surprisingly, the hardest condition was the one where the two information were presented simultaneously. The amplitude of the N400 was more prominent in the condition where labels were presented after the pictures in comparison to the condition where labels preceded picture presentation, suggesting that this second experimental situation led to smaller violation of expectation for our participants (word to picture condition) in comparison to mapping pictures to words.

The Relationship Between Executive Functions and Science Achievement

Executive function is a fundamental component of the human cognitive architecture. Here, we investigate the rela-tionship between executive function and scientific reasoning. Eighth graders completed measures of three executive functions(EFs): shifting, inhibiting, and updating. They also completed a measure of cognitive flexibility, the Wisconsin Card Sort Task(WCST), that has predicted scientific reasoning in prior studies. Scientific reasoning was measured by a standardized test ofscience achievement. A principal components analysis found that the three EFs were separable. Different EFs predicted dif-ferent aspects of cognitive flexibility; notably, participants with poor shifting ability made more perseverative errors. Both EFand WCST predicted science achievement. Of note was the finding that better updating (i.e., working memory) was associatedwith higher science scores. These findings illuminate the role of EF in cognitive flexibility and scientific reasoning, and pointthe way to future studies of the effect of training EF on science achievement.

Contrasts in reasoning about omissions

Omissions figure prominently in causal reasoning fromdiagnosis to ascriptions of negligence. One philosophicalproposal posits that omissions are accompanied by acontrasting alternative that describes a case of orthodox (non-omissive) causation (Schaffer, 2005; Bernstein, 2014). Apsychological hypothesis can be drawn from this contrastview of omissions: by default, humans should interpretomissive causations as representing at least two possibilities,i.e., a possibility representing the omission and a possibilityrepresenting a contrast. The theory of mental models supposesthat reasoners construct only one possibility (the omission) bydefault, and that they consider separate alternativepossibilities in sequential order. Two experiments test thecontrast hypothesis against the model theory, and findevidence in favor of the model-theoretic account.

Representing time in terms of space: Directions of mental timelines in Norwegian

People often use spatial vocabulary to describe temporal rela-tions, and this has increasingly motivated attempts to mapspatial frames of reference (FoRs) onto time. How people as-sign FRONT to time and to temporal entities depends on cul-tural conventions, and is crucial for diagnosing which tem-poral FoR a person actually adopts. Here, we report findingsfrom a survey with speakers of Norwegian that aimed at as-sessing the cultural conventions involved in FRONT assign-ment. Data on temporal movements of events, on the temporalorder of events, and on explicit FRONT assignments to events,time units, and “time itself” suggest that participants use dif-ferent principles for describing fixed relations (static time)versus moving events (dynamic time).

A Bayesian model of knowledge and metacognitive control:Applications to opt-in tasks

In many ecologically situated cognitive tasks, participants en-gage in self-selection of the particular stimuli they choose toevaluate or test themselves on. This contrasts with a traditionalexperimental approach in which an experimenter has completecontrol over the participant’s experience. Considering thesetwo situations jointly provides an opportunity to understandwhy participants opt in to some stimuli or tasks but not toothers. We present here a Bayesian model of cognitive andmetacognitive processes that uses latent contextual knowledgeto model how learners use knowledge to make opt-in decisions.We leverage the model to describe how performance on self-selected stimuli relates to performance on true experimentaltasks that deny learners the opportunity for self-selection. Weillustrate the utility of the approach with an application to ageneral-knowledge answering task.

Modality Switch Effects Emerge Early and Increase throughout Conceptual Processing: Evidence from ERPs

We tested whether conceptual processing is modality-specific by tracking the time course of the Conceptual Modality Switch effect. Forty-six participants verified the relation between property words and concept words. The conceptual modality of consecutive trials was manipulated in order to produce an Auditory-to-visual switch condition, a Haptic-to- visual switch condition, and a Visual-to-visual, no-switch condition. Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) were time-locked to the onset of the first word (property) in the target trials so as to measure the effect online and to avoid a within-trial confound. A switch effect was found, characterized by more negative ERP amplitudes for modality switches than no- switches. It proved significant in four typical time windows from 160 to 750 milliseconds post word onset, with greater strength in posterior brain regions, and after 350 milliseconds. These results suggest that conceptual processing may be modality-specific in certain tasks, but also that the early stage of processing is relatively amodal.

Object Representation in Multiattribute Choice

We propose a theoretical framework for understanding how everyday choice objects are represented and how decisions involving these objects are made. Our framework combines insights regarding object and concept representation in semantic memory research with multiattribute choice rules proposed by scholars of decision making. We also outline computational techniques for using our framework to quantitatively predict naturalistic multiattribute choices. We test our approach in two-object and three-object forced choice experiments involving common books, movies, and foods. Despite using complex naturalistic stimuli, we find that our approach achieves high predictive accuracy rates, and is also able to provide a good account of decision time distributions.

The Interactive Shaping of Social Learning in Transmission Chains

This study investigated the social transmission of memoriesand skills collected from a collaborative cooking task (ravioli-making) and across transmission chains. The transmissionover three generations of pairs of participants occurred undertwo conditions. In the interactive condition, transmissionsover generations occurred in face-to-face conversations,whereas in the non-interactive condition, generations video-recorded their instructions to the next generations. Weanalyzed the effects of verbal and embodied features ofinformational transfer on task performance. Our results showthat performances improved over generations regardless ofinteractivity. In the discussion we suggest that tools (likecooking utensils) may have operated as cultural affordancesencapsulating and transmitting important cultural knowledgefor the successful completion of the task.

Language Modality Affects Responses in Left IFG during Processing ofSemantically Ambiguous Sentences

Ambiguity resolution requires high-level interpretation processes, at least some of which are subserved by theinferior frontal gyrus (IFG), a region that is susceptible to modulation by task demands. This fMRI study investigates the extentto which ambiguity-related activation in IFG is modulated by the specific cognitive-linguistic demands posed by the modality inwhich a sentence is presented. In the present study, ambiguous sentences and matched unambiguous sentences were presentedin three conditions: listening, reading, and rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). The RSVP modality elicited strongerambiguity-related haemodynamic responses than the other two modalities, particularly in left anterior IFG. This indicatesthat the RSVP modality cannot be used as a simple substitute for natural reading without taking into account the additionalprocessing resources it requires.

Generalized Representation of Syntactic Structures

Analysis of language provides important insights into the un-derlying psychological properties of individuals and groups.While the majority of language analysis work in psychologyhas focused on semantics, psychological information is en-coded not just in what people say, but how they say it. Inthe current work, we propose Conversation Level Syntax Simi-larity Metric-Group Representations (CASSIM-GR). This toolbuilds generalized representations of syntactic structures ofdocuments, thus allowing researchers to distinguish betweenpeople and groups based on syntactic differences. CASSIM-GR builds off of Conversation Level Syntax Similarity Metricby applying spectral clustering to syntactic similarity matricesand calculating the center of each cluster of documents. Thisresulting cluster centroid then represents the syntactical struc-ture of the group of documents. To examine the effectivenessof CASSIM-GR, we conduct three experiments across threeunique corpora. In each experiment, we calculate the cluster-ing accuracy and compare our proposed technique to a bag-of-words approach. Our results provide evidence for the ef-fectiveness of CASSIM-GR and demonstrate that combiningsyntactic similarity and tf-idf semantic information improvesthe total accuracy of group classification.

Context reduces coercion costs – Evidence from eyetracking during reading

This paper presents an eyetracking during reading experimentthat investigated the role of supportive context on processingaspectual coercion. Coercion sentences in need of aspectualenrichment were embedded in discourse contexts providingthe necessary information for successful interpretation. Thefindings of the reported experiment show that context infor-mation can be used immediately without disrupting reading ofcoercion sentences. The lack of coercion costs in supportivediscourse contexts provides experimental evidence for the pro-posed Composition in Context Hypothesis and against theoriesthat view semantic composition as largely encapsulated fromcontext. Furthermore, the present experiment investigated therole of inter-individual differences in verbal working memorycapacity on the immediate use of contextual information incomputing coerced interpretations.

The Effects of Autonomy on Emotions and Learning in Game-Based Learning Environments

The current study examined the impact of agency on college students’ emotions and learning during gameplay with CRYSTAL ISLAND, a game-based learning environment designed to foster microbiology learning. 96 undergraduate students (59% female) from a large North American university participated in the study. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions (i.e., full agency, partial agency, no agency), based on the level of control granted during gameplay, and were asked to uncover the source, identity, and best treatment for a mysterious illness. Results revealed participants in the partial agency condition achieved the highest (pre- to post-test) proportional learning gain (PLG), even when controlling for session duration. Additionally, there was a positive correlation between evidence scores of four emotions (anger, fear, confusion, and frustration) and PLG within the partial agency condition—meaning the higher the evidence of the above emotions, the higher the PLG. Further, a stepwise multiple regression showed anger as the sole predictor of PLG. Results from this study have important implications for understanding the role of autonomy and emotions during learning and problem solving with GBLEs designed to foster scientific thinking in STEM. The current study suggests that although GBLEs offer significant learning benefits, they also induce several emotions that can facilitate or inhibit learning gains, requiring further examination.

A Computational Model of the Role of Attention in Subitizing and Enumeration

Recent studies in the perception of numerosity have indicatedthat subitizing (the rapid and accurate enumeration of smallquantities) requires attention. We present a novel computa-tional model of enumeration in which attention unifies dis-tinct processes of numerosity approximation, subitizing, andexplicit counting. We demonstrate how this model accountsfor both the reaction time results from the subitizing literatureand the effects of attentional load on subitizing accuracy.

Effects of transmission perturbation in the cultural evolution of language

Two factors seem to play a major role in the cultural evolutionof language. On the one hand, there is functional pressure to-wards efficient transfer of information. On the other hand, lan-guages have to be learned repeatedly and will therefore showtraces of systematic stochastic disturbances of the transmissionof linguistic knowledge. While a lot of attention has been paidto the effects of cognitive learning biases on the transmissionof language, there is reason to expect that the class of possiblyrelevant transmission perturbations is much larger. This papertherefore explores some potential effects of transmission noisedue to errors in the observation of states of the world. We lookat three case studies on (i) vagueness, (ii) meaning deflation,and (iii) underspecified lexical meaning. These case studiessuggest that transmission perturbations other than learning bi-ases might help explain attested patterns in the cultural evolu-tion of language and that perturbations due to perceptual noisemay even produce effects very similar to learning biases.

Recursion in Children’s Comprehension and Formulation of Algorithms

Recursive loops in informal algorithms are difficult to formulate, even for na ̈ıve adults (Khemlani et al., 2013).Children can formulate algorithms that do not require loops (Bucciarelli et al., 2016), and anecdotal evidence suggests thatthey can understand loops. As there were no previous studies, we examined how they made deductions of the consequencesof loops, and how they abduced loops in creating informal algorithms in everyday language. We therefore tested fifth-gradechildren’s ability carry out both these tasks in algorithms that rearrange the order of cars on a toy railway track with onesiding. Experiment 1 showed that they could deduce rearrangements from algorithms containing loops, and Experiment 2showed that they could formulate at least some algorithms that contained loops. These abilities are the likely precursors to thecomprehension of recursion and to computer programming.

Is Structural Priming in Children Facilitated by Interactions between Animacy andSyntax?

Sentence production relies on the activation of both semanticinformation (e.g. noun animacy) and syntactic frames thatspecify an order for grammatical functions (e.g. subjectbefore object; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyers, 1999). However, itis unclear whether these semantic and syntactic processesinteract (Gámez & Vasilyeva, 2015), and if this changesdevelopmentally. We thus examined the extent to whichanimacy-semantic role mappings in dative prime sentencesand target scenes influenced choice of syntactic structure. 143participants (47 three year olds, 48 five year olds and 48adults) alternated with the experimenter in describinganimations. Animacy mappings for themes and goals wereeither prototypical or non-prototypical and either matched ormismatched across the experimenter’s prime scenes andparticipants’ target elicitation scenes. Prime sentences wereeither double-object datives (e.g. the girl brought the monkeya ball) or prepositional datives (e.g. the girl brought the ballto the monkey). Participants’ target sentences were coded forsyntactic form. All age groups showed a main structuralpriming effect. For the youngest group, animacy-semanticrole mappings facilitated prepositional dative priming. Noanimacy facilitation was found for the older groups. Ourresults demonstrate the changing influence of animacy cueson sentence production through interactions with syntacticstructure over the course of development. The theoreticalimplications of our findings are discussed.

Equiprobability principle or “no change” principle? Examining reasoning in the Monty Hall Dilemma using unequal probabilities

The Monty Hall Dilemma (MHD) is a well-known cognitive illusion. It is often claimed that one reason for the incorrect answers is that people apply the equiprobability principle: they assume that the probability of the two remaining options must be equal. An alternative explanation for assigning the same probabilities to options is that they had the same prior probabilities and people perceive no significant change. Standard MHD versions do not distinguish these possibilities, but a version with unequal prior probabilities could. Participants were given an unequal probabilities version of MHD and told that either the high or low probability option had been eliminated. This affected participants’ choices and their posterior probabilities. Only 14% of participants’ responses were consistent with applying the equiprobability principle, but 51% were consistent with a “no change” principle. Participants were sensitive to the implications of the prior probabilities but did not appear to use Bayesian updating.

Discovering simple heuristics from mental simulation

In the history of cognitive science, there have been two com-peting philosophies regarding how people reason about theworld. In one, people rely on rich, generative models to makepredictions about a wide range of scenarios; while in the other,people have a large “bag of tricks”, idiosyncratic heuristics thattend to work well in practice. In this paper, we suggest thatrather than being in opposition to one another, these two ideascomplement each other. We argue that people’s capacity formental simulation may support their ability to learn new cue-based heuristics, and demonstrate this phenomenon in two ex-periments. However, our results also indicate that participantsare far less likely to learn a heuristic when there is no logical orexplicitly conveyed relationship between the cue and the rele-vant outcome. Furthermore, simulation—while a potentiallyuseful tool—is no substitute for real world experience

Fast and Easy: Approximating Uniform Information Density in LanguageProduction

A model of sentence production is presented, which imple-ments a strategy that produces sentences with more uniformsurprisal profiles, as compared to other strategies, and in accor-dance to the Uniform Information Density Hypothesis (Jaeger,2006; Levy & Jaeger, 2007). The model operates at the al-gorithmic level combining information concerning word prob-abilities and sentence lengths, representing a first attempt tomodel UID as resulting from underlying factors during lan-guage production. The sentences produced by this modelshowed indeed the expected tendency, having more uniformsurprisal profiles and lower average word surprisal, in compar-ison to other production strategies.

The paradox of relational development is not universal:Abstract reasoning develops differently across cultures

Recent studies demonstrate a puzzling decline in relationalreasoning during development. Specifically, 3-year-olds failin a relational match-to-sample (RMTS) task, while youngerchildren (18-30 months) succeed (Walker, Bridgers, &Gopnik, 2016). Hoyos, Shao, and Gentner (2016) propose thatolder children fail because of a bias toward individual objectproperties induced by “avid noun learning.” If this is the case,children learning a language with a stronger emphasis onverbs, like Mandarin Chinese, may show an attenuateddecline in relational reasoning. We first test this possibility byreproducing the causal RMTS task in China, and find thatMandarin-speaking 3-year-olds outperform their English-speaking peers in the U.S. In a second experiment, we showthat Mandarin speakers exhibit a corresponding bias towardrelational solutions while English speakers prefer object-based solutions in an ambiguous context. We discuss possiblemechanisms through which language and culture maypromote (or hinder) the early development of relationalreasoning.

Grammar-Based and Lexicon-Based Techniques to Extract Personality Traitsfrom Text

Language provides an important source of information to pre-dict human personality. However, most studies that have pre-dicted personality traits using computational linguistic meth-ods have focused on lexicon-based information. We investigateto what extent the performance of lexicon-based and grammar-based methods compare when predicting personality traits. Weanalyzed a corpus of student essays and their personality traitsusing two lexicon-based approaches, one top-down (Linguis-tic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC)), one bottom-up (topicmodels) and one grammar-driven approach (Biber model), aswell as combinations of these models. Results showed thatthe performance of the models and their combinations demon-strated similar performance, showing that lexicon-based top-down models and bottom-up models do not differ, and neitherdo lexicon-based models and grammar-based models. More-over, combination of models did not improve performance.These findings suggest that predicting personality traits fromtext remains difficult, but that the performance from lexicon-based and grammar-based models are on par.

Decomposability and Frequency in the Hindi/Urdu Number System

Hindi/Urdu (HU) numbers 10–99 are highly irregular, unlikethe transparent systems of most languages. I investigate themorphological decomposability of HU numbers using a seriesof computational models. While these models classify mostforms accurately, problems are encountered in high-frequencyforms of low cardinality, suggesting that some HU numbers aremore transparent (i.e., morphologically decomposable) thanothers. These results are compatible with a dual-route accessmodel proposed for the processing of numeral forms.

Is the relative distribution of verbs and nouns modulated by socio-culturalinfluences? Evidence from bilingual infants and toddlers in Malaysia.

Early vocabularies in most languages tend to contain more nouns than verbs. Yet, the strength of this noun bias hasbeen observed to vary across languages and cultures. Two main hypotheses have aimed at explaining such variations; eitherthat the relative importance of nouns vs. verbs is language- specific, or that socio-cultural influences shape early vocabularystructures. The present study compares the relative distribution of verbs and nouns, in English, between two groups of bilingualinfants and toddlers; Malay-English and Mandarin- English. We found that early English lexicons of Mandarin- Englishbilinguals contained more verbs than in the English lexicon of Malay-English bilinguals, in both comprehension and production.We discuss the potential role of socio-cultural influences on the vocabulary structure in young users of a language.

Exploring the relations between oral language and reading instruction in acomputational model of reading

To become a proficient reader, children have to learnmappings between print, sound and meaning. There isdebate over whether reading instruction should focus on therelations between print and sound, as in phonics, or on therelationship between print and meaning, as in sight wordreading. In a study where participants learned a novelartificial orthography, Taylor, Davis and Rastle (2017)compared print to sound focused or print to meaningfocused reading training, demonstrating that sound trainingwas superior for learning to read. However, a benefit fromsound focused training is likely dependent on prioracquisition of effective sound to meaning relations of words.To explore this issue, we developed a connectionist modelof reading. We exposed the model to a sound or a meaningfocused training, but varied the model’s pre-acquired orallanguage skills. The simulation results showed thatproficiency in oral language is a determinant of theadvantage of print to sound focused reading training,suggesting that reading training should address both orallanguage skills and print to sound mappings.

Evaluating vector-space models of analogy

Vector-space representations provide geometric tools for rea-soning about the similarity of a set of objects and their relation-ships. Recent machine learning methods for deriving vector-space embeddings of words (e.g., word2vec) have achievedconsiderable success in natural language processing. Thesevector spaces have also been shown to exhibit a surprising ca-pacity to capture verbal analogies, with similar results for nat-ural images, giving new life to a classic model of analogies asparallelograms that was first proposed by cognitive scientists.We evaluate the parallelogram model of analogy as applied tomodern word embeddings, providing a detailed analysis of theextent to which this approach captures human relational sim-ilarity judgments in a large benchmark dataset. We find thatthat some semantic relationships are better captured than oth-ers. We then provide evidence for deeper limitations of the par-allelogram model based on the intrinsic geometric constraintsof vector spaces, paralleling classic results for first-order simi-larity.

Anticipation Effect after Implicit Distributional Learning

Distributional learning research has established that humanscan track the frequencies of sequentially presented stimuli inorder to infer the probabilities of upcoming events (e.g., Hasher& Zacks, 1984). Here, we set out to explore anticipation of astimulus after implicit distributional learning. We hypothesizethat as people learn the category frequency informationimplicitly, response times will scale according to the relativefrequency of the stimulus category. Twelve adult participantsviewed photographs of faces, tools, and buildings whileperforming a simple classification task. We found that responsetimes significantly decreased with greater frequencies in thedistribution of stimulus categories. This result suggested thatdistributional information about the internal representations ofthe stimuli could be learned and indicated the possibility thatparticipants anticipated the stimuli proportional to theprobability of the category appearing and thereby reducedresponse times for the more frequent categories.

Analytic Causal Knowledge for Constructing Useable Empirical Causal Knowledge:Two Experiments on Preschoolers

The present paper examines what domain-general causalknowledge reasoners need for at least some outcome-variabletypes to construct useable content-specific causal knowledge.In particular, it explains why it is essential to have analyticknowledge of causal-invariance integration functions:knowledge for predicting the expected outcome assuming thatthe empirical knowledge acquired regarding a causal relationholds across the learning context and an application context.The paper reports two studies that support the hypothesis thatpreschool children have such knowledge regarding binarycauses and effects, enabling them to generalize acrosscontexts rationally, favoring the causal-invariance hypothesisover alternative hypotheses, including interaction (e.g., linear)integration functions, heuristics, and biases.

The relationship between fairness, cognitive control, and numerical encoding

Fairness, or the ability to distribute resources in a manner thataccords with societally recognized principles of justice, is ahallmark of human cooperation. Young children rapidlydevelop the ability to enact fairness, but the cognitiveunderpinnings of this ability remain unknown. The presentstudy investigated 4-7-year-olds’ acquisition of threeprinciples of fairness -- equality (the principle that all partiesshould have the same), merit (the principle that those whowork harder should get more), and starting opportunity (theprinciple that those who started with less should get more) --in relation to their emerging cognitive control and memory fornumerical information (numerical accuracy). Cognitivecontrol predicted children’s equal sharing, whereas numericalaccuracy predicted merit-based sharing. Children up throughthe oldest age we tested ignored starting opportunities. Theresults suggest that different principles of fairness may beunderpinned by distinct cognitive processes.

Risky Decision Making for Medications: Age and Social Influence Effects

Prior studies on older adults’ risk taking have paid little attention to the healthcare domain or social influences on decision making. This study examined age-related differences in medication risk taking and the effects of a collaborative decision-making experience on individuals’ tendency to take risks. We recruited 24 younger (mean age = 19.50, SD = 1.41) and 24 older adults (mean age = 70.54, SD = 2.30), and asked them to choose between hypothetical medications that differed in probabilities and outcomes of treatment success. To investigate the effects of risk-neutral versus risk- advantageous trials, participants chose between a risky option and a sure option that had equal expected values (risk-neutral) or between a risky option and a sure option that had a lower expected value (risk-advantageous). Participants completed the decision task first individually (the pre-collaboration phase), then in dyads (the collaboration phase), and once again individually (the post-collaboration phase). During the pre-collaboration phase older adults showed a smaller increase in risk-taking tendency in response to risk- advantageous trials compared to younger adults. The pre-and post-collaboration data showed that older adults’ risk preferences converged towards their partner’s preference to a greater extent following collaboration relative to younger adults. These findings highlight the importance of designing decision aids to encourage older adults to take risks when risk taking is beneficial, and considering how social processes influence patients’ medication decisions.

Finding Creative New Ideas:Human-Centric Mindset Overshadows Mind-Wandering

Finding creative new ideas requires both release from fixationand a productive search mindset. Recent research has shownthat messy desks, walking, and mind-wandering can lead tomore new uses for old objects. Here we show that a human-centric mindset is superior to mind-wandering for generatingmore alternative uses and more creative uses because itprovides both release from fixation and an effective searchstrategy. A human-centric mindset entails perspective-taking,and perspective-taking is likely to be an effective generalstrategy for enhancing creativity, problem-solving andinnovation.

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) and the Face Inversion Effect: Anodal stimulation at Fp3 reduces recognition for upright faces

Perceptual learning is a key perceptual skill that people possess, in particular, it contributes to their ability to distinguish between faces thus recognize individuals. Recently, we showed that anodal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) at Fp3 abolishes the inversion effect (that would otherwise exist) for familiar checkerboards created from a prototype. Because of the close analogy between the inversion effect obtained with checkerboards, which we use as a marker for perceptual learning, and the traditional face inversion effect (upright faces recognized better than inverted ones), we investigated the effects of anodal tDCS at Fp3 during an old/new recognition task for upright and inverted faces. Results showed that stimulation significantly reduced the face inversion effect compared to controls. The effect was strongest in reducing recognition performance to upright faces. This result supports our account of perceptual learning and its role as a key factor in face recognition.

Beliefs about sparsity affect causal experimentation

What is the best way of figuring out the structure of a causalsystem composed of multiple variables? One prominent ideais that learners should manipulate each candidate variable inisolation to avoid confounds (known as the “Control of Vari-ables” strategy). Here, we demonstrate that this strategy is notalways the most efficient method for learning. Using an opti-mal learner model which aims to minimize the number of tests,we show that when a causal system is sparse, that is, whenthe outcome of interest has few or even just one actual causeamong the candidate variables, it is more efficient to test mul-tiple variables at once. In a series of behavioral experiments,we then show that people are sensitive to causal sparsity whenplanning causal experiments.

Path salience in motion events from verbal and visual languages

Languages differ in the way they convey paths. S-languagesconveying manner of motion directly in a main verb, while V-languages require a separate verb. This difference has beenshown to influence the conceptualization and narration ofmotion events. We therefore asked: would this difference arisein the paths that people draw, particularly in visual narratives?We annotated the representations of path information (source,trajectory, goal) in a corpus of 35 comics from S- and V-languages. We found that panels from S-languages depicted thepath of an action more often than those from V-languages,consistent with previous research on increased motion eventsalience for S-languages. These findings suggest that theconceptualization of paths from spoken language mayinfluence the graphic depiction of paths.

Using Prior Data to Inform Initial Performance Predictions of Individual Students

The predictive performance equation (PPE) is a mathematicalmodel of learning and retention that uses regularities seen inhuman learning to predict future performance. Previous research(Collins, Gluck, Walsh Krusmark & Gunzelmann, , 2016) foundthat prior data could be used to inform PPE’s free parameterswhen generating predictions of a group’s aggregate performance,allowing for more accurate initial performance predictions. Herewe investigate an extension of this methodology to predictperformance of individuals, rather than aggregate samples. Thispaper documents the results of that investigation, which is on thecritical path to the use of this cognitive technology in educationand training.

Determinants of judgments of explanatory power: Credibility, Generalizability, and Causal Framing

This study investigates how judgments of explanatory power are affected by (i) the prior credibility of a potential explanation, (ii) the causal framing used to describe the explanation, and (iii) the generalizability of the explanation. We found that the prior credibility of a causal explanation plays a central role in explanatory reasoning: first, because of the presence of strong main effects on judgments of explanatory power, and second, because of the gate-keeping role prior credibility has for other factors. Highly credible explanations were not susceptible to causal framing effects. Instead, highly credible hypotheses were sensitive to the generalizability of an explanation. While these results yield a more nuanced understanding of the determinants of judgments of explanatory power, they also illuminate the close relationship between prior beliefs and explanatory power and the relationship between abductive and probabilistic reasoning.

PACKER: An Exemplar Model of Category Generation

Generating new concepts is an intriguing yet understudiedtopic in cognitive science. In this paper, we present a novelexemplar model of category generation: PACKER (ProducingAlike and Contrasting Knowledge using Exemplar Representa-tions). PACKER’s core design assumptions are (1) categoriesare represented as exemplars in a multidimensional psycholog-ical space, (2) generated items should be similar to exemplarsof the same category, and (3) generated categories should bedissimilar to existing categories. A behavioral study revealsstrong effects of contrast- and target-class similarity. Theseeffects are novel empirical phenomena, which are directly pre-dicted by the PACKER model but are not explained by existingformal approaches.

Opening Up and Closing Down Discussion: Experimenting with Epistemic Statusin Conversation

Managing disagreement in conversation requires subtlelinguistic and pragmatics skills. One key dimension is thedegree of ‘knowingness’ with which people present theirstance on an issue. It has been hypothesised that framingstances as ‘knowing’, i.e. with higher implied levels ofspeaker certainty limits the potential for challenge by others.We present the first experimental test of this hypothesis. Usinga text based chat-tool paradigm and a debating task we areable to systematically manipulate how ‘knowing’ people’sturns appear to one-another. The results show that ‘knowing’stances tend to close off discussion leading to less carefullyformulated, truncated turns, but do not reliably affect the rangeof solutions considered. Unknowing stances, by contrast,do not affect turn length or formulation but do encouragemore deliberation and include more signals of certainty in themessage contents.

Learning Relational Concepts throughUnitary versus Compositional Representations

Current theories of relational learning on structure mappingemphasize the importance of compositional representations,based on the concept’s components and the relations amongthem. We consider the possibility that relational concepts canalso be represented unitarily, whereby the concept is aproperty of the stimulus as a whole. The distinction betweencompositional and unitary representations of relationalconcepts is a natural consequence of structure-mappingtheory, but its psychological implications have not beenexplored. We report two experiments in which we examinehow encouraging subjects to represent relational conceptscompositionally versus unitarily affects learning onclassification- and inference-based category learning tasks.Our findings show that unitary representations lead to betterlearning than compositional representations, especially for theinference task. We conclude that unitary representations incurless cognitive load than structural alignment of compositionalrepresentations, and thus may be the default for everydayrelational reasoning.

They Know as Much as We Do: Knowledge Estimation and Partner Modelling ofArtificial Partners

Conversation partners’ assumptions about each other’sknowledge (their partner models) on a subject are importantin spoken interaction. However, little is known about whatinfluences our partner models in spoken interactions withartificial partners. In our experiment we asked people to name15 British landmarks, and estimate their identifiability to aperson as well as an automated conversational agent of eitherBritish or American origin. Our results show that people’sassumptions about what an artificial partner knows are relatedto their estimates of what other people are likely to know -but they generally estimate artificial partners to have moreknowledge in the task than human partners. These findingsshed light on the way in which people build partner models ofartificial partners. Importantly, they suggest that people useassumptions about what other humans know as a heuristicwhen assessing an artificial partner’s knowledge.

Folk Attributions of Control and Intentionality Over Mental States

Influential theories in social psychology, philosophy, andlinguistics assume that ordinary people judge many mentalstates as outside voluntary control, yet few studies have directlyinvestigated these claims. We report four studies suggestingthat, contrary to several prominent models, ordinary peopleattribute at least moderate intentional control to others over awide variety of mental states. Furthermore, it appears thatperceived control may vary systematically according to mentalstate type (e.g. emotions vs. desires vs. beliefs). These resultspoint to several important directions for future research inbehavior explanation and moral judgment.

Judgment Before Emotion:People Access Moral Evaluations Faster than Affective States

Theories about the role of emotions in moral cognition makedifferent predictions about the relative speed of moral andaffective judgments: those that argue that felt emotions arecausal inputs to moral judgments predict that recognition ofaffective states should precede moral judgments; theoriesthat posit emotional states as the output of moral judgmentpredict the opposite. Across four studies, using a speededreaction time task, we found that self-reports of felt emotionwere delayed relative to reports of event-directed moraljudgments (e.g. badness) and were no faster than person-directed moral judgments (e.g. blame). These results pose achallenge to prominent theories arguing that moraljudgments are made on the basis of reflecting on affectivestates.

The Lego hands: changing the affording location of graspable objects

The present study examined throughout three experiments the nature of stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) effects related to affordance perception in situations wherein object affordances and response effectors are irrelevant to each other. In the first experiment, using a foot-press response dispositive, we found a SRC effect between the orientation of the graspable part of the presented object and the laterality of the response. In Experiment 2a, we showed that constraining the subject hands in a given position (i.e., a Lego hand shape) during the same task interfered with the SRC effect. In Experiment 2b, participants performed a short training phase with their hands constrained before performing the experiment. This resulted in an inversion of the direction of the SRC effect previously observed. We discuss these results and provide arguments in favor of a specific motor activation account.

People toss coins with more vigor when the stakes are higher

We trust that the uncertainty regarding the outcome of a cointoss makes it a fair procedure for making a decision. Smalldifferences in the force used to toss a coin should not affect thisuncertainty. However, the voluntary movement involved intossing a coin is subject to motivational influences arising fromthe anticipation of the value of the outcome of the toss.Presented here are measurements of hand velocities duringcoin tossing when the outcomes entail monetary gains andlosses. Finger position measurements show that hand velocitiesare proportional to the amount of money at stake. Coin tossmovements are faster and larger for higher stakes than forsmaller monetary stakes.

Perspective-Taking in Referential Communication: Does Stimulated Attention to Addressee’s Perspective Influence Speakers’ Reference Production?

We investigated whether speakers’ referential communication benefits from an explicit focus on addressees’ perspective. Dyads took part in a referential communication game and were allocated to one of three experimental settings. Each of these settings elicited a different perspective mindset (none, self- focus, other-focus). In the two perspective settings, speakers were explicitly instructed to regard their addressee’s (other- focus) or their own (self-focus) perspective before construing their referential message. Results indicated that eliciting speakers’ self- versus other-focus did not influence their reference production. We did find that speakers with an elicited egocentric perspective reported a higher perspective-taking tendency than speakers in the other two settings. This tendency correlated with actual referring behavior during the game, indicating that speakers who reported a high perspective-taking tendency were less likely to make egocentric errors such as leaking information privileged to speakers themselves. These findings are explained using the objective self-awareness theory.

Sex-Dependent Effects of Emotional Subliminal VisualStimuli on a Decision-Making Task

How do covert emotional stimuli affect decision-making? We investigated this question by exposing par-ticipants to subliminal visual stimuli during a computer-ized version of the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) to assesswhether different categories of images (negative, neutral,or positive emotional evaluations) would influence deci-sion-making behavior. Results did show sex-group inter-actions for IGT scores. In decision learning model simu-lations, it was found that different models were more ap-propriate to explain the task performance for differentsex-group pairs. Overall, women showed more of anability to integrate the additive negative signals from thestimuli to make more advantageous decisions than themen; consequently, this made the men more resilient tothe negative effects of the positive stimuli on task-performance. When taken with existing research, the re-sults indicate that subliminal emotional stimuli can havesubtle, potentially sex-dependent, effects on behaviorduring the decision-making process.

Novice to Expert continuum may affect System Response Time

To familiarize herself with a user-interface of a software system, a user needs practice. With practice, a user's think time gradually decreases—the novice to expert transition. We propose a queueing model that accounts for this transition in analyzing the performance of a distributed software system. We solve the model using deterministic simulation. Our model captures system performance in terms of system response time. We use the model to demonstrate how users— who are at various experience levels in the novice to expert continuum—may affect the system response time.

Modeling Comprehension Processes via Automated Analyses of Dialogism

Dialogism provides the grounds for building a comprehensivemodel of discourse and it is focused on the multiplicity ofperspectives (i.e., voices). Dialogism can be present in anytype of text, while voices become themes or recurrent topicsemerging from the discourse. In this study, we examine theextent that differences between self-explanations and think-alouds can be detected using computational textual indicesderived from dialogism. Students (n = 68) read a text aboutnatural selection and were instructed to generate self-explanations or think-alouds. The linguistic features of thesetext responses were analyzed using ReaderBench, anautomated text analysis tool. A discriminant function analysisusing these features correctly classified 80.9% of the students’assigned experimental conditions (self-explanation vs. thinkaloud). Our results indicate that self-explanation promotestext processing that focuses on connected ideas, rather thanseparate voices or points of view covering multiple topics.

Amortized Hypothesis Generation

Bayesian models of cognition posit that people compute prob-ability distributions over hypotheses, possibly by construct-ing a sample-based approximation. Since people encountermany closely related distributions, a computationally efficientstrategy is to selectively reuse computations – either the sam-ples themselves or some summary statistic. We refer to thesereuse strategies as amortized inference. In two experiments,we present evidence consistent with amortization. When se-quentially answering two related queries about natural scenes,we show that answers to the second query vary systematicallydepending on the structure of the first query. Using a cog-nitive load manipulation, we find evidence that people cachesummary statistics rather than raw sample sets. These resultsenrich our notions of how the brain approximates probabilisticinference.

The Causal Sampler: A Sampling Approach to Causal Representation, Reasoningand Learning

Although the causal graphical model framework has achievedsuccess accounting for numerous causal-based judgments, akey property of these models, the Markov condition, is con-sistently violated (Rehder, 2014; Rehder & Davis, 2016). Anew process model—the causal sampler—accounts for theseeffects in a psychologically plausible manner by assumingthat people construct their causal representations using theMetropolis-Hastings sampling algorithm constrained to onlya small number of samples (e.g., < 20). Because it assumesthat Markov violations are built into people’s causal represen-tations, the causal sampler accounts for the fact that those vio-lations manifest themselves in multiple tasks (both causal rea-soning and learning). This prediction was corroborated by anew experiment that directly measured people’s causal repre-sentations.

Understanding the Role of Perception in the Evolution of Human Language

In this paper, we propose a flexible modeling framework forstudying the role of perception in language learning and lan-guage evolution. This is achieved by augmenting some noveland some existing evolutionary signaling game models withexisting techniques in machine learning and cognitive science.The result is a “grounded” signaling game in which agentsmust extract relevant information from their environment viaa cognitive processing mechanism, then learn to communi-cate that information with each other. The choice of cogni-tive processing mechanism is left as a free parameter, allow-ing the model to be tailored to a wide variety of problemsand tasks. We present results from simulations using both aBayesian perception model and a neural network based per-ception model, which demonstrate how perception can “pre-process” environmental data in a way that is well suited forcommunication. Lastly, we discuss how the model can be ex-tended to study other roles that perception may play in lan-guage learning.

Leaping across the mental canyon: Analogical retrieval across disparate taskdomains

The present study provides evidence for far analogicalretrieval, i.e., analogical retrieval across disparate taskdomains, as a result of analogical comparison. Participantsread source stories, which were then retrieved after a filleddelay through abstract letter-string cues that matched therelational form of key parts of stories. They then generatedresponses to an ambiguous letter-string analogy problem.Evidence was found for far analogical retrieval of higher-order relations because 1. comparison of letter-stringanalogies cued source stories specific to the relations showedin the letter-strings, and then 2. those same relations formedthe basis for how subjects solved novel letter-string problems.The experiment offers support for the schema inductionaccount of analogical retrieval, and suggests that people aremore sensitive to relational structures than was previouslythought.

Conversational topic connectedness predicted by Simplicity Theory

People avoid changing subject abruptly during conversation.There are reasons to think that this constraint is more than asocial convention and is deeply rooted in our cognition. Weshow here that the phenomenon of topic connectedness is anexpected consequence of the maximization of unexpectednessand that it is predicted by Simplicity Theory.

The Cognitive Architecture of Recursion: Behavioral and fMRI Evidence from the Visual, Musical and Motor Domains

In this manuscript, we summarize the results of our research program aiming at describing the cognitive architecture underlying the representation of recursive hierarchical embedding. After conducting a series of behavioral and fMRI experiments in the visual, musical and motor domains, we found that, behaviorally, the acquisition of recursive rules seems supported by cognitive resources that are general across domains. However, when we test well-trained participants in the fMRI, their representation of recursion seems supported by activating schemas stored in (visual, musical and motor) domain-specific repositories. This suggests that the resources necessary to acquire recursive rules are different from those necessary to utilize these rules after extensive training.

The Temporal Cheerleader Effect:Attractiveness Judgments Depend on Surrounding Faces Through Time

Previous research has found that people are seen as moreattractive when they appear in a group rather than in isolation.The present study asks whether faces that surround us in timealso affect how attractive we appear to be. Participants ratedthe attractiveness of famous female faces presented in asequence of three and in isolation. We found that people dointegrate information about attractiveness over time, but thattemporal context has the opposite effect of static context.People perceived faces as less attractive in a series than inisolation. We also varied the attractiveness of surroundingfaces in order to examine how the serial position of contextualinformation might figure into people’s judgments. We foundthat faces presented earlier in the sequence figured moreheavily into people’s judgment than did faces presented laterin the sequence. These findings highlight the role of temporalcontext in perceptions of attractiveness.

Architectural process models of decision making: Towards a model database

We present the project aimed at creating a database of detailed architectural process models of memory-based decision models. Those models are implemented in the cognitive architecture ACT-R. In creating this database, we have identified commonalities and differences of various decision models in the literature. The model database can provide insights into the interrelation among decision models and can be used in future research to address debates on inferences from memory, which are hard to resolve without specifying the processing steps at the level of precision that a cognitive architecture provides.

Talking to Ourselves to Engage Control? Testing Developmental RelationsBetween Self-directed Speech, Cognitive Control and Talkativeness

Is self-directed speech critical to cognitive processessupporting complex, goal-directed behavior? If so, how? Aninfluential developmental hypothesis is that children talk tothemselves to support cognitive control processes, and thatwith age this speech becomes increasingly covert andstrategic. However, while many studies suggest languagesupports cognitive control, evidence that self-directed speechgradually internalizes has been mixed. Moreover, extraneousfactors that could co-vary with self-directed speech, age, andcognitive control, such as talkativeness, have not beensystematically tested. In this cross-sectional study of 86 5- to7-year-old children we measured overt, partially covert, inner,and strategic speech on four cognitive tasks, along with taskperformance and child talkativeness. We did not findconsistent evidence that self-directed speech changes withage; however, we did find consistent associations betweenself-directed speech and talkativeness. Partially covert andstrategic speech predicted performance on one task, and innerspeech was implicated on another. Self-directed speechtended to correlate across tasks, and these correlations heldcontrolling for talkativeness. Taken together, these findingssuggest 5- to 7-year-old children may use different forms ofself-directed speech to support cognitive control, and that theform this speech takes depends in part on factors beyond age,such as the cognitive demands of a task and childcharacteristics like talkativeness.

The Interplay Between Self-evaluation, Goal Orientation, and Self-efficacy onPerformance and Learning

Objective Self-awareness Theory (Duval & Wicklund,1972) proposes that self-evaluation increases an individual’sawareness of any discrepancy between their currentperformance and an internal goal. In the current study weprompted self-evaluation throughout an intelligence test(Analysis-Synthesis Test – AST) using confidence ratings(CR). AST performance, the extent to which participantsincidentally learnt task-relevant rules (learning rules wasunnecessary because they were provided), self-efficacy, andgoals, were assessed. The results indicated an effect ofperforming CR on both performance and rule learning, butthe effect depended on self-efficacy. Compared to matchedcontrols (n=45), participants who performed CR (n=41) andhad high self-efficacy performed better on the AST butlearnt fewer rules. Performing CR had no effect onparticipants low in self-efficacy. This suggests that self-evaluation interacts with self-efficacy to modifyparticipants’ goals, specifically CR appear to shiftindividuals high in self-efficacy from a mastery goal to aperformance goal.

It’s not just what we say, it’s how we move: An examination of postural activityduring a disclosure event

The current study incorporates concepts from dynamicalsystems theory (DST) and embodied cognition topropose a novel method of answering traditionalquestions in social psychology. Namely, we wereinterested in understanding postural sway complexityduring the important interpersonal task of disclosing ahidden stigmatized identity (e.g., mental health disorder,history of sexual abuse). Using detrended fluctuationanalysis and multifractal detrended fluctuation analysis,we captured postural activity while people shared theirpersonal secrets to an imagined other. Results suggestthat disclosure context, defined by both disclosureconfidant and antecedent goals, is indeed embodied inour complex postural activity.

A theory of the detection and learning of structured representations ofsimilarity and relative magnitude

Responding to similarity, difference, and relative magnitude isubiquitous in the animal kingdom. However, humans seemunique in the ability to represent relative magnitude andsimilarity as abstract relations that take arguments (e.g.,greater-than (x,y)). While many models use structuredrelational representations of magnitude and similarity, littleprogress has been made on how these representations arise.Models that use these representations assume access tocomputations of similarity and magnitude a priori. We detail amechanism for producing invariant responses to “same”,“different”, “more”, and “less” which can be exploited tocompute similarity and magnitude as an evaluation operator.Using DORA (Doumas, Hummel, & Sandhofer, 2008), theseinvariant responses can serve to learn structured relationalrepresentations of relative magnitude and similarity from pixelimages of simple shapes.

Co-ordinating Non-mutual Realities: The Asymmetric Impact of Delay onVideo-Mediated Music Lessons

During a music lesson, participants need to co-ordinate boththeir turns at talk and their turns at playing. Verbal and musicalcontributions are shaped by their organisation within the turn-taking system. When lessons are conducted remotely by videoconference, these mechanisms are disrupted by the asymmet-ric effects of delay on the interaction; in effect a “non-mutualreality” comprised of two different conversations at each endof the link. Here we compare detailed case studies of a co-present and a remote music lesson, in order to show how thiseffect arises, and how it impacts conduct during the lesson.

When Do Vehicles of Similes Become Figurative? Gaze Patterns Show that Similesand Metaphors are Initially Processed Differently

Recent emphases on differences between metaphors andsimiles pose a quandary. The two forms clearly differ instrength, but often seem to require similar interpretations. InExperiment 1 we show that ratings of comprehensibility arehighly correlated across simile and metaphor sentencesdiffering only in the presence or absence of “like”. InExperiment 2 we show that comprehensibility ratings forfigurative forms predict both early (first pass) and late(second pass) fixation durations for metaphor vehicle, butonly late fixation durations for vehicles in similes. Similevehicles appear to initially be processed similarly to literalcomparisons, with figurative interpretation occurring later.These observations are consistent with the different pragmaticstrengths, and similar interpretations of the two forms.

Representing the Richness of Linguistic Structure in Models of Episodic Memory

The principal aim of a cognitive model is to infer the processby which the human mind acts on some select set ofenvironmental inputs such that it produces the observed set ofbehavioral outputs. In this endeavor, one of the centralrequirements is that the input to the model be represented asfaithfully and accurately as possible. However, this is ofteneasier said than done. In the study of recognition memory, forinstance, words are the environmental input of choice—yetbecause words vary on many different dimensions, andbecause the problem of quantifying this variation has longbeen out of reach, modelers have tended to rely on idealized,randomly generated representations of their experimentalstimuli. In this paper, we introduce new resources from large-scale text mining that may improve upon this practice,illustrating a simple method for deriving feature informationdirectly from word pools and lists.

Due process in dual process:A model-recovery analysis of Smith et al. (2014)

Considerable behavioral evidence has been cited in support ofthe COVIS dual-system model of category learning (Ashby &Valentin, 2016). The validity of the inferences drawn fromthese data critically depend on the accurate identification ofparticipants’ categorization strategies. In the COVIS literature,participants’ strategies are identified using a model-based anal-ysis inspired by General Recognition Theory (Maddox, 1999).Here, we examine the accuracy of this analysis in a model-recovery simulation. We find that participants can appear tobe using implicit, procedural strategies when their responseswere actually generated by explicit rule-based strategies. Theimplications of this for the COVIS literature are discussed.

Experimental Investigation into the Continuous Pattern of the RelationshipBetween Color Focality and Short-Term Memory Performance for Colors

Past studies reported that language-specific color focality hassubstantial influence on the short-term memory (STM) perfor-mance of colors of the speakers of the language, which we callthe ”focality effect.” This study attempts to clarify the contin-uous pattern of this effect, that is, the manner in which correctrecognition possibilities and misrecognition error distances ofcolors, which are two aspects of the STM performance for col-ors, change in a gradual fashion along the continuum of colorfocality. Our experiment, which tests the Japanese language,finds that a U-shaped relationship exists between the focalityand the possibility of correct recognition, and that the mis-recognition error distance increases as the focality decreases.We speculate that the subjects’ frequent and conscious employ-ment of the memorization strategy of coding colors using lin-guistic categories is one important cause of the detected effectpatterns.

econstructing Social Interaction: The Complimentary Roles of BehaviourAlignment and Partner Feedback to the Creation of Shared Symbols

This paper experimentally tests the contribution of twodistinct aspects of social interaction to the creation ofshared symbols: behaviour alignment and concurrentpartner feedback. Pairs of participants (N= 120, or 60pairs) completed an experimental-semiotic game,similar to Pictionary, in which they tried to communicatea range of recurring meanings to a partner by drawingon a shared whiteboard (without speaking or usingnumbers of letters in their drawings). The opportunityfor sign alignment and/or concurrent partner feedbackwas manipulated in a full factorial design. Each processmade a distinct contribution to the evolution of sharedsymbols: sign alignment directly influencedcommunication success, and concurrent partnerfeedback drove sign simplification and symbolization.These complimentary processes led to the interactiveevolution of effective and efficient humancommunication systems.

A Neural Network Model for Taxonomic Responding with Realistic Visual Inputs

We propose a neural network model that accounts for the emer-gence of the taxonomic constraint in early word learning. Ourproposal is based on Mayor and Plunkett (2010)’s neurocom-putational model of the taxonomic constraint and overcomesone of its limitations, namely the fact that it considers arti-ficially built, simplified stimuli. In fact, while in the originalmodel the visual stimuli are random, sparse dot patterns, in ourproposed solution they are photographic images from the Im-ageNet database. In our model the represented objects in theimage can be of different size, color, location in the picture,point of view, etc.. We show that, notwithstanding the aug-mented complexity in the input, the proposed model comparesfavorably with respect to Mayor and Plunkett (2010)’s model.

How Many People Know? Representing the Distribution of Knowledge

The representation of the distribution of knowledge guidesinformation gathering, help seeking, and communication. Theresearch aimed to explore adults’ and 4-year-olds’representation of the distribution of common (conventionaland procedural) knowledge and expert knowledge associatedwith five occupations in their community. In addition, weexamined estimates of occupation-related everyday (non-expert) knowledge. Both groups estimated that commonknowledge is more widely held than expert knowledge, witheveryday knowledge in between. For adults, but not children,the distribution of expert knowledge was correlated withestimates of the proportion of people in each occupation.

The Effects of Shared Storybook Reading on Word Learning: A Meta-Analysis

Although a rich literature documents pre-literate children’s word learning success from shared storybook reading,a full synthesis of the factors which moderate these word learning effects has been largely neglected. This meta-analysisincluded 37 studies with 2,256 children, reflecting 104 effect sizes, investigating how the number of target words, tokens,story repetitions, reading styles and related factors moderate children’s word comprehension. Dialogic reading styles, tokens,the number of words tested and story repetition all moderated word learning effects. Children’s age, who read, number oftarget words and time between story and test were not moderators. These results provide information to guide researchers andeducators alike to the factors with the greatest impact on improving word learning from shared storybook reading.

Children Learn Words Better From One Storybook Page at a Time

Two experiments tested how the number of illustrations instorybooks influences 3.5-year-old children’s word learningfrom shared reading. In Experiment 1, children encounteredstories with either two illustrations, one illustration or onelarge illustration (in the control group) per spread. Childrenlearned significantly fewer words when they had to find thereferent within two illustrations presented at the same time. InExperiment 2 a gesture was added to guide children’sattention to the correct page in the two illustrations condition.Children who saw two illustrations with a guiding gesturelearned words as well as children who had seen only oneillustration per spread. Results are discussed in terms of thecognitive load of word learning from storybooks.

A Unified Model of Speech and Tool Use Early Development

Some studies hypothesize a strong interdependence betweenspeech and tool use development in the first two years of life.To help understand the underlying mechanisms, we presentthe first robotic model learning both speech and tool use fromscratch. It focuses on the role of one important form of bodybabbling where exploration is directed towards self-generatedgoals in free play, combined with imitation learning of a con-tingent caregiver. This model does not assume capabilities forcomplex sequencing and combinatorial planning which are of-ten considered necessary for tool use. Yet, we show that themechanisms in this model allow a learner to progressively dis-cover how to grab objects with the hand, how to use objectsas tools to reach further objects, how to produce vocal sounds,and how to leverage these vocal sounds to use a caregiver asa social tool to retrieve objects. Also, the discovery that cer-tain sounds can be used as a social tool further guides vocallearning. This model predicts that the grounded exploration ofobjects in a social interaction scenario should accelerate infantvocal learning of accurate sounds for these objects’ names.

What counts as math?Relating conceptions of math with anxiety about math

What do people think of when they think of “math?” We pro-pose that individuals may have very different working defini-tions of the category of math, and that those with broader mathconceptions may have less math anxiety. In Study 1, we intro-duce a method for indexing the “breadth” of individuals’ mathconceptions, and show that there is an inverse relation betweenconception breadth and math anxiety. These results suggestthat math anxiety is related both to how expansive individualsperceive math to be, and how skillful they feel at the activitiesthey think it could involve. Study 2 attempts an intervention onstudents’ conceptions of math with a sample of middle schoolstudents. We find the same inverse relationship in students be-tween math conception breadth and math anxiety as found inadults. We discuss ongoing work that further explores quali-tative variation in math conceptions, and the lessons this mayhold for intervening on math anxiety.

After braking comes hasting: reversed effects of indirect associations in 2nd and 4th graders

The Associative Read-Out Model (AROM) suggests that associations between words can be defined by the log likelihood that they occur together more often in sentences than predicted by their single-word frequency. Moreover, semantic relations can be defined by associative spreading across many common associates. Here, we addressed developmental effects of associative and semantic priming. Thus, we manipulated sentence-co-occurrence- based direct (syntagmatic) and common (paradigmatic) associations between prime and target words in 2nd and 4th graders. Syntagmatic associations decreased response times and error rates in both, 2nd and 4th graders. Paradigmatic associations increased errors rates in 2nd graders, whereas they decreased errors rates in 4th graders. These results suggest that 2nd graders profit from syntagmatic, i.e. contiguity-based associations, while a benefit from paradigmatic-semantic relationship probably develops from generalizing across many of these simple associations.

TRACX2: a RAAM-like autoencoder modeling graded chunking in infant visual-sequence learning

Even newborn infants are able to extract structure from a stream of sensory inputs and yet, how this is achieved remains largely a mystery. We present a connectionist autoencoder model, TRACX2, that learns to extract sequence structure by gradually constructing chunks, storing these chunks in a distributed manner across its synaptic weights, and recognizing these chunks when they re-occur in the input stream. Chunks are graded rather than all-or-none in nature. As chunks are learned their component parts become more and more tightly bound together. TRACX2 successfully models the data from four experiments from the infant visual statistical-learning literature, including tasks involving low- salience embedded chunk items, part-sequences, and illusory items. The model also captures performance differences across ages through the tuning of a single learning rate parameter. These results suggest that infant statistical learning is underpinned by the same domain general learning mechanism that operates in auditory statistical learning and, potentially, in adult artificial grammar learning.1

Risk, Cognitive Control, and Adolescence: Challenging the Dual Systems Model

According to the dual systems model, adolescence is a period of imbalance between cognitive and motivational systems that results in increased tendency towards risk. In the study, we investigated the effects of rewards on risk-taking and cognitive control in 90 adolescents (13-16) and 96 adults (18- 35). Our results challenge the assumptions of the model as we observed that rewards lead adolescents to more conservative decisions in one of the risk tasks used in the study. We also observed that in cognitive control tasks, rewards influenced reaction latencies, but not the efficiency of control processes.

On an effective and efficient method for exploiting “wisdom of crowds in one mind”

Previous studies have shown that one can exploit “wisdom ofcrowds” by oneself. This is achieved by aggregating multiple“quasi-independent” estimates from the same person. However,previous methods were not necessarily easy to utilize. Therefore,we propose an efficient method based on perspective-taking. Theprocedure is as follows: First, one makes her/his own estimation.Second, one estimates again based on a different perspective(“general public”). Then these two estimations were averaged.Two experiments revealed that our method effectively induced thewisdom of crowds by oneself. More importantly, participants inour method made estimations more quickly than those in apreviously proposed method, suggesting that our method requireda relatively diminished cognitive load for participants. Furtherinvestigation suggested that our method was immune to adverseeffects of confidence. Therefore, the present findings show that ourmethod could be effective and efficient method for inducing thewisdom of crowds in one mind.

Experimental Investigation on Top-down and Bottom-up Processing in Graph Comprehension and Decision

Graph comprehension requires both bottom-up processing from the graph representation and top-down processing guided by knowledge and attitude. In the current study, we investigated which of the bottom-up process phases: extraction, interpretation, and decision: were affected by the top-down processing derived by the impressions and social attitudes. The experimental results showed that the top-down processing driven by impressions temporarily formed in specific contexts affected both the extraction of information and the following decision phase whereas top-down processing driven by attitude formed over a long time based on social norms affected only the decision phase. In the latter case, a decision was made without any need for bottom-up processing.

Measures and mechanisms of common ground: backchannels, conversationalrepair, and interactive alignment in free and task-oriented social interactions

A crucial aspect of everyday conversational interactions is ourability to establish and maintain common ground.Understanding the relevant mechanisms involved in suchsocial coordination remains an important challenge forcognitive science. While common ground is often discussedin very general terms, different contexts of interaction arelikely to afford different coordination mechanisms. In thispaper, we investigate the presence and relation of threemechanisms of social coordination – backchannels,interactive alignment and conversational repair – across freeand task-oriented conversations. We find significantdifferences: task-oriented conversations involve higherpresence of repair – restricted offers in particular – andbackchannel, as well as a reduced level of lexical andsyntactic alignment. We find that restricted repair isassociated with lexical alignment and open repair withbackchannels. Our findings highlight the need to explicitlyassess several mechanisms at once and to investigate diversesocial activities to understand their role and relations.

How do Speakers Coordinate Planning and Articulation? Evidence from Gaze- Speech Lags

How do speakers coordinate planning and articulation of more than one word at the same time? Here, we test whether they dynamically estimate how long it takes to (i) plan and (ii) articulate the words they intend to produce as a means of achieving such coordination. German speakers named two pictures without pausing, while their eye-movements were recorded. In line with previous reports, after their gaze left the first picture, speakers took longer to start speaking (i.e., the gaze-speech lag was longer) when the name of the first picture was shorter. But while gaze-speech lags were also longer when the second picture was harder to name, the two effects did not interact. We argue that speakers’ flexible planning abilities might be accounted for by reactive, rather than proactive planning mechanisms.

Automated Generation of Cognitive Ontology via Web Text-Mining

A key goal of cognitive science is to understand and map therelationship between cognitive processes. Previous workshave manually curated cognitive terms and relations,effectively creating an ontology, but do they reflect howcognitive scientists study cognition in practice? In addition,cognitive science should provide theories that informexperimentalists in neuroscience studying implementations ofcognition in the brain. But do neuroscientists and cognitivescientists study the same things? We set out to answer thesequestions in a data-driven way by text-mining and automatedclustering to build a cognitive ontology from existingliterature. We find automatically generated relationships to bemissing in existing ontologies, and that cognitive science doesnot always inform neuroscience. Thus, our work serves as anefficient hypothesis-generating mechanism, inferringrelationships between cognitive processes that can bemanually refined by experts. Furthermore, our resultshighlight the gap between theories of cognition and the studyof their implementation.

“If It Matters, I Can Explain It”: Social Desirability of Knowledge Increasesthe Illusion of Explanatory Depth

This paper explores whether social desirability affects theillusion of explanatory depth (IEOD) by comparing themagnitude of this illusion in topics with different levels ofsocial desirability within several domains. This question waschosen because prior literature shows that social expectationsabout how much a person should know about a certain topicaffect the magnitude of the IOED. Previous research showsalso that social desirability has an effect on a similar illusionrelated to argumentation, and that the IOED is affected by theway a person thinks knowledge is distributed in his or hersocial group. In order to do so, 184 participants were assignedrandomly to three knowledge domains (history, economics,and devices) and in each domain they rated theirunderstanding of a high-desirability and a low-desirabilitytopic following a standard IOED procedure. Results show thatsocial desirability has an effect on the IOED magnitude andthat overestimation of understanding varies among domains.Particularly, participants tend to overestimate theirunderstanding of high desirability topics only. This effect wasstronger in the historical domain.

Reasoning with Fundamental Rights

People often withdraw previously drawn conclusions in light of new information. This defeasible reasoning is also im- portant for law, where judges often have to change their ver- dicts in light of new evidence. Here we investigate defeasibil- ity in the context of conflicting fundamental rights. When, for instance, law to property conflicts with law to information, can one of these rights be “defeated” by the other? We em- bedded conflicting fundamental rights in inference tasks (Ex- periment 1) and in elaborated vignettes (Experiment 2). Re- sults show that people decide between two conflicting funda- mental rights in a rational way. Case by case, participants pro- tected that fundamental right whose violation evoked the highest moral outrage (Experiment 1) or whose violation was considered to be more serious (Experiment 2). We discuss the implications of our findings for law theory and psychology.

The Pragmatic Parliament: A Framework for Socially-Appropriate UtteranceSelection in Artificial Agents

One of the hallmarks of human natural language (NL) inter-action is the ability for people to balance a variety of so-cial and communicative goals when choosing how to realizetheir speech actions. These goals can include pragmatic criteriasuch as correctness, informativeness, and brevity (i.e., Griceanconversational maxims) or social factors such as politeness.However, there currently does not exist a general algorithmicmethod to explicitly modulate language generated by artificialagents based on an arbitrary number of pragmatic and socialcriteria. We propose a novel method to accomplish this task,in which rankings of candidate utterances by different prag-matic or social criteria are fused by use of a voting algorithm.We then give a proof-of-concept demonstration of the applica-tion of this method in the context of directive generation forhuman-robot interaction.

A Two-Stage Model of Solving Arithmetic Problems

This paper examines a process of solving different types of counterfactual arithmetic problems (problems contradicted a visual experience, an experience of temperature, encyclopedic knowledge, etc.) in comparison with their ‘real’ counterparts by different types of subjects (e.g., educated in math and educated in humanities). As a result, a two-stage model of solving arithmetic problems is outlined in the paper.

Discourse Acquisition in ‘Pear Stories ’ of Preschool-aged Children

This work focuses on an issue situated at the intersection of two domains: the oral mode of communication vs. the written mode of communication, and language acquisition. The backbone of this research is a conjecture that, for some age groups (babies, toddlers and preschool-aged children), to explore the acquisition of discourse as a whole (including gestures, facial expressions, prosody, pauses and discursive markers, etc.) is more appropriate than explore the acquisition of language exclusively. “The Pear Film” experimental line underpins the method of this research. The database comprises 74 ‘pear stories’ of Moscow preschool-aged children and high school students. Three parameters of the discourse are of interest for the authors: a logical structure and a coherence of the narrative; gestures and spontaneous movements lost any communicative meaning; discourse words and pauses.

Inductive reasoning influences perception of interspecies disease transmission risk

Zoonoses (diseases that enter the human population via animalcontact) are a major global health concern. Because of howzoonoses emerge, understanding human reasoning about therisk factors associated with animal contact is central to com-bating their spread. However, little is known about the factorsthat influence perception of these risks. We present an induc-tive account of zoonosis risk perception, suggesting that it isinfluenced by beliefs about the range of animals that are able totransmit diseases to each other. In Study 1, we find that partic-ipants who endorse higher likelihoods of cross-species diseasetransmission have stronger intention to report animal bites. InStudy 2, adapting real world descriptions of Ebola virus fromthe WHO and CDC, we find that communications conveyinga broader range of animals as susceptible to a disease increaseintentions to report animal bites and decrease perceived safetyof wild game meat. These findings suggest that cognitive fac-tors may be harnessed to modulate zoonosis risk perceptionand combat emerging infectious diseases.

High-Performing Readers Underestimate Their Text Comprehension: Artifact or Psychological Reality?

We focused on the controversy whether high-performing readers consistently underestimate their comprehension or are prone to detrimental overestimations as much as less skilled readers are. Therefore, we conducted an experiment (N = 105 university students) to investigate judgment bias as a function of reading skill and text difficulty in terms of text cohesion. Results showed that the easy text produced underestimation of comprehension, whereas the hard text led to overestimation. Furthermore, readers with higher reading skills were less prone to overestimate their comprehension of a hard text than less skilled readers. However, we also found that more skilled readers showed lower sensitivity in discriminating between correct and incorrect answers than less skilled readers. Overall, our results do not support the idea that high-performing readers consistently underestimate their text comprehension. Findings are discussed with respect to readers’ awareness of different text-based judgment cues and their (beliefs about their) reading skill.

Computational modeling of auditory spatial attention

Attention plays a fundamental role in higher-level cognition. In this paper we develop a computational model for how auditory spatial attention is distributed in space. Our model builds on the assumption that attentional bias has bottom-up and top-down components. We represent each component and their synthesis as a map, associating a level of attentional bias to locations in space. The maps and their interaction are modeled using an artificial intelligence approach based on constraints. We describe the behavioral task we have designed to measure the attentional bias and discuss the results. We then test different hypotheses on the shape and interaction modalities of the maps in terms of how well they fit our behavioral data. The findings showed that combining top-down and bottom-up spatial attention gradients that differ in their spatial properties produced the best fit to behavioral data, and suggested several novel mechanisms for future testing.

Recruitment of the motor system in the perception of handwritten andtyped characters

Many different functional roles have been ascribed to themotor system due to its prevalent recruitment in perceptualand cognitive tasks other than motor production. We discussfindings that suggest the motor system might take on multipleroles that vary with context and the brain networks involved.Using single-pulse TMS, we measured the corticospinalexcitability of the FDI muscle in primary motor cortex asparticipants viewed words that were either typed orhandwritten. We observed consistent facilitation ofcorticospinal excitability during reading of handwritten text.Although we observed facilitation in corticospinal excitabilityduring the presentation of typed text, this effect decreasedwith repetitive presentations of stimuli. We suggest that thefacilitation during presentation of typed words is a case ofaction simulation, and that the diminishing facilitation in thecase of typed stimuli is representative of sensory predictionby the motor system. These findings suggest that we shouldconsider multiple roles for motor recruitment during theobservation of visual stimuli, taking context intoconsideration.

A Spiking Independent Accumulator Model for Winner-Take-All Computation

Winner-take-all (WTA) mechanisms are an important compo-nent of many cognitive models. For example, they are oftenused to decide between multiple choices or to selectively di-rect attention. Here we compare two biologically plausible,spiking neural WTA mechanisms. We first provide a novelspiking implementation of the well-known leaky, competingaccumulator (LCA) model, by mapping the dynamics onto apopulation-level representation. We then propose a two-layerspiking independent accumulator (IA) model, and compare itsperformance against the LCA network on a variety of WTAbenchmarks. Our findings suggest that while the LCA net-work can rapidly adapt to new winners, the IA network is bet-ter suited for stable decision making in the presence of noise.

Folk intuitions about consciousness

In science and philosophy, there is still no general agreement on what ‘consciousness’ is. But how do normalpeople (with no education in psychology or philosophy) use the term in their everyday life? What is the folk understandingof the word “conscious”? We conducted an online study on how the general public uses the word “consciousness” in theirdaily life. Participants (n=445) answered the question “What is consciousness?” in four different formats: they (1) generatedfree definitions in their own words, (2) generated many synonyms, (3) generated one synonym, or (4) selected one alternativedescription in a multiple choice task. The most frequent words were: alertness, clarity, I-sensation, knowledge, perception,reflecting, and thinking. The word perception was provided most often across all formats. There was also a high correlationbetween all response formats. We discuss these findings and their implications for the scientific study of consciousness.

Pragmatic aspects of spatial language acquisition and use across languages

Across languages, back is produced earlier and more frequently than front. This asymmetry has been attributed either to a conceptual/semantic asymmetry in the early meanings of these locatives (with back being more basic than front; conceptual immaturity account) or to the fact that Back configurations are inherently more ‘noteworthy’ than Front configurations (pragmatic account). Here, we tested the two accounts. In Study 1, children and adult speakers of English and Greek described Front/Back motion events. In Study 2, adult speakers of 10 additional languages described the same events. Despite cross-linguistic differences, speakers of all age and language groups typically used more Back than Front adpositions; furthermore, they often encoded Back information in occlusion verbs (e.g. hide) but no such verbs were available for Front. Thus, the front/back asymmetry is not due to children’s conceptual immaturity but should be linked to pragmatic factors that also shape adult spatial language production cross-linguistically.

Cognitive Style Predicts Magical Beliefs

Magicians often rely on misdirection to fool their audience. Acommon way to achieve this is for the magician to provide aplausible and intuitive (but false) account of how an effect isperformed in order to prevent spectators from uncovering thetruth. We hypothesized that analytical thinkers would be morelikely than intuitive thinkers to seek alternative explanationswhen observing a mental magic effect because generating acoherent explanation requires analytical thought. We foundthat while intuitive thinkers often espoused explanations for amagic trick similar to one provided by the magician,analytical thinkers tended to generate new explanations thatechoed rational principles and relied on physical mechanisms(rather than mental capabilities). This difference was notpredicted by differences in numeracy skills or need forcognition.

Stopping Rules in Information Acquisition at Varying Probabilities andConsequences: an EEG Study

An experiment aiming to assess the use of stopping rules ininformation acquisition was performed. Participants wererequested to make a decision in 24 financial scenarios withthe possibility of buying information pieces. Behavioral andEEG data were recorded for analysis. Results showed thatparticipants followed Bayesian calculations in order todetermine a stop on information acquisition and decide.Moreover, the information acquisition strategies wereconsistent with prospect theory, in which participants willweigh information pieces differently and seek more or lessinformation given different manipulations in scenarioprobability and consequences. EEG data suggest SlowCortical Potentials at fronto-central electrodes.

Solving additive word problems: Intuitive strategies make the difference

Young children use informal strategies to solve arithmetic word problems. The Situation Strategy First (SSF) framework claims that these strategies prevail even after instruction. The present study was conducted with second grade students in order to investigate the persistence of intuitive, situation- based strategies, on word problems that do not involve dynamic temporal changes. This is challenging for the SSF framework, since the lack of this dimension might bypass intuitive strategies. The results revealed that intuitive strategies persist, are valid for these types of problems, and impact the problems' difficulty. Indeed problems that require the application of arithmetic principles remain hard, even though they have been practiced at school. These findings provide complementary evidence to how mental calculation strategies articulate with arithmetic word problem solving and call for the extension of the SSF framework.

Age differences in language comprehension during driving: Recovery from prediction errors is more effortful for older adults

Prior research yielded conflicting findings regarding whether older adults show a greater processing cost than younger adults when encountering unpredicted semantic material during language processing. Here, we investigated whether age-related differences in recovery from prediction error are influenced by increased demands on working memory. We used a dual task design: a primary sentence comprehension task in which semantic predictions were fulfilled or violated, and a concurrent driving task, thought to limit working memory resources in resolving prediction errors. In the dual task, older participants showed an increase in comprehension accuracy for sentences with semantic violations, while demonstrating a decrease in driving accuracy. Thus, when working memory resources were limited, older adults focused exclusively on the language task and neglected the driving task. This could be related to an age-related increase in generating semantic predictions, or to a general inability among older adults to divide attention between two cognitively demanding tasks.

Asymmetric detection of changes in volatility: Implications for risk perception

Variance of the outcomes associated with an option often provides a measure of the riskiness of that option. Hence, it is important for organisms are able to detect any sudden changes in outcome variance. In Experiment 1, we presented people with graphs of share price time series or water level time series. In half the graphs, variance (financial or flooding risk) changed at some point. People were better at detecting increases than decreases in risk - maybe because it is more important to detect increases in danger than decreases in it. However, in Experiment 2, people were still better at detecting increases than decreases in variance even when those changes did not reflect altered levels of risk. Our findings may reflect the fact that the actual change in variance exceeds the change needed to identify a regime change in variance by a larger amount for upward than for downward changes.

Compound effects of expectations and actual behaviors in human-agentinteraction: Experimental investigation using the Ultimatum Game

This study investigated how the expectations of others (i.e.,top-down processes) and actual perceived behavior (i.e.,bottom-up processes) influence negotiations during human-agent interactions. Participants took part in several sessions ofthe ultimatum game; we investigated the bargaining strategiesdirected toward the computer agent. To investigate the influ-ence of top-down and bottom-up processes on performance,we designed an experiment wherein (1) participants expectedtheir partners were humans or agents, and (2) agents used dif-ferent types of algorithmic behavior. Results revealed that ir-rational decisions, which are characteristic of human-humaninteractions, emerged when participants believed their oppo-nents were human and when opponent behaviors were ambigu-ous. Further, we found participants adopted different bargain-ing strategies according to their expectations and the agent’sspecific algorithmic behavior. We discuss interplay of the twotypes of cognitive processing in human-agent interaction.

The role of presentation order and orientation on information search and evaluations: An eye-tracking study

Previous research conducted by Bergus et al. (2002) identified that treatment evaluations are more negative when risks are presented last. Extending discussion of this order effect, the current studies investigate this effect in tabular style displays, manipulating both order and orientation; and using eye-tracking methodology, explores the effect of these variables on the information search process. Analysis from eye-tracking data revealed a tendency to read information sets sequentially (i.e. read all risk information before transitions to the other set), which is stronger for the vertical orientation where switching between information sets is less common. Further, while balanced search was observed when benefits presented first, when presented with the risks first, search becomes more risk- heavy. Eye-tracking measures did not strongly predict treatment evaluations, although, when holding other variables constant, time proportion spent on benefits positively predicted treatment evaluations.

Risk and Rationality in Decisions to Commit Crime

Criminal behavior and related disorders have been associated with abnormal neural activity when experiencingor anticipating risks and rewards, as well as when exercising inhibition. However, behavioral and neural substrates of riskpreferences and criminality have received scant attention when unconfounded with experience, anticipation, and inhibition. Wetest predictions of fuzzy-trace theory (FTT) in two experiments using a risky-choice framing task. Behavioral results show thatindividuals with a greater history of criminal behavior are less likely to engage in simple meaning-based processing and areless confident when doing so. These findings are supported by fMRI results showing a greater history of criminal behavioris associated with increased activation in regions associated with cognitive control when engaging in simple meaning-basedprocessing. These results provide insight into the cognitive processes and brain mechanisms that are associated with criminalbehavior.

Planning in Action: Interactivity Improves Planning Performance

Planning is an everyday activity that is extended in timeand space, yet is frequently studied in the absence ofinteractivity. Successful planning relies on an array ofexecutive functions including self-control. Weinvestigated the effects of interactivity and self-controlon planning using a sequential-task paradigm. Half ofthe participants first completed a video-viewing taskrequiring self-control of visual attention, whereas theother half completed the same task without the self-control constraint. Next, and within each of thesegroups, half of the participants manipulated cards tocomplete their plan (high-interactivity condition); for theother half, plans were made with their hands down (low-interactivity condition). Planning performance wassignificantly better in the high- than in the low-interactivity conditions; however the self-controlmanipulation had no impact on planning performance.An exploration of individual differences revealed thatlong-term planning ability and non-planningimpulsiveness moderated the impact of interactivity onplanning. These findings suggest that interactivityaugments working memory resources and planningperformance, underscoring the importance of aninteractive perspective on planning research.

Head and Heart Metaphors for Moral Decision Making:Conceptual or Communicative?

When faced with a moral dilemma, following your headversus your heart can result in very different decisions. Earlierwork has argued that people who “self-locate” in the headtend to make more rational and less emotional decisions tomoral dilemmas than those who “self-locate” in the heart. Wereplicate this finding, suggest an alternative interpretation ofthe result, and then extend it with a novel experiment. In ametaphor framing task, we manipulated the salience of thehead/heart metaphors—by using them (a) in a single sentence,(b) a more elaborate paragraph, or (c) by emphasizing one incontrast to the other. We found that people who received thehead metaphor made more rational decisions than those whoreceived the heart metaphor, but only in the high saliencecondition that contrasted the two metaphors. This findingillustrates the communicative value of metaphor, which canbe enhanced through comparison.

When metaphors in the mind become metaphors in the mouth:Documenting the emergence of a new system of linguistic metaphors for time

Languages exhibit striking semantic diversity, but differentlanguages often share core metaphors. Conceptual MetaphorTheory (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980) claims that universalhuman experiences give rise to conceptual representationsthat are then expressed in language. But languages changeslowly, making it difficult to observe implicitconceptualization affecting linguistic convention in realtime. Here, we describe a shared conceptualizationpreviously absent from speech that has now becomeconventionalized in linguistic metaphors. In two studies, wedocument how members of the US military talk about timeusing conventionalized lateral metaphors (e.g., ‘push themeeting right’ to mean ‘move the meeting later’). We showthat military members, unlike civilians, consider suchsentences to be acceptable—sometimes even moreacceptable than more standard phrases. Moreover, militarypersonnel seem unaware that these lateral metaphors arespecific to their linguistic sub-community. Our findingssuggest that implicit mental representations can becomeconventionalized metaphors in language.

Boosting Knowledge-Building with Cognitive Dialog Games

Dialog game tools are text chat applications which aim tostructure and promote students' collaborative learning byhaving them select a label and sentence-opener for eachmessage they type to their learning partner. In thisexperiment, we compared students’ learning from discussionsvia a dialog game tool to their learning via a standard freechatapplication. Students discussed topic questions with alearning partner. They then individually completed a multiplechoice test, for assessing knowledge-gain, and a short-answertest, to assess readiness for knowledge-building. Resultssuggest that dialog games applications lead to increasedreadiness for knowledge-building, in the form of integratingdistinct pieces of learned knowledge, than freechatapplications. Follow-up analyses suggest that the degree ofconcept overlap between students' dialog messages and topickeywords, as measured by a "semantic fingerprint" system, isa potentially useful metric for predicting students'knowledge-building. Implications and potential applicationsof our findings are discussed

Leveraging mutual exclusivity for faster cross-situational wordlearning: A theoretical analysis

Past mechanistic accounts of children’s word learningclaim that a simple type of cross-situational learning ispowerful enough to match observed rates of learning,even in quite ambiguous situations. However, a limita-tion in some of these analyses is their reliance on an un-realistic assumption that the learner only hears a word insituations containing the intended referent. This studyanalyzed a more general type of cross-situational learn-ing based on the relative frequency of word-object pairs,and found it to be slower than the simple mechanismanalyzed in prior work. We then analytically exploredwhether relative-frequency learning can be improved byincorporating the mutual exclusivity (ME) principle–an assumption that words map to objects 1-to-1. Ouranalyses show that with a certain type of correlation inword-to-word relationship, ME makes relative frequencylearning as efficient as fast-mapping, which can learn aword in one exposure.

Perceptions of Psychological Momentum in Basketball

Psychological momentum (PM) and the hot hand are relatedconcepts describing people’s beliefs regarding streaks ofsuperior performance. This study examined the susceptibilityof perceptions of PM to changes in the streakiness ofotherwise equivalent series. Fifty-five male participants (31basketballers and 24 control) completed a ‘hot-cognition’experiment where they rated individual and team momentumand assessed the likelihood of a future shot’s success afterwatching sequences of basketball shots. The experimentalmanipulation of the order of shots strongly affectedparticipants’ ratings of momentum and, less strongly, theprobability they assigned to the future shot (i.e. the hot handeffect). Basketballers showed stronger reactions tomanipulations of order than the controls, which could beattributed to greater investment in the task. The resultsdemonstrate the importance of distinguishing between PMand the hot hand and also provide a valuable extension ofprior work showing such effects into more realistic scenarios.

Translating a Reinforcement Learning Task into a Computational Psychiatry Assay: Challenges and Strategies

Computational psychiatry applies advances from computational neuroscience to psychiatric disorders. A core aim is to develop tasks and modeling approaches that can advance clinical science. Special interest has centered on reinforcement learning (RL) tasks and models. However, laboratory tasks in general often have psychometric weaknesses and RL tasks pose special challenges. These challenges must be addressed if computational psychiatry is to capitalize on its promise of developing sensitive, replicable assays of cognitive function. Few resources identify these challenges and discuss strategies to mitigate them. Here, we first overview general psychometric challenges associated with laboratory tasks, as these may be unfamiliar to cognitive scientists. Next, we illustrate how these challenges interact with issues specific to RL tasks, in the context of presenting a case example of preparing an RL task for computational psychiatry. Throughout, we highlight how considering measurement issues prior to a clinical science study can inform study design.

The Meanings of Morality: Investigating the psychometric properties ofdistributed representations of latent moral concepts

People’s beliefs about what is morally right and wrong vary widely between individuals, contexts, and cultures;however it is thought that they are governed by core latent constructs. While there is evidence that these constructs are reflectedin natural language, this requires further testing. We demonstrate that the structure of moral values in natural discourse can bemodeled by applying factor analyses to distributed representations of morally relevant terms learned by a neural network. Wefirst demonstrate that robust latent constructs can be estimated from the covariance of distributed representations of constructexemplars. We then test whether the factor structure proposed by Moral Foundations Theory (MFT) is reflected in naturallanguage. Finally, we conduct a bottom-up investigation of the structure of moral values in natural language using free-responses reported by participants. Ultimately, we find evidence that the representation of moral values in natural languagepartially corresponds to MFT.

Probability judgement from samples: accurate estimates and the conjunctionfallacy

This paper investigates a fundamental conflict in the literatureon people’s probability estimation. Research on ‘perception’of probability shows that people are accurate in their estimatesof probability of various simple events from samples. Equally,however, a large body of research shows that people’s probabil-ity estimates are fundamentally biased, and subject to reliableand striking fallacies in reasoning. We investigate this con-flict in an experiment that examines the occurrence of the con-junction fallacy in a probability perception task where peopleare asked to estimate the probability of simple and conjunc-tive events in a presented set of items. We find that people’sprobability estimates are accurate, especially for simple events,just as seen in previous studies. People’s estimates also showhigh rates of occurrence of the conjunction fallacy. We showhow this apparently contradictory result is consistent with arecent model of probability estimation, the probability theoryplus noise’ model.

Disfluencies in dialogues with patients with schizophrenia

Disfluencies such as self-repairs, filled pauses such as ‘um’and silent pauses are pervasive in dialogue, but there is no con-sensus in the literature as to whether they reflect internal pro-duction pressures, or interactive issues – or how their effectsare manifest in dialogue. It is well-known that patients withschizophrenia have problems with language and social cogni-tive skills, yet little research has investigated how these impactinteraction. We report a study on the disfluency behaviours ofpatients with schizophrenia and their interlocutors who wereunaware of the patient’s diagnosis, compared to healthy con-trol groups. Results show that patients use fewer self-repairsthan either their partners or controls and fewer filled pauses(‘er’, ‘um’) than controls. Furthermore, the presence of the pa-tient also affects patients’ partners, who use fewer filled pausesthan controls and more unfilled pauses than both patients andcontrols. This suggests that smooth coordination of turns isproblematic in patients’ dialogues

Moral Action Changes Mind Perception for Human and Artificial Moral Agents

Mind perception is studied for three different agents: a human,an artificial human, and a humanoid robot. The artificiallycreated agents are presented as being undistinguishable from ahuman. Each agent is rated on 15 mental capacities. Three mindperception dimensions are identified - Experience, Agency,and Cognition. The artificial agents are rated higher on theCognition dimensions than on the other two dimensions. Thehumanoid robot is rated lower than the human on theExperience dimension. These results show that people ascribeto artificial agents some mental capacities more than others. Ina second experiment, the effect of agent’s moral action on mindperception is explored. It is found that when the artificial agentshave undertaken a moral action, they are perceived to besimilar to the human agent. More interestingly, the presentationof the moral action leads to a restructuring of the dimensionsof mind perception.

Reconsideration on Linking Eye-movement Data with Argument Realization

Previous studies have found a processing difference between unaccusative sentences and unergative sentences. Theyargued that the difference is derived from a syntactic difference, i.e. an unaccusative sentence involves movement whereas anunergative sentence does not. In this study, we are going to show that those studies are uninterpretable due to uncontrolledstimuli and confounds in the materials. After examining their data anlysis, we argue that the effects they found does seem to bestable. This reopens the question whether there is a syntactic difference between unaccusative and unergative verbs.

A New Model of Statistical Learning: Trajectories Through Perceptual Similarity Space

Existing models of statistical learning involve computation ofconditional probabilities over discrete, categorical items in asequence. We propose an alternative view that learning occursthrough a process of tracking changes along physicaldimensions from one stimulus to the next within a “perceptualsimilarity space.” To test this alternative, we examined asituation where it is difficult or impossible to label stimuli inreal time, and where the two assumptions lead to conflictinghypotheses. We conducted two experiments in which humanparticipants passively listened to a familiarization sequence offrequency-modulated tones and were then asked to makefamiliarity judgments on a series of test bigrams. Behavioralresults were broadly consistent with a conceptualization oflearning as tracking trajectories through perceptual similarityspace. We also trained a neural network that codes stimuli asvalues along two continuous dimensions to predict the nextstimulus given the current stimulus, and show that it capturedkey features of the human data.

The role of prior knowledge and expertise on choice of referring expression

Referential success depends on choice of referring expression.The choice of referring expression will depend on contextualfactors as well as factors related to speaker and addresseeknowledge. A shared-learning paradigm was used in whichpartners learned names of objects together and separately beforea referential task. Items differed on commonality, with someindependently rated as more common and some as more rare.Speakers were less likely to use names versus other forms whenitems were rare than common (p<0.001) and less likely to usenames when items were new than learned together (p<0.001).Asymmetry effects showed that speakers were more likely touse a name when the addressee was deemed moreknowledgeable in post-test ratings (p<0.01). Together, we takethis to show speakers choose to use a name versus a descriptionbased on the likelihood that their interlocutor will know thename. Factors affecting the likelihood include prior knowledgeof what a typical addressee will know and shared experience,which includes inferring an interlocutor’s expertise, asdynamically updated during a dialog.

Beyond Almost-Sure Termination

The aim of this paper is to argue that models in cognitivescience based on probabilistic computation should not be re-stricted to those procedures that almost surely (with probabil-ity 1) terminate. There are several reasons to consider non-terminating procedures as candidate components of cognitivemodels. One theoretical reason is that there is a perfect cor-respondence between the enumerable semi-measures and allprobabilistic programs, as we demonstrate here (generalizinga better-known fact about computable measures and almost-surely halting programs). One practical reason is that the linebetween almost sure termination and non-termination is elu-sive, as well as arbitrary. We argue that this matters not onlyfor theorists, but also potentially for a learner faced with thetask of inducing programs from experience.

The Influence of Speaker’s Gaze on Sentence Comprehension: An ERPInvestigation

Behavioral studies demonstrate the influence of speakergaze in visually-situated spoken language comprehension.We present an ERP experiment examining the influence ofspeaker’s gaze congruency on listeners’ comprehension of ref-erential expressions related to a shared visual scene. Wedemonstrate that listeners exploit speakers’ gaze toward ob-jects in order to form sentence continuation expectations:Compared to a congruent gaze condition, we observe an in-creased N400 when (a) the lack of gaze (neutral) does not al-low for upcoming noun prediction, and (b) when the noun vi-olates gaze-driven expectations (incongruent). The later alsoresults in a late (sustained) positivity, reflecting the need to up-date the assumed situation model. We take the combinationof the N400 and late positivity as evidence that speaker gazeinfluences both lexical retrieval and integration processes, re-spectively (Brouwer et al., in press). Moreover, speaker gazeis interpreted as reflecting referential intentions (Staudte &Crocker, 2011).

In search for the relevant space of implicit memory deficit in dyslexia

Studies of dyslexics, whose implicit memory is impaired,suggest that their implicit inference of sound statistics and itsintegration into perception is inefficient. Specifically,dyslexics' implicit memory decays faster and consequentlyonly accumulates information over shorter temporal windows.We now ask whether this abnormal dynamic is domain generalby measuring its cortical distribution. We measure thedynamics of behavioral context effects and the concurrentneural adaptation during fast acquisition fMRI. We find asimilar pattern of fast decay of adaptation across a broad rangeof cortical areas, though most significant effects are found inauditory cortex. This broad neural distribution suggests that therelevant aspect of implicit statistical inferences is not the natureof stimuli, but their temporal distribution.

Cognition Influencing Auditory Perception in SLD Children: Revisiting the Models of Auditory Processing

The study assessed the auditory processing abilities and the cognitive skills in children with specific learning disability. It investigates the top-down or bottom-up influence on auditory processing. Using a test battery approach, the association between cognitive skills (verbal working memory and attention) and auditory processing abilities (auditory closure, binaural integration and temporal processing skills) has been measured. The results revealed that cognitive processes significantly affect the bottom-up auditory perception. The effect of cognition was more evident in speech processing than non-speech signal processing. These findings may be useful in designing appropriate therapeutic protocol for children with specific learning disability.

Failure to use probability of success in deciding whether to pursue onegoal or two.

Difficult tasks should be attempted one at a time, while easytasks can be undertaken in parallel. Reinforcing our previ-ous conclusion that people are surprisingly poor at applyingthis logic, we find people fail to select standing positions thatmaximize their probability of success in throwing a beanbaginto one of two possible hoops. We asked participants to ex-plicitly report their odds of successfully throwing a beanbaginto each hoop from the location they had chosen to stand,and estimates were highly accurate. Nonetheless, participantsfailed to use estimates of success appropriately to maximizesuccess, suggesting a failure of insight, rather than limited orinaccurate information, can account for suboptimal decisionsabout standing position.

Algebra is not like trivia:Evaluating self-assessment in an online math tutor

Appraising one’s own performance after a task, known as self-assessment, has been studied from a cognitive science perspec-tive in domains such as humor, trivia, and logic. Previous stud-ies have found that participants are systematically poor at judg-ing their own performance, though sometimes self-assessmentvaries based on actual performance. We explored calibrationof self-assessment on algebra problems, a domain where peo-ple have typically received explicit instruction. In this domain,we found that people do not behave as they do in other do-mains previously studied: they are generally well-calibrated injudging their algebra performance. This suggests that in thecourse of learning to solve algebra problems, people have alsolearned to accurately judge their performance, both absolutelyand relative to others.

Reasoning ability predicts irrational worldview but not conspiracy belief

Previous research showed that individual tendency to believe in conspiracy theories is related to numerous social, personality, and cognitive variables. Moreover, such a tendency may reflect a broader trait for epistemic irrationality, which drives other pseudo- scientific and paranormal beliefs. However, the relationship between conspiracy belief and reasoning ability (fluid intelligence; Gf) was not sufficiently studied to date, even though Gf level strongly influence the way in which individuals think and reason. Using confirmatory factor analysis, we found the robust link between conspiracy belief and other irrational beliefs. All those irrational beliefs were also substantially related to the close- minded cognitive style. However, even though Gf significantly predicted other irrational beliefs, it explained less than 2% of variance in conspiracy belief. This result suggests that effective reasoning cannot prevent even highly intelligent people from endorsing conspiracy theories.

The Motor System Does Not Use a Curvilinear Impetus Belief:Folk Physics and Embodied Cognition

Previous work shows that people often believe, contrary toactual physics, that objects travelling in a curved path througha tube will continue to travel in a curved path after exiting thetube. In the present study, previous work was replicated, butaccuracy increased in a new condition in which people wereasked to catch an actual ball emerging from a tube. That is, inthis case there is a discrepancy between how we believe theworld works, and how our motor system responds to events inthe world. This finding supports the theory that the perceptionand action systems of the brain use different methods topredict how things move in the world, and that the abstractreasoning systems used to explain how the world works areoften in conflict with the action systems.

Network Analysis of a Large Sample of Typical and Late Talkers

The focus of this paper is to examine differences in semantic network structure of late talkers and typical talkers to elucidate potential learning strategies used by late talking children. To address this question, we conducted network analysis on the vocabularies of 2,912 children, with 566 of those being late talkers. Contrary to previously reported findings, the results show that late talkers have well-connected vocabularies as measured by median degree, clustering coefficient, and mean distance, with more well-connected networks in some cases than their typical talking peers. Further analysis of word order suggests that late talkers may be selecting based on frequency and connectivity of the words in the learning environment, more so than typical talkers. The language processing difficulties in late talkers appear not to be associated with their semantic network properties. In sum, late talkers may initially benefit from using word frequency and word connectivity strategies to build well-connected vocabularies.

Spatial language: Meaning, use, and lexical choice

Accounts of spatial language aim to address both the meaningof a spatial term and its usage patterns across diverse cases,but do not always clearly distinguish these from one another.Focusing on the case of English prepositions in and on, we setout to disentangle spatial language meaning from spatiallanguage use by comparing judgments on a series of linguistictasks designed to tap each aspect of spatial language. Wedemonstrate that judgments of truth-conditional meaning andpatterns of naturalistic use show different distributionalsignatures, with judgments of meaning giving rise to a moreuniform distribution than use patterns. We explore a thirdaspect of spatial language: lexical choice, and propose thatchoice is a key factor in shaping the distribution of spatialexpression use. Our analyses reveal that the distribution oflexical choice judgments is highly correlated with thedistribution of expression use in spatial descriptions for thesame spatial scenes, supporting a model of spatial languagethat differs from traditional accounts of meaning andcategorization.

Belief Digitization in Economic Prediction

Economic choices depend on our predictions of the future.Yet, at times predictions are not based on all relevantinformation, but instead on the single most likelypossibility, which is treated as though certainly the case—that is, digitally. Two sets of studies test whether thisdigitization bias would occur in higher-stakes economiccontexts. When making predictions about the future assetprices, participants ignored conditional probabilityinformation given relatively unlikely events and reliedentirely on conditional probabilities given the more likelyevents. This effect was found for both financial aggregatesand individual stocks, for binary predictions about thedirection and continuous predictions about expected values,and even when the “unlikely” event explicitly had aprobability as high as 30%; further, it was not moderatedby investing experience. Implications for behavioralfinance are discussed.

Measuring Abstract Mindsets through Syntax: Improvements in Automating theLinguistic Category Model

The Linguistic Category Model (LCM) was developed as a manual coding scheme for quantifying abstract mindsetsin human language. Previous attempts to computationally automate the LCM have relied primarily on pre-coded semanticfeatures, which fail to incorporate important contextual information integral to the LCM coding scheme. In this paper, weintroduce Syntax-LCM, a novel method for automating LCM coding using syntax and dependency tree features as predictors ofconstrual level. We compare the accuracy of Syntax-LCM to that of two previously used automated methods: LIWC LCM andBrysbaert concreteness ratings. We find support that the Syntax-LCM approximates the hand-coded LCM with higher accuracycompared to both the Brysbaert and the LIWC LCM. We also provide evidence that the syntactic features accounted for bySyntax-LCM mirror the inclusion criteria in the original coding manual and support theoretical relationships between distanceand abstract thinking.

Iconicity in Word Learning: What Can We Learn from Cross-SituationalLearning Experiments?

Iconicity, i.e. resemblance between form and meaning, is awidespread feature of natural language vocabulary (Perniss,Thompson, & Vigliocco, 2010), and has been shown tofacilitate vocabulary acquisition (Imai, Kita, Nagumo, &Okada). But what kind of advantage does iconicity actuallygive? Here we use cross-situational learning (Yu & Smith,2007), to address the question for sound-shape iconicity (theso-called kiki-bouba effect, Ramachandran & Hubbard,2001). In contrast to Monaghan, Mattock, and Walker (2012),Experiment 1 suggests that the iconicity advantage comesfrom referential disambiguation rather than more efficientmemory encoding. Experiments 2 and 3 replicate this result,and moreover show that the kiki-bouba effect is roughlyequally strong for sharp and rounded shapes, a property thatclassic experiments were unable to confirm, and which hasimplication for the effect’s mechanism

Mindfulness and Fear Conditioning

During mindfulness-based interventions participants can beinvited to bring aversive stimuli to mind while practicingmindfulness. This is thought to help the stimuli become lessaversive. However, the mechanisms underlying this processare not fully understood. In this study we explored these byexamining the effects of mindfulness practice and stimulusvisualization on stimuli associated with electric shocks.Participants were trained on a discrimination between twovisual stimuli using a standard electrodermal conditioningprocedure, in which one stimulus (CS+) was paired withshock and the other (CS-) was not. They then visualized eitherthe CS+ or CS-, while practicing mindfulness or performing acontrol activity. Following a number of extinction trials, theimpact of these manipulations was assessed during areacquisition test-phase. Both mindfulness and visualizationof the CS+ led to slower reacquisition of the CS+/shockassociation, when measured physiologically, and their effectswere additive. Moreover, these effects dissociated fromparticipants’ expectancy of shock. If confirmed in futurework, these findings may have implications for the treatmentof stimulus-specific anxiety.

A Study on the Impact of Chess Training on Creativity of Indian School Children

Creativity is the ability to produce work that is both novel and appropriate. The study, funded by Indian government, analyzed the effect of one-year chess training on the creativity of children. A pretest and posttest with control group design was used, with 31 children in experimental and 32 in control group. The experimental group underwent weekly chess training. Wallach-Kogan Creativity Test (Indian Adaptation) was used. Analysis revealed that only the experimental group had statistically significant gains in total creativity and two nonverbal subtests. The authors conclude that systematic chess training inculcates in the child the ability to think divergently and creatively.

Document Similarity Misjudgment by LSA: Misses vs. False Positives

Modeling text document similarity is an important yet challenging task. Even the most advanced computational linguistic models often misjudge document similarity relative to humans. Regarding the pattern of misjudgment between models and humans, Lee and colleagues (2005) suggested that the models’ primary failure is occasional underestimation of strong similarity between documents. According to this suggestion, there should be more extreme misses (i.e., models failing to pick up on strong document similarity) than extreme false positives (i.e., models falsely detecting document similarity that does not exist). We tested this claim by comparing document similarity ratings generated by humans and latent semantic analysis (LSA). Notably, we implemented LSA with 441 unique parameter settings, determined optimal parameters that yielded high correlations with human ratings, and finally identified misses and false positives under the optimal parameter settings. The results showed that, as Lee et al. predicted, large errors were predominantly misses rather than false positives. Potential causes of the misses and false positives are discussed.

"The Polar Express" is Bipolar: Critical Film Reviews Influence Uncanny Valley Phenomenon in Semi-Realistic Animation Films

Previous research suggests that semi-realistic animation films such as The Polar Express are representative of the uncanny valley (UV) hypothesis, which predicts that highly human- like artificial characters can appear eerie. In the present study, we investigated the extent to which critical film reviews can influence the perceived eeriness of such films. The reviews were adopted from authentic ones and expressed either negative or positive attitudes towards the animation techniques. Audiovisual speech asynchrony, which is known to induce eeriness, was included as an objective manipulation. Our results showed large review tone effects for both implicit and explicit eeriness evaluations. In contrast, speech asynchrony failed to elicit significant effects. These results demonstrate that critical film reviews representing opposite attitudinal poles can elicit consistent changes in the viewers’ evaluations of semi-realistic animations. The present findings cannot, however, be taken as evidence against the UV hypothesis itself in computer-generated characters.

Self-other distinction in the motor system during social interaction:A computational model based on predictive processing

During interaction with others, we perceive and produce so-cial actions in close temporal distance or even simultaneously.It has been argued that the motor system is involved in bothprocesses, but how does it distinguish in this processing be-tween self and other? In this paper, we present a model ofself-other distinction within a hierarchical sensorimotor sys-tem that is based on principles of perception-action couplingand predictive processing. For this we draw on mechanisms as-sumed for the integration of cues to generate sense of agency,i.e., the sense that an action is self-generated. We report re-sults from simulations of different social scenarios, showingthat the model is able to solve the problem of the dual use ofthe sensorimotor system.

A transfer advantage of learning diagrammatic representations of mathematics

This study examined learning and transfer of a simplemathematical concept when learning a symbolic sententialformat versus learning a diagrammatic format. Undergraduatecollege students learned an instantiation of a cyclic group andwere then given a test of a novel isomorphic group of thesame order followed by a test of a novel non-isomorphicgroup of a higher order. The results were that both thesentential and the diagrammatic formats led to successfullearning and transfer to the novel isomorphic group.However, only learning from the diagrammatic representationproduced successful transfer to the non-isomorphic group.These findings suggest that learning a diagrammaticrepresentation of a mathematical concept can have transferadvantages over learning strictly sentential formats.

Gricean epistemic reasoning in 4-year-olds

Recent experimental evidence suggests that adults incorporatespeaker knowledge into the derivation of pragmaticimplicatures. Developmental studies report that 5-year-oldchildren also succeed in taking speaker knowledge intoaccount in implicature computation, but 4-year-olds fail. Thepresent study investigated the pragmatic competence of 4-year-olds, specifically the ability to incorporate speakerknowledge into the derivation of ad hoc scalar implicatures.Using a simple paradigm inspired by referentialcommunication, we found that 4- year-olds are able toincorporate speaker knowledge into implicature derivation.These results have implications for our understanding of thelinguistic, pragmatic, and epistemic abilities of youngchildren.

A Hebbian account of entrenchment and (over)-extension in language learning

In production, frequently used words are preferentially extended to new, though related meanings. In comprehension, frequent exposure to a word instead makes the learner confident that all of the word’s legitimate uses have been experienced, resulting in an entrenched form-meaning mapping between the word and its experienced meaning(s). This results in a perception-production dissociation, where the forms speakers are most likely to map onto a novel meaning are precisely the forms that they believe can never be used that way. At first glance, this result challenges the idea of bidirectional form-meaning mappings, assumed by all current approaches to linguistic theory. In this paper, we show that bidirectional form-meaning mappings are not in fact challenged by this production-perception dissociation. We show that the production-perception dissociation is expected even if learners of the lexicon acquire simple symmetrical form-meaning associations through simple Hebbian learning.

Effects of Delayed Language Exposure on Spatial Language Acquisition by Signing Children and Adults

Deaf children born to hearing parents are exposed to language input quite late, which has long-lasting effects on language production. Previous studies with deaf individuals mostly focused on linguistic expressions of motion events, which have several event components. We do not know if similar effects emerge in simple events such as descriptions of spatial configurations of objects. Moreover, previous data mainly come from late adult signers. There is not much known about language development of late signing children soon after learning sign language. We compared simple event descriptions of late signers of Turkish Sign Language (adults, children) to age-matched native signers. Our results indicate that while late signers in both age groups are native-like in frequency of expressing a relational encoding, they lag behind native signers in using morphologically complex linguistic forms compared to other simple forms. Late signing children perform similar to adults and thus showed no development over time.

A Meta-Analysis of the Joint Simon Effect

Since its design in 2003, the joint Simon task and corollaryjoint Simon effect (JSE) have been invaluable tools towardsthe study of joint action and the understanding of howindividuals represent the action/task of a co-actor. Thepurpose of this meta-analysis was to systematically andquantitatively review the sizeable behavioural evidence forthe JSE. Google Scholar was used to identify studies citingthe first report of the joint Simon task (Sebanz, Knoblich, &Prinz, 2003) up until June 23, 2015. After screening, thirty-nine manuscripts were included in the meta-analysis, thirteenof which included individual go/no-go (IGNG) control data.Separate random-effects models were conducted for both thejoint Simon and IGNG datasets, and meta-regression modelswere used to assess potential moderators that may impact thestrength of the JSE. The results provide an importantquantitative summary of the literature and serve as afoundation for future research surrounding the JSE.

A Toolbox of Methods for Probabilistic Inference

We propose that probabilistic inference is supported by a men-tal toolbox that includes sampling and symmetry-based rea-soning in addition to several other methods. To flesh out thisclaim we consider a spatial reasoning task and describe a num-ber of different methods for solving the task. Several recentprocess-level accounts of probabilistic inference have focusedon sampling, but we present an experiment that suggests thatsampling alone does not adequately capture people’s infer-ences about our task.

The Narrow Conception of Computational Psychology

One particularly successful approach to modeling withincognitive science is computational psychology.Computational psychology explores psychological processesby building and testing computational models with humandata. In this paper, it is argued that a specific approach tounderstanding computation, what is called the ‘narrowconception’, has problematically limited the kinds of models,theories, and explanations that are offered withincomputational psychology. After raising two problems for thenarrow conception, an alternative, ‘wide approach’ tocomputational psychology is proposed.

Resolving Two Tensions in 4E Cognition Using Wide Computationalism

Recently, some authors have begun to raise questions about thepotential unity of 4E (enactive, embedded, embodied, extended)cognition as a distinct research programme within cognitivescience. Two tensions, in particular, have been raised: (i) thatthe body-centric claims embodied cognition militate against thedistributed tendencies of extended cognition and (ii) that thebody/environment distinction emphasized by enactivism standsin tension with the world-spanning claims of extendedcognition. The goal of this paper is to resolve tensions (i) and(ii). The proposal is that a form of ‘wide computationalism’ canbe used to reconcile the two tensions and, in so doing, articulatea common theoretical core for 4E cognition.

Processing Spatial Relations: A Meta-Analysis

The ability to reason about relations is relevant for many spa-tial cognitive processes. This can involve: (i) to represent spa-tial information mentally, (ii) to manipulate the spatial repre-sentation, and (iii) to infer new spatial information. Severalcognitive theories make assumptions and predictions about theunderlying processes. A detailed and systematic overview andanalysis of ireliable effects across studies is missing. This ar-ticle presents a meta-analysis of 35 studies about spatial rela-tional reasoning. Studies were classified according to differentfactors including the ambiguity of the spatial description, i.e.,if it the description allows for more than one representation, thepresentation of information, i.e., if the information has beenpresented auditorily or in a written form, and the task, i.e., ifa conclusion or model of the premises needs to be generatedor verified. Implications of the findings for the mental modeltheory and working memory are discussed.

Decoding Virtual Agent’s Emotion and Strategy from Brain Patterns

Recent advances in technology have paved the way for human-agent interactions to become ubiquitous in our daily lives, anddecades worth of research on virtual agents have enhancedthese interactions. However, for the most part, the effect of dif-ferent types of agents on the human brain is unknown, and theneuroscience of human-agent interactions is rarely studied. Inthis study, we examine the underlying neural systems involvedin processing and responding to different types of negotiatingagents. More specifically, we show that different brain patternsare observed for various types of virtual agents; consequently,we can decode the strategy and emotional display of the agentbased on the counterpart’s brain activity. Using fMRI data, weanalyzed participants’ brain activity during negotiations withagents who show three different emotional expressions and usetwo different types of negotiation strategies. We demonstratethat, using Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis, we can reliably de-code agents’ emotional expressions based on the activity in theleft dorsal anterior insula, and also agents’ strategies based onthe activity in the frontal pole.

Decoding Partner Type in Human-Agent Negotiation using functional MRI

People interact differently with humans than they do with com-puters, but there is minimal research on what brings aboutthese differences. Using agents labeled as either “another par-ticipant” or a “computer program”, we investigated the differ-ences in people’s behavior and brain activity during the courseof a negotiation paradigm. Our results indicate that people per-ceive human-labeled agents more human-like than computer-labeled agents, and the level of concession in the negotiationsis dependent on agent type. We have also found that these dif-ferences can be captured in brain activation by showing thatparts of the Theory of Mind neural correlates are activated inhuman-labeled agent conditions, but not in computer-labeledagent conditions. We further demonstrate that brain activitycan predict whether the negotiation agent was introduced asa competing human player or a computer program. Overall,our study suggests that labeling an interaction partner as ei-ther another human or a computer program leads to significantimpacts on one’s decision making.

What Do We Learn from Dyslexia and Second Language Learners on theDifference Between Long-term Frequency and Short-term Sequence RepetitionEffects?

Dyslexia is a common learning disability, but its core deficit is still under debate. The anchoring deficit hypothesissuggests that dyslexics’ benefit from experimental stimuli statistics is impaired (e.g. Ahissar, 2007). In this study we askedwhether dyslexia is also associated with reduced sensitivity to long-term statistics. Spans for lists of syllables were measured,and indeed, dyslexics benefited less than controls from syllabic frequency. However, dyslexics’ benefit from sequence repetitionwas similar to controls’. In order to dissociate the impact of item familiarity from exposure unrelated factors, native Englishspeakers performed the experiment. They were expected to benefit from repetition, but not from syllabic frequency (in Hebrew).Indeed, that was the case. These data suggest that benefits from long-term distributional statistics are impaired in dyslexia,whereas on-line benefits from sequence repetition are adequate. Moreover, our results suggest different underlying mechanismsfor long-term distribution learning and short-term sequence learning.

“Oops, I did it again.” The impact of frequent behaviour on causal judgement

Current causal theories aim to incorporate the effect of statistical and prescriptive norms on causal judgements, stating that norm-violating actions are judged as more causal than norm-conforming ones. In this paper, we present two experiments that undermine this claim, showing that people attribute increased causality to agents who conform to the norm of frequent behaviour. Furthermore, we find that the time point at which a moral norm is introduced does not make a difference to causal attributions, but that the frequency of a norm violation further accentuates its causal rating. Because these findings present a challenge to current norm theories of causation, we argue for an extended counterfactual model of causal attribution.

Variability in advice taking in decision making

We investigated how people would change and vary inaccepting advice when the effectiveness of advice was unclear.In each trial, participants estimated a monthly rent of anapartment room based on the attribute list. Then, anotherestimate by a real-estate agent was given as advice.Participants made a final estimation, either by taking theadvice fully, partially, or rejecting it totally. They repeated 48estimations without feedback. The weight of advice index,representing how much each participant weighed a givenadvice, gradually decreased as the number of trials increased.Interestingly, the gradual reduction of acceptance was notobserved in participants with high empathy and lowdepressive scores; they kept accepting advice even when theeffectiveness of advice was unclear. These results suggest thatthe willingness of accepting and using advice depends onhistory of advice taking, the individual traits, and mood.

Inner speech in post-stroke motor aphasia

The goal of the present study was to determine whether chronic post-stroke patients with motor aphasia have impaired inner speech abilities and whether they use inner speech in everyday life. To answer these questions, we recruited eight chronic post-stroke aphasic patients and 13 cognitively healthy adults, who underwent testing on a range of evaluative tests and four experiments specifically designed for the purposes of this study. The experimental results suggest that post-stroke patients with motor aphasia have impaired inner speech. However, patients’ subjective reports indicate that they use various types of inner speech, despite the observed deficit. Taken together, our data suggest that impairment of certain aspects of inner speech may still allow a degree of use of other aspects of inner speech, emphasizing a need to extend research on inner speech in aphasia to the variety of its forms.

The Impact of Population Structure on Models of Language Change

The dynamics of language evolution and learning in individuals have been extensively studied. Our knowledge ofthe transmission process in particular has been advanced by the iterated learning model. Additionally, work has been done inthe area of population structure and social networks. However, less has been described about the interaction between individual-level transmission and network structures. We present a general framework for representing transmission and learning algo-rithms within social networks. We demonstrate that population structure interacts with the transmission process to influencethe dynamics of change. Taking network effects into account, studies on language evolution will capture a fuller picture of thephenomenon.

Eye movements during reference production:Testing the effects of perceptual grouping on referential overspecification

When referring to a target object in a visual scene, speakers areassumed to consider certain distractor objects that are visible tobe more relevant than others. However, previous research thathas tested this assumption has mainly applied offline measuresof visual attention, such as the occurrence of overspecificationin speakers’ target descriptions. Therefore, in the current study,we take both online (eye-tracking) and offline (overspecifica-tion) measures of attention, to study how perceptual groupingaffects scene perception, and reference production. We manip-ulated three grouping principles: region of space, type similar-ity, and color similarity. For all three factors, we found effects,either on eye movements (region of space), overspecification(color similarity), or both (type similarity). The results for typesimilarity provide direct evidence for the close link betweenscene perception and reference production.

Modelling conceptual change as foraging for explanations on an epistemiclandscape

We discuss here conceptual change and the formation of robustlearning outcomes from the viewpoint of complex dynamicsystems, where students’ conceptions are seen as context de-pendent and multifaceted structures which depend on the con-text of their application. According to this view the conceptualpatterns (i.e. intuitive conceptions) may be robust in a cer-tain situation but are not formed, at last not as robust ones, inanother situation. The stability is then thought to arise dynami-cally in a variety of ways and not so much mirror rigid ontolog-ical categories or static intuitive conceptions. We use compu-tational modelling in understanding the generic dynamic andemergent features of that phenomenon. The model shows howcontext dependence, described here through structure of epis-temic landscape, leads to formation of context dependent ro-bust states. The sharply defined nature of these states makeslearning to appear as a progression of switches from state toanother, given appearance of conceptual change as switch fromone robust state to another.

Action Understanding in High-Functioning Autism: The Faux Pas Task Revisited

Individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are said to have deficits in “theory of mind.” The present paper explores two main accounts of the mechanisms underlying these deficits. On one account, high-functioning adults with ASD struggle to infer others’ mental states. On another account, they lack an ability to integrate those mental states into a coherent understanding of action. We tested these two accounts by making several modifications to the Faux Pas task—a commonly used advanced theory of mind task— including the presentation of explicit mental state information. Surprisingly, in contrast to previous work, individuals on the autism spectrum exhibited both intact integration and intact inference.

Dependent Choices in Employee Selection: Modeling Choice Compensation andConsistency

Past choices can influence subsequent choices in employee se-lection. Previous approaches rather described similar sequen-tial effects with feedback learning or the misperception of ran-domness. However, in the selection of job candidates also theaccumulation of the moral impact of previous choices mightinfluence subsequent choices. We investigated that questionby making two major contributions to the literature. First, wedeveloped an experimental paradigm for measuring sequentialchoices in employee selection and second, we implementeda widely applicable computational model, the Dependent Se-quential Sampling Model, for explaining sequential effects inchoices. By using this methodological approach, we uncov-ered sequential effects in employee selection. Participants(N=600) were especially motivated to compensate for morallydubious choices, with some participants showing consistentchoice behavior if their previous choices had been morally vir-tuous. These results support the assumption of asymmetriccompensation of morally dubious choices, sometimes referredto as the moral cleansing hypothesis.

The Influence of Prosody and Case Marking on Thematic Role Assignment inAmbiguous Action Scenes: Adults versus Children

In two visual word eye tracking studies, we investigated theinfluence of prosody and case marking on children’s andadults’ thematic role assignment. We assigned an SVO/OVS-biasing (vs. neutral) prosodic contour tounambiguously case marked German subject-verb-object(SVO) and object-verb-subject (OVS) sentencesrespectively. Scenes depicted ambiguous action events(e.g., donkey-paints->elephant-paints->cheetah) but casemarking and prosody could, in principle, disambiguate. Inadults, case marking but not prosody rapidly guidedthematic role assignment. Children did not rely on casemarking but exploited the biasing prosody to enhance theiragent-first interpretation of the sentences. These resultssuggest that in scenes depicting fully ambiguous rolerelations, children’s understanding of case marking at theage of five is not yet robust enough to enable thematic roleassignment. Prosody did not overwrite the SVO preference,it rather enhanced it.

Enhancing metacognitive reinforcement learningusing reward structures and feedback

How do we learn to think better, and what can we do to pro-mote such metacognitive learning? Here, we propose that cog-nitive growth proceeds through metacognitive reinforcementlearning. We apply this theory to model how people learn howfar to plan ahead and test its predictions about the speed ofmetacognitive learning in two experiments. In the first experi-ment, we find that our model can discern a reward structure thatpromotes metacognitive reinforcement learning from one thathinders it. In the second experiment, we show that our modelcan be used to design a feedback mechanism that enhancesmetacognitive reinforcement learning in an environment thathinders learning. Our results suggest that modeling metacog-nitive learning is a promising step towards promoting cognitivegrowth.

Thinking and Guessing: Bayesian and Empirical Models of How Humans Search

Searching natural environments, as for example, when foraging or looking for a landmark, combines reasoningunder uncertainty, planning and visual search. Existing paradigms for studying search in humans focus on step-by-step infor-mation sampling, without examining advance planning. We propose and evaluate a Bayesian model of how people search in anaturalistic maze-solving task. The model encodes environment exploration as a sequential process of acquiring informationmodelled by a Partially Observable Markov Decision Process (POMDP), which maximises the information gained. We showthat the search policy averaged across participants is optimal. Individual solutions, however, are highly variable and can beexplained by two heuristics: thinking and guessing. Self-report and inference, a Gaussian Mixture Model over inverse POMDP,consistently assign most subjects to one style or the other. By analysing individual participants’ decision times we show thatindividuals solve partial POMDPs and plan their search a limited number of steps in advance.

The Puzzle of Conditionals with True Clauses: Against the Gricean Account

ndicative conditionals, that is sentences of the form “If p, thenq,” belong to the most puzzling phenomena of language. Onthe majority of accounts of indicative conditionals, the truth ofp and q suffices for “If p, then q” to be true or highly accept-able. Yet, many conditionals with true clauses, even if there isa meaningful connection between them, sound odd. The mostcommon reaction to this phenomenon is to attribute the oddityof conditionals with true clauses to natural language pragmat-ics. We present an experimental study investigating how thepresence or absence of a connection between the clauses af-fects the assertability of conditionals and conjunction express-ing generic and specific kind of content. The results refute thestandard pragmatic explanation.

Cascading effect of context and competition on novel word learning

Learning, especially in the case of language acquisition, is notan isolated process; there is ever-present competition betweenwords and objects in the world. Such competition is known toplay a critical role in learning. Namely, the amount andvariability of competing items during word learning havebeen shown to change learning trajectories in young childrenlearning new words. However, very little work has examinedthe interaction of competition amount, competitionvariability, and task demands in adults. The current studyassesses adults’ ability to map new word-referent pairs invarying amounts of competition and competitor variability. Inaddition, the effect of mapping context on retention wasassessed. Results suggest that retention is weak in some casesand importantly, there are cascading effects of competitorvariability in mapping on later retention of new words.Results are discussed in light of associative learningmechanisms and the implications of competition for learning.

Geometric Concept Acquisition in a Dueling Deep Q-Network

Explaining how intelligent systems come to embody knowl-edge of deductive concepts through inductive learning is afundamental challenge of both cognitive science and artificialintelligence. We address this challenge by exploring how adeep reinforcement learning agent, occupying a setting simi-lar to those encountered by early-stage mathematical conceptlearners, comes to represent ideas such as rotation and trans-lation. We first train a Dueling Deep Q-Network on a shapesorting task requiring implicit knowledge of geometric proper-ties, then we query this network with classification and prefer-ence selection tasks. We demonstrate that scalar reinforcementprovides sufficient signal to learn representations of shape cat-egories. After training, the model shows a preference for moresymmetric shapes, which it can sort more quickly than lesssymmetric shapes, supporting the view symmetry preferencesmay be acquired from goal-directed experience.

Perceived Difficulty of Moral Dilemmas Depends on Their Causal Structure:A Formal Model and Preliminary Results

We propose causal agency models for representing and reason-ing about ethical dilemmas. We find that ethical dilemmas, al-though they appear similar on the surface, differ in their formalstructure. Based on their structural properties, as identified bythe causal agency models, we cluster a set of dilemmas in Type1 and Type 2 dilemmas. We observe that for Type 1 dilemmasbut not for Type 2 dilemmas a utilitarian action does not domi-nate the possibility of refraining from action thereby constitut-ing a conflict. Hence, we hypothesize, based on the model, thatType 1 dilemmas are perceived as more difficult than Type 2dilemmas by human reasoners. A behavioral study where par-ticipants rated the difficulty of dilemmas supports the models’predictions.

Communicative efficiency in language production and learning:Optional plural marking

Recent work suggests that language production exhibits a biastowards efficient information transmission. Speakers tendto provide more linguistic signal for meaning elements thatare difficult to recover while reducing contextually inferrable(more frequent, probable, or expected) elements. This trade-off has been hypothesized to shape grammatical systems overgenerations, contributing to cross-linguistic patterns. We putthis idea to an empirical test using miniature artificial languagelearning over variable input. Two experiments were conductedto demonstrate that the inferrability of plurality informationinversely predicts the likelihood of overt plural marking, aswould be expected if learners prefer communicatively efficientsystems. The results were obtained even with input frequencycounts of the plural marker counteracting the bias, and thusprovide strong support for a critical role of inferrability ofmeaning in language learning, production, as well as in typo-logically attested variations.

Behavioral Dynamics and Action Selection in a Joint Action Pick-and-Place Task

Many common tasks require or are made more efficient by coordinating with others. In this paper we investigate the coordination dynamics of a joint action pick-and-place task in order to identify the behavioral dynamics that underlie the emergence of human coordination. More precisely, we introduce a task dynamics approach for modeling multi-agent interaction in a continuous pick-and-place task where two agents must decide to work together or alone to move an object from one location to another. Our aims in the current paper are to identify and model (1) the relevant affordance dynamics that underlie the selection of the different action modes required by the task and (2) the trajectory dynamics of each actor’s hand movements when moving to grasp, relocate, or pass the object. We demonstrate that the emergence of successful coordination can be characterized in terms of behavioral dynamics models which may have applications for artificial agent design.

Analogies Emerge from Learning Dyamics in Neural Networks

When a neural network is trained on multiple analogous tasks,previous research has shown that it will often generate rep-resentations that reflect the analogy. This may explain thevalue of multi-task training, and also may underlie the powerof human analogical reasoning – awareness of analogies mayemerge naturally from gradient-based learning in neural net-works. We explore this issue by generalizing linear analysistechniques to explore two sets of analogous tasks, show thatanalogical structure is commonly extracted, and address somepotential implications.

A Hierarchical Bayesian Model of Individual Differencesin Memory for Emotional Expressions

When participants view and then reproduce simple objects thatvary along a continuous dimension such as length or shade, orwhen they view images of faces that vary in emotional expression,their estimates tend to be biased toward the average value of thepresented objects, a phenomenon that has been modeled as theresult of a Bayesian combination of prior category knowledge withan imprecise memory trace (Corbin, Crawford & Vavra, 2017;Huttenlocher, Hedges & Vevea, 2000). Whereas previous workdescribed a general cognitive strategy based on data aggregatedacross participants, here we examined individual differences instrategy. Thirty-six participants viewed and reproduced 496morphed face stimuli that ranged from angry to happy. We foundsubstantial variation in the bias patterns participants produced.Individuals’ estimates were well fit by a model that positedattraction toward three categories, one at the happy end of therange, one at the angry end, and one that captured the entire rangeof presented stimuli, and by allowing the weight given to eachcategory to vary by participant.

Simulating performance in unconscious plagiarism

Studies of unconscious plagiarism have reported that peoplemistakenly include a partner’s responses when trying to recalltheir own (recall-own task) and include own responses whentrying to recall their partner’s (recall-partner task). In a simula-tion, we tested if participants’ memory performance at test, in-cluding source errors, can be explained by participants simplyguessing items that come easily to mind. We show that guess-ing alone cannot account for the pattern of data participantsshow at test. Modifying the simulation by including memoryfor self-generated items allows us to replicate the pattern of re-sponding in the recall-own but not the recall-partner task, evenwhen we assume that participants in the recall-partner taskstrategically withhold more fluent items from report. This sug-gests that judgements of items’ memory strength alone cannotexplain performance in the unconscious plagiarism paradigm.

When do learned transformations influence similarity and categorization?

The transformational theory of similarity suggests that whenjudging similarity, people are sensitive to the number of trans-formation operations needed to make two compared repre-sentations match. Although this theory has been influential,little is known about how transformations are learned andto what extent learned transformations affect similarity judg-ments. This paper presents two experiments addressing thesequestions, in which people learned categories defined by atransformation. In Experiment 1, when the transformationswere directly visible, people had no trouble learning and ap-plied their knowledge to similarity and categorization judg-ments involving previously unseen items. In Experiment 2,the task required transformations to be inferred rather than ob-served. People were still able to learn the categories, but inthis more difficult case ratings were less strongly affected bytraining. Overall, this work suggests that newly learned trans-formations can impact similarity judgments but the salience ofthe transformation has a large impact on transfer.

Preschoolers and Infants Calibrate Persistence from Adult Models

Perseverance, above and beyond IQ, predicts academic outcomes in school age children, however, little is knownabout what factors affect persistence in early childhood. Here, we propose a formal Bayesian model of how children mightlearn how to calibrate effort from observing adult models and then explore this idea behaviorally across two experiments inchildren and infants. Results from Experiment 1 show that preschoolers persist more after watching an adult persist, but onlyif the adult is successful at reaching their goal. Experiment 2 and a pre-registered replication extend these findings, showingthat even infants use adult models to modulate their persistence, and can generalize this inference to novel situations. Theseresults suggest that both preschoolers and infants are sensitive to adult persistence and use it to calibrate their own effort infar-reaching ways.

You can take a noun out of syntax...: Syntactic similarity effects in lexical priming

Usage-based theories of syntax predict that words and syntactic constructions are probabilistically interconnected. If this is true, then words that occur in similar distributions of syntactic constructions should prime each other. These effects should be fine-grained; even small differences between the syntactic distributions of pairs of words of the same grammatical category should cause variation in priming. Prior research from production suggests that this prediction should hold even in tasks without any syntactic requirement. In this study, we introduce a measure of the similarity between the syntactic contexts in which two nouns occur. We show that this similarity measure significantly predicts visual lexical decision priming magnitudes between pairs of nouns. This finding is consistent with the predictions of usage-based theories where fine-grained similarity of syntactic usages between prime-target pairs affects decision latencies, over and above any effects attributable to semantic similarity.

How the Mind Exploits Risk-Reward Structures in Decisions under Risk

In many natural domains, risks and rewards are inversely re-lated (Pleskac & Hertwig, 2014). We sought to understandhow people might use this relationship in choosing amongrisky gambles. To do so we, manipulated risk-reward struc-tures of monetary gambles to be either negatively or positivelycorrelated, or uncorrelated. After substantial exposure to theseenvironments, participants completed a speeded choice taskamong non-dominated gambles. Eye-tracking data from thistask suggests that participants often shifted their attention tomainly one attribute in the correlated conditions, in which therisk-reward relationship was present. This was an adaptivestrategy that resulted in a similar proportion of expected-valuemaximizing choices, compared to a more compensatory pro-cessing strategy.

Semantic vector evaluation and human performance on a newvocabulary MCQ test

Vectors derived from patterns of co-occurrence of words inlarge bodies of text have often been used as representations ofsome aspects of the meanings of different words. Generally,the distance between such vectors is used as a measure of thesemantic similarity between the word meanings theyrepresent. One important way of evaluating the performanceof these vectors has been to use them to answer vocabularymultiple choice questions (MCQs) where the participant isasked to judge which of several choice words is closest inmeaning to a stem word. The existing vocabulary MCQ testsused in this way have been very useful but there are somepractical problems in their use as general evaluationmeasures. Here, we discuss why such tests remain usefulevaluation measures, introduce a new vocabulary test,evaluate several current sets of semantic vectors using thenew test and compare their performance to human data.

“I’m Better than You at Labeling!”: Preschoolers Use Past Reliability whenAccepting Unexpected Labels

How do young children decide to trust testimony thatcontradicts their initial beliefs? The current study examinedwhether children rely on cues to informant credibility (i.e.,history of accuracy) to determine if they would endorse anunexpected label from an informant. Three- and 4-year-olds(N = 60) saw a picture of a hybrid artifact that consisted offeatures of two typical familiar artifacts. Children made initialjudgments about the name of the hybrid object andsubsequently received a different name offered by aninformant who had earlier either accurately or inaccuratelynamed familiar objects. Children were more willing to revisetheir own judgment and accept the unexpected label if it wasfrom a previously accurate informant than if it was fromsomeone who had made obvious naming errors. This suggeststhat preschool-aged children selectively revise their ownknowledge; they are more trusting toward sources provenaccurate than inaccurate.

How does Music Reading Expertise Modulate Visual Processing of English Words?An ERP study

Music notation and English word reading have similar visualprocessing requirements. It remains unclear how the twoskills influence each other. Here we investigated the modula-tion of music reading expertise on visual processing of Eng-lish words through an ERP study. Participants matched Eng-lish real, pseudo, and non-words preceded by musical seg-ments or novel symbol strings in a sequential matching task.Musicians showed smaller N170 amplitude in response toEnglish non-words preceded by musical segments than bynovel symbol strings in the right hemisphere. This effect wasnot observed in real or pseudo-words, or in any of non-musicians’ responses. Similar to English non-words, musicalsegments do not have morphological rules or semantic infor-mation, giving rise to this modulation effect. This findingsuggested a shared visual processing mechanism in the righthemisphere between music notation and English non-wordreading, which may be related to serial symbol processing assuggested by previous studies.

Metaphor Congruent Image Schemas Shape Evaluative Judgment: ACross-Linguistic Study of Metaphors for Economic Change

Metaphor pervades discussions of important socio-politicaltopics. Recent research indicates that metaphorical languagecan influence how people reason about such topics, potentiallyaffecting real-world decision-making. In this study, we reporton research into the effects of metaphor on evaluative judg-ment, another aspect of decision-making that has been lesswell studied than reasoning. We use a cross-linguistic differ-ence in the metaphors used by English and Spanish speakersto discuss economic change to investigate how metaphoricallanguage affects evaluative judgment. We show that the imageschematic information inherent in the semantics of the differ-ent metaphors performs a central role in shaping this process.

Keystroke Dynamics Predict Essay Quality

Language entails many nested time scales, ranging from therelatively slow scale of cultural evolution to the rapid scale ofindividual cognition. The nested, multiscale nature oflanguage implies that even simple acts of text production,such as typing a sentence, entail complex interactionsinvolving multiple concurrent processes. As such, textproduction may have much in common with other cognitivephenomena thought to emerge from multiplicativeinteractions across temporal scales, namely those that exhibitfractal properties. We investigated the relationship betweenfractal scaling and the quality of produced text. Participants(N=131) wrote essays while their keystrokes were recorded.Fractal analyses were then performed on time series ofinterkeystroke intervals (IKIs). Results showed that fractalproperties characterizing IKIs positively predicted expertratings of essay quality, even after accounting for essaylength. The results support our hypotheses concerningmultiscale coordination and text production.

Visuomotor Adaptation and Sensory Recalibration inReversed Hand Movement Task

Visuomotor adaptation plays an important role in motor plan-ning and execution. However, it remains unclear how senso-rimotor transformations are recalibrated when visual and pro-prioceptive feedback are decoupled. To address this question,the present study asked participants to reach toward targets ina virtual reality (VR) environment. They were given visualfeedback of their arm movements in VR that was either con-sistent (normal motion) with the virtual world or reflected (re-versed motion) with respect to the left-right and vertical axes.Participants completed two normal motion experimental ses-sions, with a reversed motion session in between. While re-action time in the reversed motion session was longer than inthe normal motion session, participants showed the learningimprovement by completing trials in the second normal mo-tion session faster than in the first. The reduction in reactiontime was found to correlate with greater use of linear reach-ing trajectory strategies (measured using dynamic time warp-ing) in the reversed and second normal motion sessions. Thisresult appears consistent with linear motor movement plan-ning guided by increased attention to visual feedback. Suchstrategical bias persisted into the second normal motion ses-sion. Participants in the reversed session were grouped intotwo clusters depending on their preference for proximal/distaland awkward/smooth motor movements. We found that partic-ipants who preferred distal-smooth movements produced morelinear trajectories than those who preferred proximal-awkwardmovements.

The development of turn-taking: Pre-schoolers may predict what you will say, butthey don’t use those predictions to plan a reply.

Whereas adults exchange conversational turns very rapidly, children often respond after long gaps. However, it hasbeen proposed that the infrastructure necessary to take turns develops in infancy. Why are children slow to respond to turns?Adults’ turn-taking skills, it has been argued, rely on an ability to both predict when the current turn will end and prepare aresponse as soon as possible. In two experiments, we ask how these two abilities (prediction and early preparation) develop.Adults and 3-to-5-year-olds answered yes/no questions while playing an iPad-based maze game. Distributional analysis ofanswer latencies suggest that (i) neither children nor adults rely on fine-grained predictions of turn duration and (ii) bothchildren and adults use predictions about turn content to prepare their answer early. In sum, by the age of three, childrenalready have the cognitive architecture necessary to take turns successfully. ,

Mouse Tracking Shows Attraction to Alternative TargetsWhile Grounding Spatial Relations

Evidence that higher cognitive processes are coupled in agraded and time-continuous way to sensory-motor processescomes, in part, from mouse-tracking studies. In these, curvedmouse trajectories toward one of two fixed response locationsreveal the evolution of certainty about a cognitive task that par-ticipants solve. We present a paradigm in which selection ofthe response location is itself the cognitive task. From amongitems in a visual scene, participants select a target that is de-scribed by a spatial relation (e.g.,“the red to the left of thegreen”), where one target item (here, “red”) matches the de-scription better than alternative same-colored targets. In themouse trajectories, we find clear evidence for attraction tothe alternative targets, attraction to the reference item (here“green”), and an early biasing influence of the spatial term.

Prediction and uncertainty in an artificial language

Probabilistic prediction is a central process in language com-prehension. Properties of probability distributions over predic-tions are often difficult to study in natural language. To obtainprecise control over these distributions, we created artificiallanguages consisting of sequences of shapes. The languageswere constructed to vary the uncertainty of the probability dis-tribution over predictions as well as the probability of the pre-dicted item. Participants were exposed to the languages in aself-paced presentation paradigm, which provides a measureof processing difficulty at each element of a sequence. Therewas a robust pattern of graded predictability: shapes were pro-cessed faster the more predictable they were, as in natural lan-guage. Processing times were also affected by the uncertainty(entropy) over predictions at the point at which those predic-tions were made; this effect was less consistent, however.

Explain, Explore, Exploit: Effects of Explanation on Information Search

How does actively seeking explanations for one’sobservations affect information search over the course oflearning? Generating explanations could plausibly leadlearners to take advantage of the information they havealready obtained, resulting in less exploration. Alternatively,explaining could lead learners to explore more, especiallyafter encountering evidence that suggests their current beliefsare incorrect. In two experiments using a modified observe orbet task, we investigate these possibilities and find support forthe latter: participants who are prompted to explain theirobservations in the course of learning tend to explore more,especially after encountering evidence that challenges acurrent belief.

Can Illness be Bright? Metaphor Comprehension Depends on Linguistic and Embodied Factors

Conceptual representations in language processing employ both linguistic distributional and embodied information. Here, we aim to demonstrate the roles of these two components in metaphor processing. The linguistic component is captured by linguistic distributional frequency (LDF), that is, how often the constituent words appear together in context. The embodied component, on the other hand, refers to how easy it is to generate an embodied simulation, operationalised by a previous norming study. In the current study, we looked at the interplay of these components in metaphor processing, and investigated their roles at different depths of processing in two experiments. Thus, we required participants to engage in shallow processing (Experiment 1: Sensibility Judgement), or deep processing (Experiment 2: Interpretation Generation). Results showed that the increase of both variables made it more likely to accept a metaphor. However, whereas ease of simulation (EoS) contributed to the speed of processing at both levels of depth, LDF only affected the speed in shallow processing. Specifically, LDF acted as a heuristic, both to speed up responses to accept metaphors as sensible when the frequency is high, and to flag up potentially unsuccessful processing when it is low. Overall, these results support views of language processing that emphasise the importance of both linguistic and embodied components according to task goals.

Want to prime exercise? Calorie labels work better than activity ones!

‘Activity-equivalent’ food labels are believed to encourageconsumers to partake in exercise. This may occur by semanticpriming, where featuring images of physical activity increasesthe mental accessibility of the concept of exercise, making itmore ‘fluent’ and therefore more influential on people’sbehaviour. We tested how the format of labels (image vs.text) and representation of energy (‘activity’ vs. ‘calorie)affected mental accessibility of exercise in a word-fragmentcompletion task and participants’ behavioural intentions forexercise (N = 142). Participants exposed to calorie labelsproduced more exercise-related words and viewed animagined exercise scenario as shorter and more enjoyable.Images led to higher intentions to exercise than text whenthey described activities but they led to lower intentions toexercise than text when they described calories. Findingssuggest that activity labels do not trigger more activity relatedthoughts, but could increase exercise intentions only ifpresented in pictorial format.

Failure to replicate talker-specific syntactic adaptation

Sentence understanding is affected by recent experience. Animportant open question is whether this reflects adaptationto the statistics of the input. Support for this hypothesiscomes from the recent finding that listeners can simultaneouslylearn and maintain the syntactic statistics of multiple talkers(Kamide, 2012). We attempt—and fail—to replicate this find-ing. This calls into questions whether recency effects in sen-tence processing originate in the same adaptive mechanismsoperating during speech perception (for which talker-specificadaptation is well-established).

Instrumental Representations of Sensorimotor Control: Representations at Intermediate Level

In cognitive science, computation is largely accompanied with representational theory of mind. Yet, it remains unclear whether this companionship also appears in the realm of sensorimotor control. Grush’s (2004) and Pezzulo’s (2008, 2011) account of anticipatory representations provide a limited answer, as they are only suitable for forward models, but not the entire sensorimotor control. Rescorla’s (2016) representational explanation for sensorimotor psychology addresses several intentional states considered in Bayesian inference and optimal modeling. However, the above accounts does not explain how motor commands are produced and chosen in the course of sensorimotor control for maintaining accuracy of goal-achievement. The present paper aims to explain it with a representational account by considering instrumental representations of sensorimotor control, which appear at the intermediate level and are exemplified by motor commands and costs. Such representations do not presume decouplability, as they need to run on-line in the maintenance of accuracy.

Disentangling perceptual and linguistic factors in parsing

We offer a re-evaluation of the tone-monitoring technique inthe study of parsing. Experiment 1 shows that reaction times(RTs) to tones are affected by two factors: a) processing load,resulting in a tendency for RTs to decrease across a sentence,and b) a perceptual effect which adds to this tendency andmoreover plays a role in neutralising differences between sen-tence types. Experiment 2 successfully discriminates thesetwo factors by registering event-related brain potentials dur-ing a monitoring task, establishing that the amplitudes of theN1 and P3 components —the first associated with temporaluncertainty, the second with processing load— correlate withRTs. Experiment 3 then behaviourally segregates the two fac-tors by placing the last tone at the end of sentences, activating awrap-up operation and thereby both disrupting the decreasingtendency and highlighting structural factors.

Grasping Multisensory Integration: Proprioceptive Capture after Virtual ObjectInteractions

According to most recent theories of multisensory integration,weighting of different modalities depends on the reliability ofthe involved sensory estimates. Top-down modulations havebeen studied to a lesser degree. Furthermore, it is still debatedwhether working memory maintains multisensory informationin a distributed modal fashion, or in terms of an integrated rep-resentation. To investigate whether multisensory integrationis modulated by task relevance and to probe the nature of theworking memory encodings, we combined an object interac-tion task with a size estimation task in an immersive virtualreality. During the object interaction, we induced multisen-sory conflict between seen and felt grip aperture. Both, visualand proprioceptive size estimation showed a clear modulationby the experimental manipulation. Thus, the results suggestthat multisensory integration is not only driven by reliability,but is also biased by task demands. Furthermore, multisensoryinformation seems to be represented by means of interactivemodal representations.

Goal-Directed Deployment of Attention in a Computational Model:A Study in Multiple-Object Tracking

We present a computational model exploring goal-directeddeployment of attention during object tracking. Once selected,objects are tracked in parallel, but serial attention can bedirected to an object that is visually crowded and in danger ofbeing lost. An attended object’s future position can beextrapolated from its past motion trajectory, allowing theobject to be tracked even when it is briefly occluded. Usingthe model, we demonstrate that the difficulty of trackingthrough occlusions increases with the number of objectsbecause they compete for serial attention.

‘It’s More Fun With My Phone’:A Replication Study of Cell Phone Presence and Task Performance

From distracted driving, to work focus on a computer,increasing amounts of research is investigating how digitaltechnology influences users’ attention. A couple of widelycited studies have found that the mere presence of cell phonesinterferes with social interactions and cognitive performance,even when not actively in use. These studies have importantimplications but they have not yet been replicated, and alsosuffer from methodological shortcomings and lack ofestablished theoretical frameworks to explain the observedeffects. We improved the methodology used in a previousstudy of phone presence and task performance (Thornton,Faires, Robbins, & Rollins, 2014), while testing an‘opportunity cost’ model of mental effort and attention(Kurzban, Duckworth, Kable, & Myers, 2013). We wereunable to replicate Thornton et al.’s finding that presence ofcell phones reduces performance in a specific cognitive task(additive digit cancellation). Moreover, contrary to ourexpectations, we found that participants who used theirphones more, and who were more attached to them, found thetasks more fun/exciting and effortless, if they completed themwith their phone present.

The strategic advantages of micro-targeted campaigning: A proof of principleBayesian Agent-Based Model

Predicting the effect of persuasion campaigns is difficult, asbelief changes may cascade through a network. In recentyears, political campaigns have adopted micro-targetingstrategies that segment voters into fine-grained clusters formore specific targetting. At present, there is little evidencethat explores the efficiency of this method. Through anAgent-Based Model, the current paper provides a novelmethod for exploring predicted effects of strategic persuasioncampaigns.The voters in the model are rational and revise their beliefsin the propositions expounded by the politicians inaccordance with Bayesian belief updating through a sourcecredibility model.The model provides a proof of concept and shows strategicadvantages of micro-targeted campaigning. Despite havingonly little voter data allowing crude segmentation, the micro-targeted campaign consistently beat stochastic campaignswith the same reach. However, given substantially greaterreach, a positively perceived stochastic candidate can nullifyor beat a strategic persuasion campaigns.

Growing a Bayesian Conspiracy Theorist: An Agent-Based Model

Conspiracy theories cover topics from politicians to worldevents. Frequently, proponents of conspiracies hold thesebeliefs strongly despite available evidence that may challengeor disprove them. Therefore, conspiratorial reasoning hasoften been described as illegitimate or flawed. In the paper,we explore the possibility of growing a rational (Bayesian)conspiracy theorist through an Agent-Based Model. The agenthas reasonable constraints on access to the total informationas well its access to the global population.The model shows that network structures are central tomaintain objectively mistaken beliefs. Increasing the size ofthe available network yielded increased confidence inmistaken beliefs and subsequent network pruning, allowingfor belief purism. Rather than ameliorating and correctingmistaken beliefs (where agents move toward the correctmean), large networks appear to maintain and strengthenthem. As such, large networks may increase the potential forbelief polarization, extreme beliefs, and conspiratorialthinking – even amongst Bayesian agents.

The dilution effect: Conversational basis and witness reliability

The dilution effect occurs when the introduction of non-diagnostic information lessens the impact on reasoning ofdiagnostic information despite having no relevance to thehypothesis in question. While the effect has been reproducedin several studies, the psychological basis of the effectremains unclear. Some believe it to be conversational whileothers believe it to be cognitive and social.The paper tests the conversational basis of the effect byminimising pragmatic, conversational influence. To this end,it makes use of a legal setting with witness testimonies. Thestudies replicate the dilution effect, which suggests that thebasis of the results in the original studies is notconversational. However, the credibility of the sourcestrongly influences whether or not the effect occurs. Ifreliable sources provide the non-diagnostic information, theeffect lessens. Conversely, if unreliable sources provide thenon-diagnostic information, we observe a stronger dilutioneffect.

Influence of using 3D images and 3D-printed objects on spatial reasoning ofexperts and novices

This study focuses on the infuence of a three-dimensional (3D)graphic image and a 3D-printed object on the spatial reasoningof experts and novices in the medical field. The spatial rea-soning task of this study required doctors specializing in di-gestive surgery to infer cross sections of a liver with a 3D im-age and a 3D-printed object in a situation where liver resectionsurgery was simulated. The task performance was comparedwith that of university students who conducted the same task inMaehigashi et al. (2016). The results of the analysis indicatedthat the doctors showed the same task performance when usingthe 3D image and the 3D-printed object. However, the univer-sity students learned faster and inferred the inside of a liverstructure more accurately with the 3D-printed object than withthe 3D image, and they performed equally to the professionaldoctors. Our results are then discussed in relation to previousstudies.

Chunking Ability Shapes Sentence Processing at Multiple Levels of Abstraction

Several recent empirical findings have reinforced the notion that a basic learning and memory skill—chunking—plays a fundamental role in language processing. Here, we provide evidence that chunking shapes sentence processing at multiple levels of linguistic abstraction, consistent with a recent theoretical proposal by Christiansen and Chater (2016). Individual differences in chunking ability at two different levels is shown to predict on-line sentence processing in separate ways: i) phonological chunking ability, as assessed by a variation on the non-word repetition task, predicts processing of complex sentences featuring phonological overlap; ii) multiword chunking ability, as assessed by a variation on the serial recall task, is shown to predict reading times for sentences featuring long-distance number agreement with locally distracting number-marked nouns. Together, our findings suggest that individual differences in chunking ability shape language processing at multiple levels of abstraction, consistent with the notion of language acquisition as learning to process.

The Interaction of Worked-Examples/ Self-Explanation Prompts and Time onAlgebra Conceptual Knowledge

Success in Algebra I often predicts whether or not a studentwill pursue higher levels of mathematics and science.However, many students enter algebra holding persistentmisconceptions that are difficult to eliminate, thus, hinderingtheir ability to succeed in algebra. One way to address thesemisconceptions is to implement worked-examples and self-explanation prompts, which have been shown to improvestudents’ conceptual knowledge. However this effect seems tobe greater after a delay. The current study sought to exploresuch time-related effects on algebra conceptual knowledge. Ina year-long random-assignment study, students either studiedworked-examples and answered self-explanation prompts (n =132) or solved typical isomorphic problems (n = 140). Athree-way mixed ANCOVA (pre-algebra knowledge xcondition x time) found a significant condition by time effect.The growth of algebra conceptual knowledge was greater forstudents studying worked-examples than for those solvingtypical problems.

Semantic Ambiguity Effects: A Matter of Time?

Are different amounts of semantic processing associated with different semantic ambiguity effects? Could this explain some discrepant ambiguity effects observed between and across tasks? Armstrong and Plaut (2016) provided an initial set of neural network simulations indicating this is indeed the case. However, their empirical findings using a lexical decision task were not clear-cut. Here, we use improved methods and five different experimental manipulations to slow responding- --and the presumed amount of semantic processing---to evaluate their account more rigorously. We also expanded the empirical horizon to another language: Spanish. The results are partially consistent with the predictions of the neural network and differ in several important ways from English data. Potential causes of these discrepancies are discussed in relation to theories of ambiguity resolution and cross-linguistic differences.

Inferring Intentional Agents From Violation of Randomness

Humans have a strong “cognitive compulsion” to infer in-tentional agents from violation of randomness and such anagency–nonrandomness link emerges early in development.In two studies, we directly quantified, formalized, and com-pared both ends of this link for the first time. In Experiment1, two groups of participants viewed the same 256 binary se-quences (e.g., AABAAABA) and classified each as generatedby agents/non-agents or by nonrandom/random processes. Wefound a strong correlation between two judgments: sequencesviewed as more agentive also tended to be judged as less ran-dom. In Experiment 2, another two groups were asked toproduce sequences that others might appreciate as agentive ornonrandom. Participant-generated sequences in the two con-ditions had a substantial overlap, indicating common guidingprinciples of agency and nonrandomness generation. Taken to-gether, the present studies provide evidence for a shared cog-nitive basis of agency detection and subjective randomness.

Does Presentation Format Modulate Adults’ Automatic Processing of Proportions?

Whereas much is known about how humans categorize andreason based on absolute quantities, research investigating theprocessing of relative quantities, such as proportions, iscomparatively limited. The current study used a Stroop-likeparadigm to examine adults’ automatic processing ofnonsymbolic proportions and how presentation formatsmodulate this processing. Participants were asked to compareindividual components across proportions in six differentpresentation formats. Congruity between component size andoverall proportion affected accuracy of comparison, such thatparticipants were less accurate when proportion (the irrelevantdimension) was incongruent with absolute quantity (therelevant) dimension. Moreover, the congruity effect wasmodulated by the presentation format. These findings serve asevidence that humans automatically access relative quantitywhen presented in nonsymbolic formats and provide evidencethat the strength of this processing is modulated by the formatof presentation.

Utilizing simple cues to informational dependency

Participants can adequately take into account several cues regarding the weight they should grant majority opinions,but that they do not consistently take into account cues regarding whether the members of the majority have formed theiropinions independently of each other. We suggest that these conflicting results can be explained by hypothesizing that somecues are evolutionarily valid (i.e. they were present and reliable during human evolution), and others not. Using this frameworkwe derive and test hypotheses about two facets of informational dependency. The first 3 experiments show that participantsadequately take into account cues to informational dependency when they are presented in a simple, evolutionarily valid way.Experiments 4 to 7 show that people consistently take into account shared motivation, but not shared cognitive traits, as a sourceof potential dependency, as predicted by the likely greater importance of differences in motivation during our evolutionaryhistory.

Children’s EEG Indices of Directed Attention during Somatosensory Anticipation: Relations with Executive Function

Children’s ability to direct attention to salient stimuli is a key aspect of cognitive functioning. Here we examined the magnitude and lateralization of EEG indices during somatosensory anticipation elicited by a left or right directional cue indicating the bodily location of an upcoming tactile stimulus. In 50 children aged 6-8 years, somatosensory anticipation was accompanied by anticipatory negativity and alpha mu rhythm desynchronization at contralateral central electrode sites (C3 and C4) overlying the hand area of the somatosensory cortex. Individual differences in these contralateral brain responses during somatosensory anticipation were associated with scores on a flanker task of executive function. The results suggest that processes involved in directing attention in the tactile modality may overlap with those involved in broader executive function abilities.

Modeling Unsupervised Event Segmentation:Learning Event Boundaries from Prediction Errors

Segmenting observations from an input stream is an impor-tant capability of human cognition. Evidence suggests that hu-mans refine this ability through experiences with the world.However, few models address the unsupervised developmentof event segmentation in artificial agents. This paper presentswork towards developing a computational model of how anintelligent agent can independently learn to recognize mean-ingful events in continuous observations. In this model, theagent’s segmentation mechanism starts from a simple stateand is refined. The agent’s interactions with the environ-ment are unsupervised and driven by its expectation failures.Reinforcement learning drives the mechanism that identifiesevent boundaries by reasoning over a gated-recurrent neuralnetwork’s expectation failures. The learning task is to reduceprediction error by identifying when one event transitions intoanother. Our experimental results support that reinforcementlearning can enable detecting event boundaries in continuousobservations based on a gated-recurrent neural network’s pre-diction error and that this is possible with a simple set of fea-tures.

Objections to Computationalism. A Short Survey

In this paper, I review the objections against the claim that brains are computers, or, to be precise, information- processing mechanisms. By showing that practically all the popular objections are based on uncharitable (or simply incorrect) interpretations of the claim, I argue that the claim is likely to be true, relevant to contemporary cognitive (neuro)science, and non-trivial.

The Role of Schema-Governed Relational Categories in Analogical Inference

The standard approach posits that analogical inferences aregenerated by copying unmapped base relations, substitutingbase entities by their corresponding target ones, and generatingslots for unmapped base entities. Contra this account, resultsfrom Experiment 1 revealed that analogical inferences seldominclude relations that resemble the base relation from whichthey were derived. Most of the inferences, however, could becategorized as exemplars of a schema-governed categorycapable of characterizing the base information to be projected.To gather further precision about the criteria that guide inferencegeneration, in Experiment 2 we showed that analogicalinferences tend to match the base information from which theyare derived in values of salient dimensions of the relationalcategory to which they belonged. Our results suggest that therelational constructs employed in modeling analogical inferenceshould move beyond one-term multiplace predicates so as toinclude more complex relational structures.

An Investigation of Factors that Influence Resource Allocation Decisions

We investigate how people allocate a limited set of resourcesbetween multiple risky prospects. We found that only a smallpercentage of decisions followed some form of naive diversifi-cation or mean-variance optimization. In general, people wereless mean-variance optimal than a naive 1/N heuristic. As-pects of choice sets, such as domain, skew, and second orderstochastic dominance, affected resource allocation decisions ina similar manner to their influence on single choice gambles.Individual traits traditionally linked to risk propensity seem tomanifest in terms of the degree to which people are inclinedto diversify. Lower risk aversion and higher risk seeking traitsare linked to increasing diversification. Risk congruency, thedegree to which peoples’ self-reported and elicited risk aver-sion matches, moderates how susceptible people are to costframing nudges. We find evidence for heterogeneous clusterswhere people either under-weight or over-weight segregatedcosts, leading to the same nudge producing opposite behav-ioral results within two risk incongruent groups.

Predicting Individual Differences in Working Memory Training Gain: A MachineLearning Approach

Working memory (WM) capacity is critically important for the success in school and complex cognitive activitiesacross the lifespan. Training WM skills has shown to lead to improvements in a variety of important cognitive tasks. One’sperformance on an adaptive and challenging longitudinal WM intervention may serve as an assay of cognitive plasticity. Withover 400 participants having completed a minimum of 15 sessions of WM training, we have a rich dataset that allows investigat-ing individual differences and other factors that might determine training outcome using a novel machine learning techniques.Preliminary results suggest that factors such as age, type of n-back, and baseline abilities significantly impact one’s ability toimprove in training. Other factors such as gender and whether or not training was supervised were not significant. Finally, ourmodel allows prediction of training gain with 78% accuracy.

A Longitudinal Study of Differences between Predicted, Actual, and Remembered Personal ChangeLongitudinal Study of Differences between Predicted, Actual, and Remembered Personal Change

We investigated people’s assessments of their own personal change over time, comparing predicted, actual, and recalled change in personality, values, and performance. On average, participants underestimated the absolute magnitude of their personal change in both prediction and recall. However, people specifically neglected negative future change, resulting in overly optimistic predictions of improvement. In contrast, recall of positive and negative change was relatively more balanced, such that assessments of past improvement were better calibrated on average. Our findings provide insight into how people think about their own identity over time and address disparate theories in the literature regarding predictions of personal stability versus improvement.

Visual Data Exploration: How Expert Astronomers UseFlipbook-Style Visual Approaches to Understand New Data

What are the cognitive processes in play when someone uses avisualization tool to interactively explore a new dataset? Here,we focus on one particular type of visualization—the scatterplot—which, despite (or perhaps because of) its simplicity, isstill one of the most frequently used plot types in many data-intensive disciplines. We conducted a pilot study to investigatehow expert astronomers interact with an unfamiliar dataset us-ing a visualization tool called Filtergraph, which supports rapidand easy visualization of large datasets. We present both quali-tative and quantitative results, including observations about thetemporal dynamics of visual data exploration as well as inter-esting behavioral patterns that we saw in our participants, suchas users taking “circular walks” through the data at various lev-els of abstraction.

The cultural evolution of complex linguistic constructions in artificial signlanguages

Though most documented sign languages make use of spaceto denote relationships between predicate arguments, studiesof emerging sign languages suggest that spatial reference doesnot emerge fully-formed but takes time to develop. We presentan artificial sign language learning experiment that expandsthe cultural evolutionary framework to investigate complexlinguistic constructions. Our results demonstrate the gradualemergence of consistent devices to distinguish between sen-tence arguments, some of which rely on iconic spatial con-trasts. These findings mirror data from emerging sign lan-guages and point to the cultural mechanisms that facilitate theevolution of complex linguistic structures.

Do Accurate Metacognitive Judgments Predict Successful Multimedia Learning?

Successful performance during multimedia learning requiresaccurate metacognitive judgments. However, little researchhas investigated the influence of accurate metacognitivejudgments for different representations of information (e.g.,text and diagram) on performance during multimedialearning. As such, we investigated if participants’metacognitive judgments for text and diagrams (i.e., contentevaluations; CEs) were significantly related to increasedperformance and higher confidence during multimedialearning. Metacognitive judgments and performance measureswere collected from 48 undergraduate participants during 18randomized trials. Results using multilevel modelingindicated that participants’ CEs for text-based content weresignificantly predictive of performance. Results also showedthat accurate CEs for diagrams interacted with accuratemultiple-choice responses to predict higher retrospectiveconfidence judgments (i.e., higher confidence). Identifyingmetacognitive judgments predictive of increased performanceduring multimedia learning has important theoretical,conceptual, and analytical implications.

Interactive Communicative Inference

In the search for an understanding of human communication,researchers often try to isolate listener and speaker roles andstudy them separately. Others claim that it is the intertwined-ness of these roles that makes human communication special.This close relationship between listener and speaker has beencharacterized by concepts such as common ground, backchan-neling, and alignment, but they are only part of the picture. Un-derlying these processes, there must be a mechanism for mak-ing inferences about our interlocutors’ understanding of wordsand gestures that allows us to communicate robustly and effi-ciently without assuming that we take the same words to havethe same meaning. In this paper, I explore this relationship be-tween language and concepts and propose an interactive mech-anism that can facilitate these latent conceptual inferences. Fi-nally, I show how this proposal paves the way for a more pre-cise account of the role of interaction in communication.

Cognitive and Attentional Process in Insight Problem Solving of the puzzle game “Tangram”

The purpose of this study is to demonstrate a constraint relaxation which is followed by the transition to an appropriate representation in insight problem solving. The puzzle game “Tangram” was used as a new insight problem, in which problem-solvers were presented a silhouette and asked to make the same configuration by arranging 7 pieces. At the beginning, problem-solvers had a constraint allocating the pieces into a geometric shape, but then relaxed this to reach the correct configuration at a later stage of problem solving. Participants’ subjective assessments of their confidence to reach the solution predicted neither the constraint relaxation nor the successful problem solving. However, eye-tracking data suggested that the successful problem-solvers tended to search the problem space more widely than the unsuccessful-problem solvers.

First Step is to Group Them: Task-Dynamic Model Validation for Human Multiagent Herding in a Less Constrained Task

Biological systems are capable of acting in a shared environment to produce emergent, self-organized behavior that is the result of the constraints imposed by local interactions– such as bird flocking or ant swarming behavior. These examples present minimal demands for a shared-intention between co-actors, whereas other instances necessitate the formation of a shared goal. In these goal-directed tasks, how much of the observed complexity can be explained by the constraints imposed by both the environment and adherence to the shared task goal? This paper begins to investigate this question by presenting results from a two-person cooperative “shepherding” task first developed in Nalepka et al. (2017) but with fewer constraints. Results provide further evidence that the emergent behavior is the result of the constraints imposed by the task. The included task-dynamic model suggests a general model that can be used to understand multiagent herding behavior in a variety of contexts.

Conflicts Processing among Multiple Frames of Reference: An ERP Study

People rely on various frames of reference (FORs), such as egocentric (EFOR) and intrinsic (IFOR), to represent spatial information. The present study examined electroencephalogram profiles on a two-cannon task, which could regulate the conflict of IFOR-IFOR (red cannon, blue cannon) and IFOR-EFOR (target cannon, observer), to elucidate the brain mechanisms of FOR conflict processing by using event-related potentials (ERPs). Results showed that both of the conflicts occurred in the reaction time (RT) and there was an interaction between them. ERP results showed more negative amplitudes on N2 (276-326 ms) and P3 (396- 726 ms) for IFOR-IFOR conflict of the 180° cannon angle condition and EFOR-IFOR conflict of the target cannon point-down condition. What’s more, there was also an interaction between these two conflicts on the P3 amplitudes (561-726 ms). In summary, our findings shed new light on the domain-specific conflict monitoring and domain-general executive control for the IFOR-IFOR and EFOR-IFOR conflicts.

Beyond Distributed Cognition: Towards a Taxonomy of Nonreductive Social Cognition

Studies of social cognition often assume a reductionist, computational-representational conceptual framework. Distributed cognition is one of the few extant conceptual frameworks for a nonreductive understanding of social cognition. This concept’s prototypical cases are exclusively of technical-scientific human institutions, including ships, cockpits, and the Hubble Space Telescope. In the first part of the paper, we outline the properties of distributed cognitive systems. We look at the case of wolf (Canis lupus) packs as an instance of distributed cognition in nonhuman systems. Nevertheless, a broad range of social cognitive phenomena across human and animal populations may not fit into this conceptual framework. We present a case study of bird flocks as a counterexample to distributed cognition. We propose “swarm intelligence” as an alternative concept of nonreductive social cognition. This is not to replace distributed cognition as a concept, but to add to and diversify the taxonomy of nonreductive social cognitive systems.

Modeling categorical perception with auditory neurons

It is well-known that the auditory perception of speech sounds is strongly influenced by the phonetic categorieswhich divide up acoustic space. This paper approaches the problem of modeling categorical perception from the ground up,using a linear model of the tuning properties of auditory neurons – the spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF). An STRFwhich discriminates voiced from voiceless stops was derived from the TIMIT corpus, and two computer simulations wereconducted to investigate its properties. In one simulation, this model neuron was found to exhibit a categorical response to alinear voice-onset-time continuum, closely tracking human behavior. In the second simulation, the STRF was found to exhibita less categorical, more linear response to a stop-voicing continuum, also in line with human behavior. These two simulationsshow that perceptual responses to speech, whether non-linear or veridical, can be modeled by the action of auditory neurons.

Brief Mindfulness Meditation Improves Attention in Novices

Past research has found that mindfulness meditation trainingimproves executive attention and that this effect could bedriven by more efficient allocation of resources on demandingattentional tasks, such as the Flanker Task. However, it is notclear whether these changes depend on long-term practice.We sought to investigate the effects of a brief, 10-minutemeditation session on attention in novice meditators,compared to a control activity. We also tested moderation byindividual differences in Neuroticism. We found thatparticipants randomly assigned to meditate for 10 minutesshowed improved performance on incongruent trials on aFlanker task, with no detriment in reaction times, indicatingbetter allocation of resources. Neuroticism moderated thiseffect, as only those low in Neuroticism showed improvedallocation of attentional resources following meditation.

Who makes use of prior knowledge in a curriculum on proportional reasoning?

Understanding proportions is a time-intensive process that does not come cheap during late childhood and early adolescence. It is fostered by learning experiences in which students have opportunities to explore, discuss and experiment with situations involving proportions. Children must undergo many informal learning opportunities before they can gain from direct instruction on proportional reasoning. In this study, we aimed to determine whether physics curricula focusing on the concept of density prepares students for learning from a curriculum on proportional reasoning. A 2x2 design with the factors “physics curricula” (with, without) and “concept used to introduce proportional reasoning” (speed, density) was applied to 253 children from 12 classrooms at the beginning of grade 5. We expected the “density, with physics curriculum” group to outperform the other three groups. However, only the students who scored in the highest quartile on an intelligence measure gained from the prior knowledge they had acquired through the physics curricula. The results show that curricula on proportional reasoning are worthwhile for all students in early adolescence. However, more capable students can boost their proportional reasoning if they have the chance to acquire prior knowledge through a physics curriculum.

The relationship between self-control in intertemporal choices and the control ofegocentric during perspective taking

We infer the thoughts and feelings of other people by takingtheir perspectives, the accuracy of which depends on abilitiesto control egocentric bias. Similar processes could arguablybe used to understand how we would be affected by futureevents, such as delayed rewards in intertemporal decisions, byallowing us to accurately take the perspective of future selves.In this paper, we test this idea in two studies. In Study 1, weattempted to lower preferences for delayed rewards toexamine if this redced abilities to control egocentric bias in avisual perspective-taking task. In Study 2, we examined theneural overlap in intertemporal decision-making and thecontrol of egocentric bias in a false-belief theory-of-mindtask. In both studies, a positive relationship was identifiedbetween behavioural and neural markers of egocentric biascontrol and preferences for delayed rewards. The overallpattern of results suggest the overlap in processes ofegocentric bias control and those that determine preferencesin intertemporal choices, and demonstrate for the first timethe effect of sexual arousal on social cognition in reducingabilities to separate one’s own perspective from others’.

The dot perspective task revisited: Evidence fordirectional effects

Humans are highly social creatures. Evidence from the dotperspective task suggests that humans automatically track theperspective of other individuals – a disposition that, if true,may help to facilitate social interaction. However, variants ofthe original dot perspective task suggest the alternativeinterpretation that the effect in the task is not due toperspective taking. Here, we present a new variant, usingimproved stimuli to address these issues. Our results replicateprevious findings, across both animate and inanimate stimuli,and suggest that the effect is due to directional cueing ratherthan automatic perspective taking.

Creation of Spatial Mental Models with Figural Stimuli:Validation of the Emoji-based Spatial Integration Task

The current study examined a new spatial integration (SI)task, based on figural rather than linguistic stimuli, tomeasure the construct of mental modeling ability.Previous tasks conflated linguistic ability with mentalmodeling ability by requiring sentence processing, whichmay have contributed to mixed findings with respect tothe relationship between mental model ability andworking memory capacity (WMC). The figural spatialintegration task produced the canonical continuity effect,such that discontinuous items had lower accuracy thancontinuous items. Furthermore, WMC and visuospatialability predicted SI task performance, and both werestronger predictors for the continuous condition. Theinteractions between predictors and task conditionssuggest reliance on heuristics and/or rehearsal duringperformance of the more difficult discontinuous items.

A Domain-Independent Approach of Cognitive Appraisal Augmented by HigherCognitive Layer of Ethical Reasoning

According to cognitive appraisal theory, emotion in an indi-vidual is the result of how a situation/event is evaluated by theindividual. This evaluation has different outcomes among peo-ple and it is often suggested to be operationalised by a set ofrules or beliefs acquired by the subject throughout develop-ment. Unfortunately, this view is particularly detrimental forcomputational applications of emotion appraisal. In fact, it re-quires providing a knowledge base that is particularly difficultto establish and manage, especially in systems designed forhighly complex scenarios, such as social robots. In addition,according to appraisal theory, an individual might elicit morethan one emotion at a time in reaction to an event. Hence, de-termining which emotional state should be attributed in rela-tionship to a specific event is another critical issue not yet fullyaddressed by the available literature. In this work, we showthat: (i) the cognitive appraisal process can be realised withouta complex set of rules; instead, we propose that this processcan be operationalised by knowing only the positive or nega-tive perceived effect the event has on the subject, thus facili-tating extensibility and integrability of the emotional system;(ii) the final emotional state to attribute in relation to a specificsituation is better explained by ethical reasoning mechanisms.These hypotheses are supported by our experimental results.Therefore, this contribution is particularly significant to pro-vide a more simple and generalisable explanation of cognitiveappraisal theory and to promote the integration between theo-ries of emotion and ethics studies, currently often neglected bythe available literature.

Tracking the temporal course of counterfactual understanding

This paper explores the dual meaning of counterfactual conditionals, such as ‘if there had been gloves, then therewould have been scarves’, by tracking the temporal course to envisage the possibility corresponding to the conjecture ‘therewere gloves and there were scarves’ and the presupposed facts, ‘there were no gloves and there were no scarves’. To testthis, we used the visual world paradigm, in which counterfactual and indicative conditionals were heard while four imagescorresponding to the conjecture, such as an image of gloves and scarves, and the presupposed facts, such as an image of nogloves and no scarves, and two distractors were shown on the screen and eye movements were monitored. We found thatpeople looked at the affirmative image in the indicative conditional, and both types of images (affirmative and negative) in thecounterfactual conditional. Results support the dual meaning of counterfactuals.

Investigating Sensitivity to Shared Information and Personal Experience in Children’s Use of Majority Information

When learning from others, rather than simply following the majority’s opinion, we need to accurately evaluate the quality of the information both the majority and the minority provide, and integrate that information with our own personal experience. This is especially true when the majority’s opinion is based on lower quality information, because they shared the same evidence rather than collecting evidence independently. Previous work demonstrated that adults are sensitive to the quality of the majority’s information, consistent with the predictions of a Bayesian rational model (Whalen, Griffiths, & Buchsbaum, in press). In two behavioural experiments, we investigated how preschoolers combine testimony from a majority that conflicts with a minority or with the child’s own personal evidence. Unlike adults, children over-relied on the majority when given only testimony. However, when also given their own conflicting evidence, children relied significantly less on the majority and over-relied on their own evidence. These findings help explain why children may follow the majority at times, but in others trust their own judgements.

Do Speaker’s Emotions influence their Language Production? Studying the Influence of Disgust and Amusement on Alignment in Interactive Reference

The influence of emotion on (the early stages of) speech production processes, notably content selection has received little scholarly attention. Goudbeek & Krahmer (2012) found evidence for alignment at the conceptual level: speakers may start using a dispreferred attribute over a preferred attribute in their referring expressions when they are primed by a pre- recorded female voice in a preceding interaction. The current study aimed to assess the role of emotion (using amusement and disgust) in alignment, while simultaneously replicating this finding in a more naturalistic setting involving two human participants in naturalistic dialogue. Our results replicate the findings by Goudbeek & Krahmer (2012), generalizing their findings to a much more naturalistic setting. In addition, we found that amused, but not disgusted speakers tend to use the preferred attribute more to describe objects to their conversational partner.

Nudging Problematic Smartphone Use to a Lower Level

Smartphone usage has evolved in people’s lives from necessity to habit and in some cases leading to compulsive use and addiction. However, only a little research has been performed on the prevention of Problematic Smartphone Usage (PSU). Behavioral economics has been applied to investigate how smartphone users respond to nudges that try to lower their smartphone usage. Findings revealed that the Total Screen On Time (SOT) decreased when nudging smartphone users with information on their usage behaviors. Intermittent glancing, as well as the median session time increased, and the reduction in SOT was no longer statistically significant in the observation period after the nudges were no longer applied, suggesting relapse in smartphone usage behavior.

The effect of acute physical activity on children’s memory for language

Research on the relationship between acute physical activityand cognition in children has often found beneficial effects ofexercise on a variety of cognitive abilities. One domain thatremains underexplored, however, is the relationship betweenexercise and long-term memory in children, and in particularwhether the general-domain effects observed in previousstudies could translate to a school-based learning activity,such as vocabulary learning. To address this issue, this studyfocused on the possible effects that a bout of moderate,aerobic physical activity could have on the immediate anddelayed recall of newly acquired word forms and form-meaning connections of children in a school setting. In linewith previous research, the results show a positive effect ofexercise, but only for word form recall. This study expandsour understanding of the differential effects of exercise onmemory, while raising questions regarding the possiblemoderating influence of gender and memory consolidation.

Replacing Language: Children Use Non-Linguistic Cues and Comparison in Category Formation

Language is a powerful instrument for extracting relational information from stimuli. In a label extension task common labels invite comparison processes that help children focus on the more subtle relational similarity and away from the readily available perceptual similarity of the stimuli. The current experiment aims to explore whether non-linguistic representations of category membership are sufficient to invite such abstractions of relational information. Preschool children were asked to extend a category to either a relational or an object match. When given the opportunity to compare two instances of the category, and provided with a non- linguistic cue children extended the category to the relational match. These results further extend the benefit of comparison in learning, and suggest that language labels are not the only cue children can use in category formation.

When is Likely Unlikely: Investigating the Variability of Vagueness

An important part of explaining how people communicate isto understand how people relate language to entities in theworld. In describing measurements, people prefer to use quali-tative words like ‘tall’ without precise applicability conditions,also known as vague words. The use of vague language varieswidely across contexts, individuals, and tasks (single referencevs. comparisons between targets), but despite this variabil-ity, is used quite successfully. A potential strategy for usingvague language is to leverage the set of alternative descrip-tors to settle on the best option. To determine whether peopleuse this strategy, we conducted an experiment where partici-pants picked vague words from sets of alternatives to describeeither probability or color values. We varied the set of alter-natives from which participants could choose. Empirical evi-dence supports the hypothesis that people use the set of avail-able options to pick vague descriptors. The theoretical impli-cations of this work are discussed.

Counterfactuals, indicative conditionals, and negation under uncertainty:Are there cross-cultural differences?

In this paper we study selected argument forms involvingcounterfactuals and indicative conditionals under uncertainty.We selected argument forms to explore whether people withan Eastern cultural background reason differently about con-ditionals compared to Westerners, because of the differencesin the location of negations. In a 2 × 2 between-participantsdesign, 63 Japanese university students were allocated to fourgroups, crossing indicative conditionals and counterfactuals,and each presented in two random task orders. The datashow close agreement between the responses of Easterners andWesterners. The modal responses provide strong support forthe hypothesis that conditional probability is the best predic-tor for counterfactuals and indicative conditionals. Finally,the grand majority of the responses are probabilistically coher-ent, which endorses the psychological plausibility of choosingcoherence-based probability logic as a rationality frameworkfor psychological reasoning research.

Abductive, Causal, and Counterfactual ConditionalsUnder Incomplete Probabilistic Knowledge

We study abductive, causal, and non-causal conditionals inindicative and counterfactual formulations using probabilis-tic truth table tasks under incomplete probabilistic knowledge(N = 80). We frame the task as a probability-logical inferenceproblem. The most frequently observed response type acrossall conditions was a class of conditional event interpretationsof conditionals; it was followed by conjunction interpreta-tions. An interesting minority of participants neglected someof the relevant imprecision involved in the premises when in-ferring lower or upper probability bounds on the target con-ditional/counterfactual (“halfway responses”). We discuss theresults in the light of coherence-based probability logic and thenew paradigm psychology of reasoning

Pragmatics Influence Children’s Use of Majority Information

Do children always conform to a majority’s testimony, or do the pragmatics of that testimony matter? We investigate children’s reasoning about mapping a novel word to a referent in an object-labeling task. Across four conditions, we modified the testimony in an object-labeling task, to account for pragmatic principles, so that the majority does and does not provide an explicit opinion about the alternative object chosen by the minority. Four- and 5-year-olds were given a choice between an object endorsed by a three-person majority, or one endorsed by a single minority informant. In the unendorsed condition, informants explicitly unendorsed the unchosen object. In the nothing condition, informants said nothing about the unchosen object. In the ignorance condition, informants explicitly expressed uncertainty about the unchosen object, and in the hidden condition, the chosen object was the only one present at the time of the endorsement. Children were most likely to endorse the majority object in the unendorsed condition, in which the majority explicitly stated that the label applied to only one referent, whereas in the hidden condition, where only one object at a time was present in the discourse, children chose objects endorsed by the majority and the minority equally, with the other two conditions intermediate. This suggests that children might not simply have a conformity bias; rather, they are sensitive to the majority’s implied intentions when learning from testimony.

A categorical (fixed point) foundation for cognition: (adjoint) corecursion

Computationalism has been the pre-eminent framework for models of mind, since the cognitive revolution. However,the plethora of apparently incommensurate approaches seems to undermine hope for a common computational foundation.Category theory provides a mathematically rigorous foundation for computation that includes recursion and corecursion. Weshow that corecursion unifies various cognitive behaviours for comparison and contrast in a principled and novel way. Forinstance, Chomsky’s merge function is a universal morphism, which has a dual, called comerge. One implication of this workis that corecursion appears to be the rule rather than the (human) exception in contrast to Chomsky’s view of recursion.

Dual-routes and the cost of computing least-costs

Theories of cognition that posit complementary dual-route processes afford better fits to the data when each routeexplains a part of the data not explained by the other route. However, such theories must also explain why each route isinvoked, lest one can fit any data set with enough routes. One possible explanation is that route selection is based on a least-costprinciple: the route that requires fewer cognitive resources (including time) relative to the goal at hand. We investigated thisexplanation with a dual-route version of visual search, where the target could be identified via opposing (easy or hard formsof) feature and conjunction search conditions. The data support a contextualized version of the least-cost principle in that thecost of computing least-cost also influences route selection: participants assessed alternatives, but only when the cost of thatassessment was relatively low.

The Effect of Economic Scarcity Priming on Perception of Race

Existing research suggests that White individuals are morelikely to categorise biracial faces as Black in conditions ofresource scarcity. It has been theorised that this effect is dueto in-group boundaries becoming more exclusive in scarceconditions. An alternative explanation refers to implicit socio-economic association between Black individuals and lowerlevel of resources. These two approaches entail differentpredictions for Black participants performing thecategorisation task. If scarcity prompts greater in-groupexclusivity, Black participants should, ceteris paribus,categorise more biracial faces as White. If, however, scarcityinvokes socio-economic status associations, Blackparticipants should categories biracial faces in the same wayas White participants. Experiment 1, explored the effects ofpriming on White and Black groups. It provided support forthe implicit socio-economic association theory. Furthermore,experiment 2 on Asian sample, provided additional support asAsian participants showed the same pattern of response. Thepaper discusses implications of these findings.

Perception is in the Details:A Predictive Coding Account of the Psychedelic Phenomenon

Psychedelic substances are used for clinical applications(e.g., treatment of addictions, anxiety and depression) aswell as an investigative tool in neuroscientific research.Recently it has been proposed that the psychedelicphenomenon stems from the brain reaching an increasedentropic state. In this paper, we use the predictive codingframework to formalize the idea of an entropic brain. Wepropose that the increased entropic state is created whentop-down predictions in affected brain areas break up anddecompose into many more overly detailed predictions dueto hyper activation of 5-HT2A receptors in layer Vpyramidal neurons. We demonstrate that this novel, unifiedtheoretical account can explain the various and sometimescontradictory effects of psychedelics such as hallucination,heightened sensory input, synesthesia, increased trait ofopenness, ‘ego death’ and time dilation by up-regulation ofa variety of mechanisms the brain can use to minimizeprediction under the constraint of decomposed prediction.

Repetition improves memory by strengthening existing traces: Evidence from paired-associate learning under midazolam

Here, we examined how repetition under midazolam, abenzodiazepine that prevents the storage of novel associations,affects cued-recall performance of paired-associates. We contrastedword pairs that were initially studied and tested repeatedly withoutany successful recall prior to the midazolam injection, with otherpairs that were studied for the first time after the injection ofmidazolam. According to our SAC (Source of ActivationConfusion) memory model, repetition leads to strengtheningexisting memory traces rather than creating multiple traces for eachrepetition. As such, it predicts that repetition under midazolamshould benefit only pairs that were originally studied prior to themidazolam injection. This prediction was confirmed. The resultssuggest that memory traces for pairs studied prior to the midazolaminjection were strengthened under midazolam. However, word pairsthat had not been studied prior to the injection were not bound inlong-term memory because midazolam prevents the formation ofnew associations.

When Less Isn’t More: A Real-World Fraction Intervention Study

Although an understanding of fractions is a critical precursorfor other mathematical concepts, including proportionalreasoning, algebra, and success in STEM fields, surveys ofmathematics education in the United States indicate thatschool-age children lack age-appropriate math skills andproficiency. Thus, understanding the critical precursors offraction knowledge is important for the development ofinstructional materials. The aim of the present study was toexamine whether instructional format affected children’slearning and transfer of fraction concepts, and whetherindividual variables such as executive function and mathknowledge moderated these effects. Six- to 8-year-oldchildren participated in a longitudinal, pre/post test design, inwhich they received a fraction-training intervention.Critically, we manipulated the extent to which real-worldinstruction was grounded in visual vs. symbolicrepresentations. We find that 1 st and 2 nd graders were able tolearn fraction concepts following this intervention, despitehaving no formal fraction education. The extent to which theinstructional stimuli were grounded in visual vs. symbolicrepresentations affected children’s proportional reasoningknowledge in a transfer task, and condition effects weremoderated my children’s working memory and prior mathknowledge. This work has implications for instructionaldesign and curriculum development in the classroom.

Is ambiguity detection in haptic imagery possible? Evidence for Enactive imaginings

A classic discussion about visual imagery is whether it affords reinterpretation, like discovering two interpretations in the duck/rabbit illustration. Recent findings converge on reinterpretation being possible in visual imagery, suggesting functional equivalence with pictorial representations. However, it is unclear whether such reinterpretations are necessarily a visual-pictorial achievement. To assess this, 68 participants were briefly presented 2-d ambiguous figures. One figure was presented visually, the other via manual touch alone. Afterwards participants mentally rotated the memorized figures as to discover a novel interpretation. A portion (20.6%) of the participants detected a novel interpretation in visual imagery, replicating previous research. Strikingly, 23.6% of participants were able to reinterpret figures they had only felt. That reinterpretation truly involved haptic processes was further supported, as some participants performed co-thought gestures on an imagined figure during retrieval. These results are promising for further development of an Enactivist approach to imagination.

Categorical vs Coordinate Relationships do not reduce to spatial frequencydifferences

Categorical and coordinate stimulus processing were hypoth-esized by Kosslyn (1987) to be lateralized visual tasks, dif-ferentiated by task-relevant spatial frequencies. Slotnick et al.(2001) directly tested Kosslyn’s hypothesis and concluded thatthe lateralization presents only when tasks are sufficiently dif-ficult. Our differential encoding model is a three layer neuralnetwork that accounts for lateralization in visual processingvia the biologically plausible mechanism of differences in con-nection spread of long-range lateral neural connections (Hsiao,Cipollini, & Cottrell, 2013). We show that our model accountsfor Slotnick’s data and that Slotnick’s analysis does not con-vincingly explain their results. Instead, we propose that Koss-lyn’s initial hypothesis was based on an incorrect assumption:categorical and coordinate stimuli are not solely differentiatedby spatial frequencies. The results that our model capturescannot be reproduced by Ivry and Robertson’s (1998) Dou-ble Filtering by Frequency theory, which is driven solely bylateralized spatial frequency processing.

Refuting Overconfidence: Refutation Texts Prevent Detrimental Effects of Misconceptions on Text Comprehension and Metacomprehension Accuracy in the Domain of Statistics

Refutation texts are beneficial for removing misconceptions and supporting comprehension in science. Whether these beneficial effects hold true in the domain of statistics is, however, an open question. Moreover, the role of refutation texts for the accuracy in judging one’s own comprehension (metacomprehension accuracy) has received little attention. Therefore, we conducted an experiment in which students with varying levels of statistical misconceptions read either a standard text or a refutation text in statistics, judged their text comprehension, and completed a comprehension test. The results showed that when students read the standard text, having more misconceptions resulted in poorer text comprehension and more inaccurate metacomprehension as indicated by overconfident predictions. In contrast, when students read the refutation text, the number of misconceptions was unrelated to text comprehension and metacomprehension accuracy. Apparently, refutation texts help students to pay attention to inaccuracies in their knowledge and, thereby, can promote self-regulated learning from texts.

Varieties of Numerical Estimation: A Unified Framework

There is an ongoing debate over the psychophysical functionsthat best fit human data from numerical estimation tasks. Totest whether one psychophysical function could account fordata across diverse tasks, we examined 40 kindergartners, 38first graders, 40 second graders and 40 adults’ estimates usingtwo fully crossed 2 × 2 designs, crossing symbol (symbolic,non-symbolic) and boundedness (bounded, unbounded) onfree number-line tasks (Experiment 1) and crossing the samefactors on anchored tasks (Experiment 2). Across all 8 tasks,88.84% of participants provided estimates best fit by a mixedlog-linear model, and the weight of the logarithmiccomponent (λ) decreased with age. After controlling for age,the λ significantly predicted arithmetic skills, whereasparameters of other models failed to do so. Results suggestthat the logarithmic-to-linear shift theory provides a unifiedaccount of numerical estimation and provides uniquelyaccurate predictions for mathematical proficiency.

Towards a Computational Analogical Theory of Mind

Several theories about Theory of Mind (ToM) have beenproposed. The most well-known of these are Theory Theoryand Simulation Theory, although alternative and hybridtheories do exist. One such theory, proposed by Bach (2011,2014), is based on the Structure-Mapping theory of analogy,which has been shown to play a key role in cognitivedevelopment. There is evidence that children are more likely topass false belief tasks when trained using stories that are easyto compare via structural alignment, as opposed to stories thatare difficult to compare in this way (Hoyos, Horton & Gentner,2015). This paper shows how a computational model based onBach’s account can provide an explanation for the Hoyos et al.training study and proposes directions for future research onhuman subjects.

A Theory of Resonance: Towards an Ecological Cognitive Architecture

This paper may be seen as a blueprint for an ecological cognitive architecture. Ecological psychology, I contend,must be complemented with a story about the role of the CNS in perception, action, and cognition. Such a story must be atheory of resonance compatible with the main tenets of ecological psychology. I offer here the two main elements of such atheory: a framework (Anderson’s neural reuse) and a methodology (multi-scale fractal DST).

A model of structure learning, inference, and generation for scene understanding

Humans possess rich knowledge of the structure of the world, including co-occurrences among entities, and co-variation among their discrete and continuous features. But how people learn, infer and predict this structure is not wellunderstood. Here we explore everyday scene understanding as a case study of people’s structural knowledge and reasoning.We introduce a probabilistic model over scene graphs that can learn the relational structure of objects and their arrangementsand support inference and generation. Our model was able to learn the underlying structure of real-world scenes, and use it forinference and compression. In two human psychophysical experiments we found that a corresponding computational cognitivemodel was able to explain how people learn novel scene distributions and use it for classification and construction. Our workrepresents the first computational theory of human scene understanding that can account for people’s rich capacity for learningand reasoning about structure.

Challenging the superficial similarities superiority account for analogical retrieval

The predominant view concerning determinants ofanalogical retrieval is that it is preferentially guided by superficialcues. In order to test the cognitive plausibility of a structuralsimilarities-based retrieval, we constructed a story-recall task inwhich life-like scenarios shared structural correspondences. InExperiment 1, we showed that such structural similarities induceretrievals when the participant had several source candidatesituations sharing superficial similarities with the target cue.Experiment 2 was designed to test whether the encoding wassufficiently oriented on structural similarities to drive retrievals,even if the participants possess only one source candidate situationwith superficial matches in memory. The results of the two presentexperiments lead us to conclude that in some contexts, abstractencoding induces a superiority of structural similarities oversuperficial ones in retrieval. Further implications for analogicalretrieval approaches are discussed.

Improving a Fundamental Measure of Lexical Association

Pointwise mutual information (PMI), a simple measure of lexical association, is part of several algorithms used as models of lexical semantic memory. Typically, it is used as a component of more complex distributional models rather than in isolation. We show that when two simple techniques are applied—(1) down-weighting co-occurrences involving low- frequency words in order to address PMI’s so-called “frequency bias,” and (2) defining co-occurrences as counts of “events in which instances of word1 and word2 co-occur in a context” rather than “contexts in which word1 and word2 co- occur”—then PMI outperforms default parameterizations of word embedding models in terms of how closely it matches human relatedness judgments. We also identify which down- weighting techniques are most helpful. The results suggest that simple measures may be capable of modeling certain phenomena in semantic memory, and that complex models which incorporate PMI might be improved with these modifications.

Priming the production of implications

We present two experiments investigating the production of implicit constructions. Using a confederate scripting paradigm we find that after making an inference participants were more likely to subsequently produce an implicature. This effect occurred at a global and a local level and was unaffected by the perceived role of the conversational partner. Our findings demonstrate that the choice of whether to be implicit is determined by the activation levels of representations specific to implicatures and that inference and implications have overlapping processing representations.

How could a rational analysis model explain?

Rational analysis is an influential but contested account of howprobabilistic modeling can be used to construct non-mechanistic but self-standing explanatory models of the mind.In this paper, I disentangle and assess several possibleexplanatory contributions which could be attributed to rationalanalysis. Although existing models suffer from evidentialproblems that question their explanatory power, I argue thatrational analysis modeling can complement mechanistictheorizing by providing models of environmental affordances.

Randomness in binary sequences: Conceptualizing and connecting two recentdevelopments

Recent theoretical research has shown that the assumptionsthat both laypeople and researchers make about randomsequences can be erroneous. One strand of research showedthat the probability of non-occurrence of streaks of repeatedoutcomes (e.g., HHHHHH) is much higher than that for amore irregular sequence (e.g., HTTHTH) in short series ofcoin flips. This tallies with human judgments of theirlikelihood of occurrence, which have conventionally beencharacterized as inaccurate and heuristic-driven. Anotherstrand of research has shown that patterns of hits and missesin games like basketball, traditionally seen as evidence for theabsence of a hot-hand effect, actually support the presence ofthe effect. I argue that a useful way of conceptualizing thesetwo distinct phenomena is in terms of the distribution ofdifferent sequences of outcomes over time: Specifically, thatstreaks of a repeated outcome cluster whereas less regularpatterns are more evenly distributed.

Using punctuation as a marker of sincerity and affective convergence during texting

Face-to-face communication is a rich, natural form ofcommunication that incorporates multimodal behavioral cuesbelying meaning and intention. However, computer-mediatedcommunication (e.g., texting) removes many of themultimodal cues in face-to-face communication (e.g., vocalprosody). Recent research has suggested that punctuationmight mimic vocal prosody in text (Gunraj et al., 2016), butthere is no clear indication of what the overall effects may be.Therefore, the current study investigates the use ofpunctuation to express intonation. We first replicate Gunrajand his colleagues by showing that a single word ending in aperiod promotes the appraisal of negative affect. Interestingly,we extend this research by demonstrating that intonationalpunctuation has the potential to increase social distance,which our preliminary results suggest may occur throughprocesses of emotional contagion and interactive alignment.

Influencing Network Graph Perception and Judgment: Effects of DirectConnections, Base Rates, and Visual Layout Proximity on Social Network Analysis

Social network graphs are often used to help informjudgments in a variety of domains, such as public health, lawenforcement, and political science. Across two studies, weexamined how graph features influenced probabilisticjudgments in graph-based social network analysis andidentified multiple heuristics that participants used to informthese judgments. Study 1 demonstrated that participants’judgments were influenced by information about directconnections, base rates, and layout proximity, andparticipants’ self-reported strategies also reflected use of thisinformation. Study 2 replicated findings from Study 1 andprovided additional insight into the hierarchical ordering ofthese strategies and the decision process underlyingjudgments from social network graphs.

Explicit Predictions for Illness Statistics

People’s predictions for real-world events have been shown tobe well-calibrated to the true environmental statistics (e.g.Griffiths and Tenenbaum 2006). Previous work, however, hasfocused on predictions for these events by aggregating acrossobservers, making a single estimate for the total durationgiven a current duration. Here, we focus on assessingpredictions for both the mean and form of distributions in thedomain of illness duration prediction at the individual level.We assess understanding for both acute illnesses for whichpeople might have experience, as well as chronic conditionsfor which people are less likely to have knowledge. Our datasuggests that for common acute illnesses people canaccurately estimate both the mean and form of thedistribution. For less common acute illnesses and chronicillnesses, people have a tendency to overestimate the meanduration, but still accurately predict the distribution form.

Information theoretic factors in marking linguistic focus:A laboratory-language approach

We present an experimental study investigating the role ofinformation-theoretic factors in determining patterns of redun-dancy and focus in language and other communication sys-tems. Pairs of participants played a simple communicationgame using a non-linguistic visual medium to send messagesto each other. We manipulated noise, effort, and time pres-sures and measured message length, redundancy, and accuracy.Participants behaved as predicted based on an information-theoretic model, with message length and redundancy varyingaccording to circumstance, but accuracy remaining constant.

Manual Response Dynamics Reflect Rapid Integration of Intonational Informationduring Reference Resolution

Intonation plays an integral role in comprehending spoken lan-guage. It encodes post-lexical pragmatic functions such as sen-tence modality and discourse contexts. The present experimentinvestigates how and when listeners integrate intonational in-formation to anticipate reference resolution. While most workon the real-time processes of intonation-based intention recog-nition has utilized eye tracking, the present study uses themouse tracking paradigm, a valuable complementary methodto investigate the time course of speech processing. Partici-pants had to choose an interpretation based on pre-recorded in-structions containing different intonation contours. Recordingsof the x,y coordinates of participants’ computer mouse move-ments reveal that listeners integrate intonational informationrapidly as soon as they become available and anticipate poten-tial referential interpretations early on.

Beyond candidate inferences: People treat analogies as probabilistic truths

People use analogies for many cognitive purposes such asbuilding mental models, making inspired guesses, andextracting relational structure. Here we examine whether andhow analogies may have more direct influence on knowledge:Do people treat analogies as probabilistically trueexplanations for uncertain propositions?We report an experiment that explores how a suggestedanalogy can influence people’s confidence in inferences.Participants made predictions while simultaneouslyevaluating a suggested analogy and observed evidence. In twoconditions, the evidence is either consistent with or in conflictwith propositions based on the suggested analogy. Weanalyze the responses statistically and in a psychologicallyplausible Bayesian network model. We find that analogies areused for more than just generating candidate inferences. Theyact as probabilistic truths that affect the integration ofevidence and confidence in both the target and sourcedomains. People readily treat analogies not as a one-wayprojection from source to target, but as a mutually informativeconnection.

A Model-based Approach for Assessing Attentional Biases in People with Depressive Symptoms

Biased attention is assumed to play an important role in the etiology and maintenance of depression and depressive symptoms. In this paper, we used data from a categorization task and an associated model to assess the attentional bias of people with varying levels of depressive symptoms. Attentional bias was operationalized as the parameter estimate in a prototype model of categorization. For estimation, we used a Bayesian hierarchical mixture approach. We expected to find a positive correlation between depressive symptoms and an AB for negative material and a negative correlation between depressive symptoms and a bias toward positive material. Despite good model fit, Bayesian regression analyses revealed weak or moderate evidence in favor of the null model assuming no association between attentional preferences and depressive symptoms, both for negative and positive material.

Is it fair? Textual effects on the salience of moral foundations

Many of the important decisions we make have moral implications. Moral Foundations Theory (Haidt & Joseph, 2004) identifies 5 distinct styles of moral reasoning that may be applied to such decisions. This paper explores how reading text that emphasizes one of these styles might affect our reasoning. After participants read a series of tweets that emphasized the Fairness/Cheating foundation they exhibited an increased reliance on this style compared to when they read tweets emphasizing the Care/Harm foundation. This affected participants’ answers to a questionnaire designed to measure the perceived importance of the different foundations, as well as in their rating of the foundations evident in other tweets. Interestingly, this effect was short lived and was not observed for the Care/Harm foundation. These results suggest that exposure to the moral reasoning of others might temporarily influence what moral arguments we are likely to accept and employ.

Estimating Causal Power between Binary Cause and Continuous Outcome

Previous studies of causal learning heavily focused on binary outcomes; little is known about causal learning with continuous outcomes. The present paper proposes a qualitative extension of the causal power theory to the situation where a binary cause influences a continuous effect, and induces causal power under various ceiling situations with the continuous outcomes. To test the predictions, we systematically manipulated the type of outcome (continuous vs. percentage vs. binary) and the contingency information. The experiment shows that people estimate causal strength based on the linear-sum rule for continuous outcomes and the noisy-OR rule for binary outcomes. In the partial ceiling situation where causal power is partially inferred but not precisely estimated, the distribution of participants’ judgments was bimodal with one mode at the minimum value and the other at the maximum value, suggesting some participants made conservative estimates while others made optimistic estimates. These results are generally consistent with the predictions of the causal power theory. Theoretical implications and future directions are discussed.

Spatial Training and Mathematics: The Moderating Effect of Handedness

The positive relationship between spatial ability andmathematical skills is a classical result in developmental andcognitive psychology. Given this correlational relationship,researchers have tried to establish whether spatial training canincrease mathematical ability. Such research has providedmixed results. In this study, we analysed the effects of twotypes of spatial training and handedness on primary schoolchildren’s arithmetical ability. The participants were pre-testedon a test of arithmetic and assigned to one of three groups: (a)one hour of mental rotation and translation training, (b) onehour of mental translation training only, or (c) a no-contactgroup. The results showed no significant difference betweentraining groups and a significant interaction between traininggroup and category of handedness. Interestingly, onlyextremely right-handed children in the mental rotation andtranslation group seemed to benefit from the training. Theseoutcomes suggest that any spatial training needs to includemental rotation activities to be effective, and that therelationship between spatial training and achievementmathematics appears to be moderated by handedness.

Dissolving the Grounding Problem: How the Pen is Mightier than the Sword

The computational metaphor for mind is still the central guiding idea in cognitive science despite many insightfuland well-founded rejections of it. There is good reason for its staying power: when we are at our cognitive best, we reasonabout our world with our concepts. But the challengers are right, I argue, in insisting that no reductive account of that capacityis forthcoming. Here I describe an externalist account that grounds representations in organism-level engagement with itsenvironment, not in its neural activity.

The Causal Frame Problem: An Algorithmic Perspective

The Frame Problem (FP) is a puzzle in philosophy of mindand epistemology, articulated by the Stanford Encyclopedia ofPhilosophy as follows: “How do we account for our apparentability to make decisions on the basis only of what is relevantto an ongoing situation without having explicitly to considerall that is not relevant?” In this work, we focus on the causalvariant of the FP, the Causal Frame Problem (CFP). Assumingthat a reasoner’s mental causal model can be (implicitly) repre-sented by a causal Bayes net, we first introduce a notion calledPotential Level (PL). PL, in essence, encodes the relative po-sition of a node with respect to its neighbors in a causal Bayesnet. Drawing on the psychological literature on causal judg-ment, we substantiate the claim that PL may bear on how timeis encoded in the mind. Using PL, we propose an inferenceframework, called the PL-based Inference Framework (PLIF),which permits a boundedly-rational approach to the CFP, for-mally articulated at Marr’s algorithmic level of analysis. Weshow that our proposed framework, PLIF, is consistent withseveral findings in the causal judgment literature, and that PLand PLIF make a number of predictions, some of which arealready supported by existing findings.

Experimental and Computational Investigation of the Effect of Caffeine onHuman Time Perception

Perception of time is an active process that takes place contin-ually. However, we are yet to learn its exact mechanisms con-clusively. The temporal bisection task is ideal to investigate thecircuitry underlying time perception. Caffeine, a commonlyused stimulant, has been known to play a role in modulation oftime perception. The objective of this article is to explore therole of caffeine, a neuromodulator, in the perception of time inhuman beings by conducting suitable experiments. The exper-iment shows that an expansion of time is perceived by subjectsafter caffeine ingestion and that caffeine has an acceleratingeffect on our time perception system. Additionally, we presenta preliminary 2-step decision model that fits the results of theexperiment and potentially gives insights into the mechanismsof caffeine. We conclude by pointing out future directions to-wards a more biologically realistic computational model.

Peculiarity doesn't trump ordinarity: On recognition memory for exceptions to the category rule

While exceptions to a regularity might be rare, categories that have exceptions are not. Previous studies on learning categories that have exceptions suggested special status of exceptional items in memory (e.g. Palmeri & Nosofsky, 1995, Sakamoto and Love, 2004). However, this might be true only for a special kind of exceptions – those that call for forming complex binding structures, and could be learned only if they are fully memorized. In the two experiments in this study, we show that memory for exceptions is not better than memory for regular category members (Experiment 1). On the contrary, both children and adults had better memory for the features of regular items (Experiment 2). In addition, adults, but not 4- year-olds, showed better memory for the rule than for probabilistic features. The overall results challenge the idea of the special status of exceptions in memory.

Modeling scope ambiguity resolution as pragmatic inference:Formalizing differences in child and adult behavior

Investigations of scope ambiguity resolution suggest that childbehavior differs from adult behavior, with children strugglingto access inverse scope interpretations. For example, childrenoften fail to accept Every horse didn’t succeed to mean not allthe horses succeeded. Current accounts of children’s scope be-havior involve both pragmatic and processing factors. Inspiredby these accounts, we use the Rational Speech Act frameworkto articulate a formal model that yields a more precise, ex-planatory, and predictive description of the observed develop-mental behavior.

Surprisingly: Marker of Surprise Readings or Intensifier?

We investigate the influence of the adverb surprisingly on themeaning of the quantity words few and many, which them-selves have been associated with a reading expressing sur-prise. To learn about the meaning contribution of “surprise”,we compare surprisingly with the intensifier incredibly anda compared to phrase explicitly marking surprise. Based onan empirical measure of subjects’ expectations about everydayevents, a Bayesian model uses data from a sentence judgmenttask to infer likely levels of surprise associated with the differ-ent constructions of interest.

Developing cognitive flexibility in solving arithmetic word problems

In problem solving situation, cognitive flexibility appears tobe a major skill. Fostering cognitive flexibility is therefore aspecific stake in mathematics education. This researchintroduces a learning method to develop mathematicalconcepts when solving word arithmetic problems. The studywas conducted with 8 classes (4 th -5 th Grades) from high-priority education schools in the Paris area following thisprotocol: pre-tests, 5 learning sessions for experimental andcontrol groups, post-tests. During learning sessions, studentsstudied arithmetic word problems that can be solved in twodifferent ways: an expansion strategy and a factorization one.The experimental teaching method, based on arecategorization principle, allowed experimental students toimprove more than the control students in ability to use thefactorization strategy even in contexts where it is the lessintuitive and to consider the two successful strategies.Educational entailments of our finding are discussed.

Aging of the Exploring Mind: Older Adults Deviate more from Optimality inComplex Choice Environments

Older adults (OA) need to make many important and difficultdecisions. Often, there are too many options available to ex-plore exhaustively, creating the ubiquitous tradeoff betweenexploration and exploitation. How do OA make these com-plex tradeoffs? We investigated age-related shifts in solvingexploration-exploitation tradeoffs depending on the complex-ity of the choice environment. Participants played four andeight option bandit problems with numbers of gambles and av-erage rewards available on the screen. OA reliably performedworse in a more complex choice environment and were alsomore deviant from an optimality model (Thompson sampling),which keeps track of uncertainty beyond just the mean or lastreward. OA seem to process important information in morecomplex choice environments sub-optimally, suggesting lim-ited representations of future rewards. This interpretation fitsto multiple contexts in the complex cognitive aging literature,in particular to the context of challenges in the maintenance ofgoal-directed learning.

The impact of sleep on the formation and consolidation of spatial surveyknowledge

A supporting effect of sleep on memory consolidation was reported for different contents. Here, we investigated theinfluence of sleep on the transformation of previously learned route and place knowledge into survey knowledge, a more abstractrepresentation. The results support the assumption of both a consolidating as well as transforming effect: the wayfindingperformance in the test session - namely the usage of unfamiliar shortcuts - suggests a consolidating effect of sleep.

The roles of item repetition and position in infant sequence learning

We examined mechanisms underlying infants’ ability to detect, extract, and generalize sequential patterns, focusing on how saliency and consistency of distributional information guide infant learning of the most “likely” pattern in audiovisual sequences. In Experiment 1, we asked if 11- and 14-month-old infants could learn a “repetition anywhere” rule (e.g., ABBC, AABC, ABCC). In Experiment 2 we asked if 11- and 14-month-olds could generalize a “medial repetition” rule when its position is consistent in sequence, and in Experiment 3 we asked if 11-month-olds could identify a nonadjacent dependency occurring at edge positions. Infants were first habituated to 4-item sequences (shapes + syllables) containing repetition- and/or position-based structure, and were then tested with “familiar” structure instantiated across new items or combinations of items vs. “novel” (random) sequences. We found that 11-month-olds failed to learn the repetition rule both when the structure appeared in initial, medial, or final position (Experiment 1) and when it was restricted to the medial position (Experiment 2). Fourteen- month-olds learned repetition rules under both conditions. Finally, in Experiment 3 11-month-olds succeeded in learning a nonadjacent dependency in sequences identical to those used to test repetition learning in Experiment 2. Our results suggest that infants at 11 months, like adults, are relatively insensitive to patterns in the middle of sequences.

Silent gesture and noun phrase universals

In this paper we investigate a hypothesized cognitive bias forisomorphic mappings between conceptual structure and linearorder in the noun phrase. This bias has been proposed as a pos-sible explanation for a striking asymmetry in the typology ofthe noun phrase–linear orders which place the adjective clos-est to the noun, then the numeral, then the demonstrative, areover-represented in the world’s languages. Previous experi-mental work has provided evidence that an isomorphism biasaffects English-speaking learners’ inferences about the relativeorder of modifiers in an artificial language. Here, we use thesilent gesture paradigm to explore whether the isomorphismbias influences spontaneous gestures innovated by participantsin a modality with which they have relatively little prior experi-ence. We find that gesture string order largely conforms to thesame striking pattern found in noun phrase typology, support-ing the role of the isomorphism bias in shaping the emergenceof language (and language-like) systems.

Discourse continuity promotes children’s learning of new objects labels

The present study examined the influence of continuity ofreference (i.e., discourse continuity) on children’s learning ofnew objects labels. Four-year-old children were taught threenew label/objects pairs, where the speaker’s references toobjects were either continuous (i.e., clusters of utterancesreferred to the same object) or discontinuous (i.e., no twosequential sentences referred to the same object). In twoexperiments, children learned new word/object mappingsmore successfully when object labels were accompanied bycontinuous references to the same object. This researchreveals how discourse cues support children’s encoding ofnew words, and in doing so, advances our understanding ofthe specific features of parents’ language input that facilitatechildren’s language development.

The Facilitatory Effect of Referent Gaze on Cognitive Load in Language Processing

This paper considers prediction in language processing by ex-amining the role of the visual context, and specifically, the roleof speaker referent gaze on cognitive load. We inspect the an-ticipatory visual attention during sentence processing togetherwith the cognitive load induced at the points of the gaze cue,and the linguistic referent. Employing a novel measurementof cognitive load - the Index of Cognitive Activity (Marshall,2000) allowed us to simultaneously consider both anticipatoryeye-movements and cognitive load. Our results show that thegaze cue is being followed, and considered as a relevant pieceof information, which subsequently reduces the cognitive loadon the linguistic referent. In addition, we found that consider-ing the gaze cue is in itself not costly, unless it cues an objectmismatching with the previous linguistic context.

Gestural Hesitation Reveals Children’s Competence on MultimodalCommunication: Emergence of Disguised Adaptor

Speakers sometimes modify their gestures during the processof production into adaptors such as hair touching or eyescratching. Such disguised adaptors are evidence that thespeaker can monitor their gestures. In this study, weinvestigated when and how disguised adaptors are firstproduced by children. Sixty elementary school childrenparticipated in this study. There were ten from each schoolyear (from 7 to 12 years of age). They were instructed toremember a cartoon and retell its story to their parents. Theresults showed that children did not produce disguisedadaptors until the age of 8. The disguised adaptorsaccompany fluent speech until the children are 10 years oldand accompany dysfluent speech until they reach 11 or 12years of age. These results suggest that children start tomonitor their gestures when they are 9 or 10 years old.Cultural influences and cognitive changes were considered asfactors to influence emergence of disguised adaptors.

Adaptability and Neural Reuse in Minimally Cognitive Agents

Cognitive agents are continuously faced with new problems.To facilitate adaptation, emerging theories of neural reuse pro-pose that evolution might often favor re-purposing existingbrain structures for new functions. This paper presents a novelapproach to the study of neural reuse based on the evolutionof simulated agents in an object-categorization task. We arti-ficially evolve populations of dynamic neural networks to per-form two variants of a categorization task that alternate overevolutionary time. We find that populations become increas-ingly adaptive over repeated exposures to the tasks. Analysisof evolved networks reveals two types of equally-fit solutions:one that is specialized to a given task variant and does not adaptto changes easily; and another that is more general, in that itcan adapt to the other task with minimal change to its structure.Interestingly, we find that populations exposed to alternatingtasks spontaneously locate the latter type of structures.

The role of talker similarity in the perceptual learning of L2 tone categories

Different hypotheses were proposed concerning the role oftalker variability in lexical learning. It remains unclearwhether new phonetic categories are acquired as episodicmemory traces with talkers’ voice information preserved or asabstract categories. The current study investigated the role ofvoice similarity in perceptual learning of Cantonese tones. Sixhigh-variability training sessions were given to 12 Mandarinspeakers. Voice similarity was controlled in the training andpre-and posttests. Results indicate that the training positivelytransferred to both similar and dissimilar talkers. However, inthe pretest, the performance was not significantly differentbetween similar and dissimilar voices, whereas significantbetter performance was found in the similar voices in theposttest. These results suggest that learners retained speakers’information in the learning process and made use of suchinformation for future perception. This implies that lexicaltones are probably encoded episodically in the mentalrepresentation of Mandarin L2 learners.

A Spiking Neural Bayesian Model of Life Span Inference

In this paper, we present a spiking neural model of life spaninference. Through this model, we explore the biologicalplausibility of performing Bayesian computations in the brain.Specifically, we address the issue of representing probabil-ity distributions using neural circuits and combining them inmeaningful ways to perform inference. We show that applyingthese methods to the life span inference task matches humanperformance on this task better than an ideal Bayesian modeldue to the use of neuron tuning curves. We also describe po-tential ways in which humans might be generating the priorsneeded for this inference. This provides an initial step towardsbetter understanding how Bayesian computations may be im-plemented in a biologically plausible neural network.

The Role of Causality in Temporal Binding: Evidence for an Intentional Boost

Temporal binding refers to the subjective contraction in timebetween an action and its consequence. Since it was reportedin 2002 the effect has generated much interest, although aconsensus regarding the mechanisms behind it remainselusive. While multiple theoretical accounts have beenproposed, a key point of contention remains whether theeffect is the result of the perception of intentionality orcausality. We deployed a new apparatus to compareintentional to mechanical causation. Thirty participantsreported the interval between two events in self-causal,mechanical-causal and non-causal conditions. The results of aBayesian analysis pointed to smaller temporal estimates in theself-causal condition compared with the mechanical-causalcondition, in addition to smaller estimates in the mechanical-causal condition compared with the non-causal condition. Theevidence presented here suggests that causality alone may besufficient for temporal binding to occur, but that this effect isboosted by the presence of intentional action.

Familiarity-matching in decision making: Experimental studies on cognitive processes and analyses of its ecological rationality

Previous studies have shown that individuals often make inferences based on heuristics using recognition, fluency, or familiarity. In the present study, we propose a new heuristic called familiarity-matching, which predicts that when a decision maker is familiar (or unfamiliar) with an object in a question sentence, s/he will choose the more (or less) familiar object from the two alternatives. We examined inference processes and ecological rationality regarding familiarity-matching through three studies including behavioral experiments and ecological analyses. Results showed that participants often used familiarity- matching in solving difficult binary choice problems, and that familiarity-matching could be applied in an ecologically rational manner in real-world situations. A new perspective on human cognitive processes is discussed in this study.

Bridging a Conceptual Divide: How Peer Collaboration Facilitates Science Learning

Collaboration is generally an effective means of learning new information, but is collaboration productive in domains where collaborators may hold qualitatively different conceptions of the domain’s causal structure? We explored this question in the domain of evolutionary biology, where previous research has shown that most individuals construe evolution as the uniform transformation of an entire population (akin to metamorphosis) rather than the selective survival and reproduction of a subset of the population. College undergraduates (n = 44) completed an assessment of their evolutionary reasoning by themselves (pretest), with a partner (dyad test), and several weeks later (posttest). Collaboration proved ineffective for the higher- scoring partner in each dyad, as their scores generally remained unchanged from pretest to dyad test to posttest, but it proved effective for the lower-scoring partner. Not only did lower- scoring partners increase their score from pretest to dyad test, but they maintained higher scores at posttest as well. Follow- up analyses revealed that participants’ posttest scores were predicted by their partners’ pretest scores but only for lower- scoring partners, and the relation was negative: the smaller the difference between pretest score, the greater the gain from pretest to posttest for lower-scoring partners. These findings indicate that collaboration in domains characterized by conceptual change is possible, but that learning from such collaboration is asymmetric (i.e., individuals with low levels of understanding benefit more than their partners do) and unequal (i.e., individuals with low levels of understanding benefit more if their partner’s understanding is only moderately higher). Thus, bridging the gap between a novice’s view of a conceptually complex domain and an expert’s view appears to require instruction more aligned with the former than the latter.

A Data Driven Approach for Making Analogies

Making analogies is an important way for people to explain and understand new concepts. Though making analogies is natural for human beings, it is not a trivial task for a dia- logue agent. Making analogies requires the agent to estab- lish a correspondence between concepts in two different domains. In this work, we explore a data-driven approach for making analogies automatically. Our proposed approach works with data represented as a flat graphical structure, which can either be designed manually or extracted from In- ternet data. For a given concept from the base domain, our analogy agent can automatically suggest a corresponding concept from the target domain, and a set of mappings be- tween the relationships each concept has as supporting evi- dence. We demonstrate the working of this algorithm by both reproducing a classical example of analogy inference and making analogies in new domains generated from DBPedia data.

Word-object associations are non-selective in infants and young children

For decades, theories of early word learning have assumedthat infants are equipped with learning biases that help themlearn words at a fast pace. One of these biases, called MutualExclusivity, suggests that infants reject second labels forname-known objects. Our first two experiments, with childrenand with infants, suggest that novelty preference duringMutual Exclusivity tasks should not be taken as evidence thatassociations between novel labels and name-known objectshave not taken place. A third experiment, supplemented withcomputational modeling, ruled out cascaded activationpatterns as alternative explanations and, instead, confirmedthat word-object associations are non-selective throughoutinfancy and childhood.

The tortoise wins only when the race is long: How the task environment changesthe behavior of Tetris models

Tetris can be viewed as a highly complex decision making task, and used as a paradigm for studying human expertise.We hypothesized that models capable of playing Tetris for a long time are doing so by adopting slow but steady strategies toaccumulate points, while human players are much more prone to using high-risk, high-reward strategies that earn more pointsin a shorter time frame. This work used the MindModeling.org computational cognitive modeling platform to develop the bestmodels capable of playing long term games and short term games, and then compared the performance of the two. The bestlong term model adopted the slow and steady strategy, while the best short term model displayed the higher-risk, higher-rewardstrategy that more closely matches behavior observed in human players. Models that ”trained long” but ”played short” didworse than those that both trained and played ”short.”

Information density of encodings: The role of syntactic variation in comprehension

The Uniform Information Density (UID) hypothesis linksproduction strategies with comprehension processes,predicting that speakers will utilize flexibility in encoding inorder to increase uniformity in the rate of informationtransmission, as measured by surprisal (Jaeger, 2010).Evidence in support of UID comes primarily from studiesfocusing on word-level effects, e.g. demonstrating thatsurprisal predicts the omission/inclusion of optional words.Here we investigate whether comprehenders are sensitive tothe information density of alternative encodings that are moresyntactically complex. We manipulated the syntacticencoding of complex noun phrases in German via meaning-preserving pre-nominal and post-nominal modification incontexts that were either predictive or non-predictive. Wethen used the G-maze reading task to measure onlinecomprehension during self-paced reading. Results wereconsistent with the UID hypothesis. In predictive contexts,post-nominal encodings elicited a more uniform distributionof processing effort. Conversely, in non-predictive contexts,more uniform effort was found for pre-nominal encodings.

Bridging Visual Working Memory Research from Infancy through Adulthood with Dynamic Neural Field Modeling

Theories that span tasks and developmental periods require explaining how a single cognitive system can flexibly adapt across contexts yet show stable age-related improvement. Here we present a computational model that embodies a unified the- ory of visuospatial cognitive development. We use this model to bridge between previously disconnected domains, as diverse as infant habituation and visual working memory capacity in adults. We illustrate how the same real-time and developmental processes can account for behavior across tasks and age groups. We conclude with a discussion of the implications of a unified theory for understanding cognition and development more broadly, with an eye toward early intervention.

Pupil Dilation and Cognitive Reflection as Predictors of Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task

Risky decisions involve cognitive and emotional factors. As the primary test for the Somatic Marker Hypothesis (SMH), the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) examines these factors. Skin conductance shows anticipatory physiological responses on the IGT supporting SMH. Pupil dilation offers an alternative physiological marker. Predictive effects of anticipatory pupillary responses to positive and negative decks on IGT performance were examined in an extended IGT. The extended Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) examined the relationship between reflective thinking and IGT performance. Data demonstrated correlations between reflective thinking and performance from the second block onwards and that task learning continued into the additional blocks - performance was not optimized even in the final block. Regression analysis showed both anticipatory pupil dilation for disadvantageous and advantageous decks, and reflective thinking were strong predictors of IGT performance. While both emotional and reflective processes are implicated in IGT performance, analytic cognition is more important than traditionally acknowledged.

Talking Through Your Arse:Sensing Conversation with Seat Covers

People move in characteristic ways during conversation andthese movements correlate with their level of particpation.For example, speakers normally gesture significantly morethan listeners. These visible, overt movements are normallyanalysed using full body video or motion capture. Here weexplore the potential of a ’minimal’ approach to sensing theseparticipatory movements in part of the natural environmentof everyday interactions; chair seat covers. Using custombuilt fabric sensors we test whether we can detect people’sinvolvement in a conversation using only pressure changeson the seats they are sitting in. We show that even fromthis impoverished data we can distinguish between talking,backchanneling and laughter; each state is associated withdistinctive patterns of pressure change across the surfaceof the chair. We speculate on the possible applications ofthis new, unintrusive form of social sensing for architecture,performance and augmented human interaction.

Individual Differences in Spontaneous Analogical Problem-Solving: The Reflective Mind Account

Analogical problem-solving involves transfer of knowledge that has been obtained from a source analog and successfully applying it in the solution of a structurally similar target problem. What is usually found in the so-called hint/no-hint paradigm is that spontaneous solution to a problem is hard to achieve. This leaves the possibility for individual differences. This study searched for and found a positive correlation to exist between scores on the Cognitive Reflection Test and spontaneously solved analogical problems which, although a weak one, possibly accounts for the differences that exist between people who need a hint to solve an analogical problem, and people that do not need a hint.

20-month-olds Use Social-Group Membership to Make Inductive Inferences

Previous research suggests that preschool children expectmembers of social groups to share stable, inherentcharacteristics (e.g., Waxman, 2013). Here we explored theorigins of these social-group based inferences by examiningwhether infants generalize food preferences across membersof an arbitrary social group. Experiment 1 demonstrated thatinfants expected two individuals to share food preferenceswhen they belonged to the same social group, but not whenthey belonged to two different social groups. Experiment 2replicated and extended these findings to social groups thatwere labeled with adjectives instead of nouns. These resultssuggest that by 20 months of age, infants use social-groupmembership to make inductive inferences about the behaviorof group members.

Thinking inside the box:Motion prediction in contained spaces uses simulation

Theories of the mental processes people use to perform physi-cal reasoning often differ on whether they are based on simu-lation or on logical reasoning. Here we test how these differentprocesses might combine in a motion-prediction task that canbe solved either by simulation or by reasoning about the topol-ogy of the scene. Participants were asked to predict which oftwo goals a computerized ball would reach first, but in someof these scenes the ball was ‘contained’ in the same space asone goal but was topologically separated from the other. Evenin these contained scenes, participants responded faster whenthey received motion information that would speed up simu-lation but not affect topological parsing. This suggests thatsimulation contributes to predicting short-range motion, evenwhen alternate strategies are available.

Promoting Spontaneous Analogical Transfer: The Role of Category Status

Analogical comparison promotes spontaneous transfer by encouraging a more abstract representation that may be easier to retrieve. The category status hypothesis states that: if knowledge is represented as a relational category, it is easier to activate as a result of categorizing (as opposed to cue-based reminding). To investigate these two pathways to analogical transfer, participants were assigned to different study conditions: 1) standard comparison of two analogs; 2) standard comparison followed by a second comparison of two new analogs; or 3) a guided category-building task based on sequential summarization. Category-building showed a reliably higher rate of spontaneous transfer during an analogical problem solving task than standard comparison (numerically higher than double-comparison). Another experiment measured spontaneous remindings to cues on the basis of matching structure. Category-building showed a reliable advantage over both comparison conditions. This supports categorization as a novel pathway to spontaneous transfer by enhancing retrieval of structurally similar information.

Geometry-based Affordances

A representational approach to ecological psychology is presented. This paper identifies a computational-level commonality in ecological psychology research related to passability of apertures. It is argued that a cognitive mechanism capable of comparing the geometric properties of an environment and the geometric properties of the agent can be used to support judgments for action in space.

Acquiring pitch associations across modalities: the role of experience

When interpreting our perceptual world, information from multiple perceptual modalities is often associated. Suchcrossmodal associations can arise from innate structural connections in the brain, statistical correlations in the environment, orthrough language. In a large group of participants across a wide age range and language background, we tested crossmodalassociations between pitch and 7 dimensions in comparison modalities. We found evidence supporting the existence of all 7types of associations, but the strength of association varied by dimension. Pitch-angularity and pitch-weight judgments werethe most robust associations. In general, strength of associations increased with age, with significant associations occurring inthe oldest age group (age 19+), consistent with experiential accounts of crossmodal associations.

Theory of Mind and Valuation during Cooperation

Societal progress requires humans to excel at cooperation over time. To sustain successful cooperation, peoplecoordinate, especially about who is in the best position at any given moment to make the best decision or to take the best actionfor the team as a whole. We used a novel cooperation task between involving dynamic assignment of Teacher and a Learnerunder conditions of uncertainty both about reward and about who is the expert at any given time. The task is similar to Theoryof Mind tasks but actually gives the participants a stake in the outcome. We found evidence for effortful representation ofthe preferences of others, and that successful prediction fosters cooperative success. Neural components and putative sourcessignaled changes in the role of expert in the task. Further, the task design allows novel applications of computational models tothe cognitive dynamics and associated neural systems for cooperation.

Fake News and False Corroboration: Interactivity in Rumor Networks

Rumors inundate every social network. Some of them aretrue, but many of them are false. On rare occasions, a falserumor is exposed as the lie that it is. But more commonly,false rumors have a habit of obtaining apparent verification,by corroboration from what seems to be a second independentsource. However, in complex social networks, theconnectivity is such that a putative second source is almostnever actually independent of the original source. In thepresent work, rumor network simulations demonstrate howremarkably easy it is for a node in the network to be fooledinto thinking it has received independent verification of afalse rumor, when in fact that “second source” can be tracedback to the original source. By developing a theoreticalunderstanding of the circumstances under which the spread offalse rumors, “alternative facts,” and fake news can becontrolled, perhaps the field can help prevent them fromruining elections and ruining entire nations.

Adapting to a listener with incomplete lexical semantics

Speakers involved in a communicative exchange construct aninternal model of their addressees and draw upon the model tocraft utterances that are likely to be understood. In many real-world situations (e.g., when talking to a non-expert, non-nativespeaker, or a child), this process of audience design involvesidentifying gaps in the lexical-semantic knowledge of thelistener and selecting alternative expressions. We examinespeaker adaptation to a listener with incomplete lexicalknowledge in the spatial domain, specifically a failure tocomprehend the basic terms left/right. Experimental andmodeling results provide evidence of rapid adaptation that ismodulated by the availability of alternative spatial terms. Weconsider how our approach relates to recent work incomputational pragmatics, and suggest that adaptation to thelexical knowledge of the addressee is an important butrelatively understudied topic for future research.

Children’s use of lexical flexibility to structure new noun categories

Because most common words have multiple meanings,children are often learning new senses of existing words,rather than entirely new words. Here, we explore whetherchildren can use their knowledge of an existing word sense toconstrain their interpretation of a new word meaning. Acrosstwo studies, we teach 3- and 4-year-olds and adults novelwords for materials, and manipulate whether those words arealso used flexibly, to label objects made from those materials.We find that participants of all ages assign markedly differentinterpretations to the object labels when they have a prior,material meaning: Rather than extending them to otherobjects of similar shapes, they extend them on the basis ofshared material, thus overriding the well-documented shapebias. These findings suggest that language learners can use aword’s prior meaning to learn about the structure of its newmeaning.

Rationalizing subjective probability distortions

You cannot know the contents of a memory until after you have actually retrieved it. This paper considers theimplications of this straightforward observation upon the psychological process of preference construction. We show that thisconstraint renders observers with random access memory susceptible to tail risks. We show that this difficulty can be rectifiedby permitting observers to weight memory retrieval for such observations, that outcome utility cannot be used for this purpose,but information-theoretic surprise can serve as a useful proxy for it. Using two novel experiments, we present evidence insupport of our account. With the first, we show that humans find surprising experiences easier to remember. With the second,we show that surprising experiences in the past have a greater influence on future decisions than is statistically warranted. Thistwofold demonstration substantiates a psychologically plausible account for the origin of subjective probability distortions.

Memory of relative magnitude judgments informs absolute identification

The question of whether people store absolute magnitude information or relative local comparisons of magnitudeshas remained unanswered despite persistent efforts over the last three decades to resolve it. Absolute identification is one of themost rigorous experimental benchmarks for evaluating theories of magnitude representation. We characterize difficulties withboth absolute and relative accounts of magnitude representation and propose an alternative account that potentially resolvesthese difficulties. We postulate that people store neither long-term internal referents for stimuli, not binary comparisons ofsize between successive stimuli. Rather, they obtain probabilistic judgments of size differences between successive stimuliand encode these for future use, within the course of identification trials. We set up a Bayesian ideal observer model for theidentification task using this representation of magnitude and propose a memory-sampling based approximation for solving it.Simulations suggest that the model adequately captures human behavior patterns in absolute identification.

Exploration and Skill Acquisition in a Major Online Game

Using data from a major commercial online game, Des-tiny, we track the development of player skill across time.From over 20,000 player record we identify 3475 playerswho have played on 50 or more days. Our focus is onhow variability in elements of play affect subsequent skilldevelopment. After validating the persistent influence ofdifferences in initial performance between players, wetest how practice spacing, social play, play mode vari-ability and a direct measure of game-world explorationaffect learning rate. These latter two factors do not af-fect learning rate. Players who space their practice morelearn faster, in line with our expectations, whereas play-ers who coordinate more with other players learn slower,which contradicts our initial hypothesis. We conclude thatnot all forms of practice variety expedite skill acquisition.Online game telemetry is a rich domain for exploring the-ories of optimal skill acquisition.

Biases and labeling in iterative pragmatic reasoning

This paper presents a series of reference game experiments(Frank and Goodman, 2012) and fits the results to a numberof Bayesian computational models in order to explore the roleof linguistic and perceptual bias in iterative pragmatic reason-ing. We first discuss the modeling choices made by Franke andJ ̈ager (2016) and others who have used similar frameworks tomodel reference game tasks. We introduce a space of differentplausible Bayesian models based on this work, and comparemodels’ fit to new experimental data to replicate the basic find-ings of Franke and J ̈ager (2016) regarding the strong role forperceptual salience (e.g., the primacy of color over shape asa differentiating property for possible referents) and linguis-tic category (e.g., a preference for nouns over adjectives) inpragmatic reference resolution. We then uncover an additionalpossible effect of what we call labeling, whereby a hearer maysimply ignore non-salient, non-differentiating semantic prop-erties, in a manner similar to how an incremental algorithm(Reiter and Dale, 1992) might ignore certain semantic proper-ties when generating referring expressions.

Actions that Modify Schedules of Reinforcement

Many everyday activities involve the use of one action tomodify the effects of another: When driving, shiftinggears modifies the influence of pressing the gas pedal onacceleration; when cooking, the rate of adding aparticular ingredient modifies the influence of stirring onviscosity. Here, we investigate a general ability to learnhow to use actions to control schedules of reinforcement.In Experiment 1, participants quickly discovered theoptimal rate of responding on an action that controlledthe rate of reward contingent on performing a differentaction. In Experiment 2, when the modifying action wasitself rewarded, participants failed to discover the optimalrate. Implications for formal theories of instrumentalbehavior are discussed.

Shaping the Dynamics of Category Learning in Infants and Adults by Varying Learning Context

During the first year of life, infants develop a remarkable ability to group objects based on their similarities and differences. This ability of category formation represents one of the main mechanisms underlying the organisation of the semantic system. Early categories are formed spontaneously, in a non-supervised fashion and this type of category acquisition remains present even when more sophisticated forms of supervised category learning emerge. Even though there are various models of categorisation mechanisms across the lifespan, there is a gap in the research investigating implicit categorisation at different stages of cognitive development. Therefore, the aim of the current study was to compare processes of spontaneous concept formation in infants and adults using an experimental paradigm based on novelty preference. We discovered that both infants and adults show evidence of category learning (Experiment 1), though with different amount of training being needed to achieve the task. Adults successfully categorised objects already after a single block of training. Infants reached a level comparable to that of adults after twice the amount of training. As these tasks inevitably pose different cognitive and sensory demands to the two groups, in Experiments 2 and 3 we explored how varying parameters of the learning context affect dynamics of category formation. Decreasing memory demands of the task resulted in an acceleration of infants’ category formation (Experiment 2), whereas posing memory load in an implicit category learning task decelerated adults’ dynamics of category formation (Experiment 3).

Early produced signs are iconic: Evidence from Turkish Sign Language

Motivated form-meaning mappings are pervasive in signlanguages, and iconicity has recently been shown to facilitatesign learning from early on. This study investigated the role oficonicity for language acquisition in Turkish Sign Language(TID). Participants were 43 signing children (aged 10 to 45months) of deaf parents. Sign production ability was recordedusing the adapted version of MacArthur Bates CommunicativeDevelopmental Inventory (CDI) consisting of 500 items forTID. Iconicity and familiarity ratings for a subset of 104 signswere available. Our results revealed that the iconicity of a signwas positively correlated with the percentage of childrenproducing a sign and that iconicity significantly predicted thepercentage of children producing a sign, independent offamiliarity or phonological complexity. Our results areconsistent with previous findings on sign language acquisitionand provide further support for the facilitating effect of iconicform-meaning mappings in sign learning.

A Minimal Neural Network Model of The Gambler’s Fallacy

The gambler’s fallacy has been a notorious showcase of humanirrationality in probabilistic reasoning. Recent studies suggestthe neural basis of this fallacy might have originated from thepredictive learning by neuron populations over the latent tem-poral structures of random sequences, particularly due to thestatistics of pattern times and the precedence odds betweenpatterns. Here we present a biologically-motivated minimalneural network model with only eight neurons. Through unsu-pervised training, the model naturally develops a bias towardalternation patterns over repetition patterns, even when bothpatterns are equally likely presented to the model. Our analysessuggest that the way the neocortex integrates information overtime makes the neuron populations not only sensitive to thefrequency signals but also relational structures embedded overtime. Moreover, we offer an explanation for how higher-levelcognitive biases may have an early start at the level of sensoryprocessing.

Temporal variability in moral value judgement

Moral judgments are known to change in response tochanges in external conditions. But how variable aremoral judgments over time in the absence of environ-mental variation? The moral domain has been describedin terms of five moral foundations, categories that ap-pear to capture moral judgment across cultures. We ex-amined the temporal consistency of repeated responsesto the moral foundations questionnaire over short timeperiods, fitted a set of mixed effects models to the dataand compared them. We found correlations betweenchanges in participant responses for different founda-tions over time, suggesting a structure with at leasttwo underlying stochastic processes: one for moral judg-ments involving harm and fairness, and another formoral judgments related to loyalty, authority, and pu-rity.

Enforced pointing gesture can indicate invisible objects behind a wall

The pointing gesture is regarded as indicating an object or location in the environment. People sometimes point to invisible objects, but the inferential mechanism is not known. This study examined comprehension of pointing with a bent index finger at an invisible object behind a wall. The experimenter pointed at an object using either typical pointing or “enforced pointing” behind a wall that was either opaque or transparent. In enforced pointing, the experimenter moved his arm in an arc movement. The participants guessed which object was being denoted. The wall was also either relatively high or relatively low. When the participants looked at typical pointing, they thought that objects both in front of the wall and behind the wall were being denoted. However, when they looked at enforced pointing, they more frequently thought that objects behind the wall were denoted. People seemed to use pragmatic knowledge on this “enforced” pointing gesture.

Similarities Between Objects in Analogies Framed by Schema-Governed Categories

The present study was aimed at assessing the effect of object similarities on participants' evaluations of analogical quality. Results from an experimental condition in which the relations involved in the compared situations were explicitly highlighted, showed that general object similarities (membership to same category) positively affected the evaluations of analogical quality. In contrast, no such effect was found under another experimental condition in which the analogical comparisons between the same situations were framed by a schema-governed category. An analysis of participants' justifications revealed that the object similarities that were taken into account under this second condition were related to central dimensions of the schema-governed category that was used to frame the analogies. We explain these findings within the category assignment approach developed by Minervino et al., and discuss the implications of this alternative perspective of analogical reasoning for the role of similarities between entities playing several thematic roles.

What can Hand Movements Tell us about Audience Engagement?

Conventional seated audiences have relatively restricted op-portunities for response. Perhaps the most salient is applausebut they use their hands to make other visible movements: tofix hair, adjust glasses, scratch ears. The question we addresshere is whether these apparently incidental movements mayprovide systematic clues about an audience’s level of engage-ment with a performance. We investigate this in the contextof contemporary dance performances by analysing audiencehand movements in four performances at the London Contem-porary Dance School. Hand movements were tracked using areflective wristband worn by each audience member. A blobdetection algorithm applied to the video recording examinedwhether changes in hand movement are associated with audi-ence arousal levels to the performance. The results show thathands move least during the most preferred and most duringthe least preferred dance pieces. We conclude that still handsare a signal of higher levels of engagement.

Generalizing relations during analogical problem solving in preschool children: does blocked or interleaved training improve performance?

Analogical reasoning, the mapping of structured relations across conceptual domains, is commonly recognized as essential to human cognition, but young children often perform poorly in the classical A:B::C:? analogical reasoning task. Particularly, young children have trouble when the objects in the task are not strongly associated with each other, and/or when there are strong associative lures among the potential answers. Here, we examine whether successive trials that repeat the same relation needed to solve the analogy can help overcome some of the challenges with weakly associated items. In the first of two experiments, our results were mixed. In the second, we simplified the design, and were able to more clearly show a benefit of repeating relations across consecutively solved problems.

Generalizing novel names in comparison settings: Role of conceptual distance during learning and at test

In a comparison setting (two stimuli), we tested 4- and 6-year- old children’s generalization of novel names for objects. We manipulated the semantic distance between the two learning items (e.g., two bracelets versus a bracelet and a watch), and the semantic distance between the learning items and the test items (e.g., a pendant versus a bow tie). We tested whether smaller semantic distance between learning items would lead to more taxonomic (vs. perceptual) choices at test, than broader semantic distance during learning, especially in the case of distant test stimuli. Results revealed main effects of learning distance, of generalization distance and that only children aged 6 years benefited from broader semantic during learning at test. Four year-old children failed to generalize to far test stimuli even with semantically distant learning items. We discuss how conceptual distance during learning differentially affects generalization performance across age groups.

Solving the Puzzle to Reach the Summit:Using Metaphor to Gauge Public Perceptions of Science

Skepticism towards science has risen sharply in recent years.Cognitive scientists can help address this issue byilluminating how people conceptualize the scientific process,paving the way for improved communication with the public.We recruited a large sample of lay Americans, as well asacademics in the sciences and humanities, to answer a seriesof questions assessing their views about science. Becausemetaphors have been identified as useful tools forcommunicating about complex domains, we askedparticipants to choose which of two metaphors––working on apuzzle or scaling a mountain––best captured their beliefsabout the scientific process. Results revealed substantialvariation in perceptions of science across groups, and wehighlight the ways in which scientists seem to conceptualizescience differently from non-scientists. Importantly, metaphorpreference was associated with particular patterns of thinking,though not always in our originally hypothesized direction.We discuss the implications of these findings.

Metaphors, Roles, and Controls in Framing Studies

Metaphors have been shown to be effective explanatory andcommunicative tools, shaping how people think and reasonabout complex domains. To date, however, most studies haveaddressed only coarse-grained effects of metaphor framing,leaving many questions unanswered about the relative powerof metaphor compared to more literal linguistic framingdevices. We addressed this issue in a large, pre-registeredframing study, comparing the effects of describing the role ofpolice officers as (a) metaphorical guardians of a community(b) literal protectors of a community, and (c) a no-labelcontrol. We found no main effect of framing condition,suggesting that positively valenced metaphors may exert littleinfluence on their own in this domain. However, we didobserve an interaction between condition and politicalideology, such that the guardian metaphor was especiallyeffective at improving attitudes towards police officers forliberals, whose initial approval ratings were relatively low.

Part-whole categorization is culture-specific

We present two experiments on the role of culture in the categorization of object part-whole structures. A triadic categorization task pitted shape against function as factors driving similarity judgments on selected parts of different types of objects. Speakers of American English were significantly more likely than speakers of two indigenous languages of Mexico, Tseltal Maya and Isthmus Zapotec, to choose categorization by function, even when familiarity of the various stimulus objects was factored in. In the second study, members of the two indigenous groups matched parts of a doll to parts of novel objects of unfamiliar shape. The Tseltal participants were significantly more likely to match according to a shape-analytical algorithm rather than global analogy, consistent with predictions based on prevalent strategies in verbal part labeling in the two languages. We conclude that while cognition of object parts undoubtedly has a strong biological basis, there are also robust cultural effects.

The Stroop Effect From a Mixture of Reading Processes: AFixed-Point Analysis

For the last 80 years, the Stroop task has been used totest theories of attention and cognitive control and ithas been applied in many clinical settings. Most theo-ries posit that the overwhelming power of written wordsovercomes strict instructions to focus on print color andignore the word. Recent evidence suggests that trials inthe Stroop task could in fact be a mixture of readingtrials and non-reading trials. Here we conduct a criticaltest of this mixture hypothesis, where a mixture of pro-cesses should satisfy the fixed-point property (Falmagne,1968).

Interleaving area problems in the 4 th grade classroom:What is the role of context and practice?

Typical mathematics instruction involves blocked practiceacross a set of conceptually similar problems. Interleaving, orpractice across a set of conceptually dissimilar problems,improves learning and transfer by repeatedly reloadinginformation and increasing discrimination of problemfeatures. Similarly, comparing problems across differentcontexts highlights relevant and irrelevant knowledge. Ourexperiment is the first to investigate the relative effects ofinterleaving geometry problems and interleaving contexts.Thirty-three fourth-grade students received the same practiceproblems but were randomly assigned to one of threeconditions: interleaved by math skill, interleaved by context,and interleaved by math skill and by context (i.e., hyper-interleaved). Afterward, each participant was exposed to testsassessing declarative and procedural knowledge. The resultssuggest that interleaving math skill within varying contextsenhances the acquisition of mathematical procedures.

Scarcity impairs online detection and prospective memory

Operating under limited resources poses significant demands on the cognitive system. Here we demonstrate that people under time scarcity failed to detect time-saving cues as they occur in the environment (Experiment 1a). These time-saving cues, if noticed, would have saved time for the time-poor participants. Moreover, the visuospatial proximity of the time-saving cues to the focal task determined successful detection, suggesting that scarcity altered the spatial scope of attention (Experiment 1b & 1c). People under time scarcity were also more likely to forget previous instructions to execute future actions (Experiment 2). These instructions, if remembered and followed, would have saved time for the time-poor participants. Failures of online detection and prospective memory are problematic because they cause neglect and forgetting of beneficial information, perpetuating the condition of scarcity. The current study provides a new cognitive account for the counterproductive behaviors in the poor, and relevant implications for interventions.

Specificity and entropy reduction in situated referential processing

In situated communication, reference to an entity in theshared visual context can be established using either anexpression that conveys precise (minimally specified) orredundant (over-specified) information. There is, however, along-lasting debate in psycholinguistics concerning whetherthe latter hinders referential processing. We present evidencefrom an eye tracking experiment recording fixations as wellas the Index of Cognitive Activity – a novel measure ofcognitive workload – supporting the view that over-specifications facilitate processing. We further presentoriginal evidence that, above and beyond the effect ofspecificity, referring expressions that uniformly reducereferential entropy also benefit processing.

A Cognitive Model of Social Influence

We describe two different cognitive process models of a wellknown experiment on social influence (Salganik, Dodds, &Watts, 2006). One model, the social influence model,reproduced the choices that participants took by modelingboth the cognitive processes the participant engaged in andthe social influences that the participant saw. The secondmodel, the pure cognitive model, used only cognitivecapabilities and did not model any social influences that theparticipant saw. Somewhat surprisingly, the two modelsshowed no difference in quality of fit (the pure cognitivemodel actually fit slightly better than the social influencemodel), suggesting that social influence models should takecognitive functions into account in their theories.

Why Teach How Things Work?Tracking the Evolution of Children’s Intuitions about Complexity

Mechanistic information can be characterized as theinteracting causal components underlying a phenomenon - inshort, how something works. Children and adults arenotoriously poor at learning, remembering, and applyingmechanistic information, so it comes as no surprise that thewisdom of teaching mechanism has come under increasingscrutiny in science education. However, while a rich memoryfor mechanistic details may be out of the average student’sgrasp, we argue that exposure to mechanism does not leavestudents empty-handed. Instead, it refines their intuitionsabout science and the world in significant ways. For thecurrent study, we focused on one kind of intuition inparticular: beliefs about causal complexity. Children ages 6-11 rated the complexity of a heart and a lock and were thengiven either mechanistic or non-mechanistic informationabout them. Afterwards, they were asked if their intuitionsabout complexity had changed and if so by how much. Threeweeks later, children were asked again about their intuitionsabout complexity. Crucially, children who were givenmechanistic information demonstrated a significantly greatershift in their assessments of complexity for both the heart anddoor lock compared to their counterparts who were givennon-mechanistic information. This contradicts the notion thatmechanism provides learners with few benefits while alsodemonstrating how mechanism can be a powerful force inshaping children’s intuitions.

The Impact of Presentation Order on the Attraction Effect in Decision-making

The attraction effect in decision-making is a famous exampleof how preferences are influenced by the availability of otheroptions. One emerging hypothesis for the effect is that biasesin attention influence preferences. In the past, these ideashave been explored indirectly through computationalmodeling and eye tracking. In the present paper, we directlymanipulate attention through presentation order, presentingchoice options sequentially. Our results show thatpresentation order has a large impact on the effect – somepresentation orders enhance the effect and other ordersreverse the effect. To understand these results, we fit adynamic model, called the Multiattribute Linear BallisticAccumulator model, to the choice and response time data.Modeling results reveal that presentation order influences theallocation of attention on the positive and negative differencesbetween options. In sum, our results show that attention has adirect impact on the attraction effect.

Interpretation and Processing Time of Generalized Quantifiers: Why your Mental Space Matters

Classical quantifiers (e.g., “all”, “some” and “none”) have been extensively studied in logic and psychology. In contrast, generalized quantifiers (e.g., “most”) allow for fine-grained statements about quantities. The discrepancy in the underlying mental representation and its interpretation among interpreters can affect language use and reasoning. We investigated the effect of quantifier type, quantification space (set size) and monotonicity on processing difficulty (in response time, RT) and response diversity of 77 generalized quantifiers. Shannon entropy was employed to measure response diversity. Our findings indicate: (i) Set size is a significant factor of response diversity, which implies that the underlying space is relevant for the interpretation. (ii) Quantifiers possess a rather static underlying representation within and across tasks within a participant. (iii) Quantifier type and monotonicity can affect response diversity; while the response diversity can predict RT. (iv) In reasoning, the number of generalized quantifiers versus classical quantifiers in a syllogism is a factor of re- sponse diversity. Diversity in the interpretation of generalized quantifiers may be a cause of human’s deviation from logical responses.

Right hemisphere lateralization and holistic processing do not always go together:An ERP investigation of a training study

Holistic processing (HP) and right-hemispheric lateralizationboth mark expertise in visual object recognition such as faceand sub-ordinate object perception. However, counter-examples have been found recently: Experiences of selectiveattention to parts such as writing experiences in Chinesecharacters reduced HP while increased right hemispherelateralization. We investigated the association between HPand brain activities measured by event-related potentials(ERP) in participants trained to recognize artificially-createdscripts using either whole-word or grapheme-to-phonemeapproaches. Stronger N170 activities were found in bothhemispheres in both training approaches. Though the type oftraining approaches induced opposite directions incorrelations between HP and the ERP signals in the righthemisphere: In the whole-word condition, the HP effectincreased with stronger right-hemispheric N170 activities;while the direction of this correlation was reversed in thegrapheme-to-phoneme condition. This demonstrates that HPand right hemispheric lateralization are separate processesthat are associated with different perceptual mechanisms.

He’s pregnant": simulating the confusing case of gender pronoun errors in L2English

Even advanced Spanish speakers of second language Englishtend to confuse the pronouns ‘he’ and ‘she’, often withouteven noticing their mistake (Lahoz, 1991). A study by Antón-Méndez (2010) has indicated that a possible reason for this er-ror is the fact that Spanish is a pro-drop language. In order totest this hypothesis, we used an extension of Dual-path (Chang,2002), a computational cognitive model of sentence produc-tion, to simulate two models of bilingual speech production ofsecond language English. One model had Spanish (ES) as anative language, whereas the other learned a Spanish-like lan-guage that used the pronoun at all times (non-pro-drop Span-ish, NPD_ES). When tested on L2 English sentences, the bilin-gual pro-drop Spanish model produced significantly more gen-der pronoun errors, confirming that pronoun dropping couldindeed be responsible for the gender confusion in natural lan-guage use as well.

Deconstructing Transitional Probabilities: Bigram Frequency and Diversity in Lexical Decision

Statistical learning paradigms traditionally use transitional probabilities as a measure of statistical distribution within a language. The current study suggests that alternative metrics may exist that can account for differences in language processing ability. Two primed lexical decision tasks are used to examine the effects of bigram frequency and diversity on speed and accuracy of word recognition. It is demonstrated that both frequency and diversity contribute to word recognition performance; findings and theoretical implications are discussed.

Extraneous visual noise facilitates word learning

Variability is important to learning; however, whether itsupports or hinders language acquisition is unclear. 3D objectstudies suggest that children learn words better when targetobjects vary, however storybook studies indicate thatcontextual variability impairs learning. We tested a dynamicsystems account in which background variability should boostlearning by speeding the emergence of new behaviors. Twogroups of two-year-old children saw arrays of one novel andtwo known objects on a screen, and heard a novel or knownlabel. Stimuli were identical across conditions, with theexception that in the constant condition objects appeared on awhite background, and in the variable condition backgroundswere colored. Only children in the variable condition showedevidence of word learning, suggesting that extraneousvariability supports learning by decontextualizingrepresentations, and indicating that adding low-level entropyto the developmental system can trigger a change in behavior.

Comparison strategies in the change detection task are influenced by taskdemands.

Current models of visual working memory (VWM) assume that comparing memory with the environment obligato-rily involves a spatial comparison process. Can changing task demands determine whether a spatial or non-spatial comparisonprocesses is employed? Study displays of three colored shapes were presented, followed by test displays of three colouredshapes. Participants decided whether a feature changed between displays. Task-irrelevant changes to the probed item’s lo-cations or feature bindings reduced memory performance, suggesting that participants employed spatially guided comparisonprocess. This finding occurred irrespective of whether participants decided about the whole display, or only a single cueditem within the display. When task-irrelevant feature changes occurred amongst uncued items, performance was unaffected byirrelevant changes in location or feature bindings. These results suggest that participants can flexibly shift comparison strat-egy in response to changing task demands. These findings have implications for models of VWM, which assume obligatorylocation-based comparisons in VWM.

Weight matters: The role of physical weight in non-physical language across ageand culture

Languages commonly use physical properties to discuss dis-tinctly non-physical states and events in the world (e.g., “I’mnot a huge fan of licorice”). Here, we investigate the degreeto which associations between physical properties and abstractconcepts are culturally specific constructs. To do this, wetested three distinct populations—US adults, US children, andadults from an indigenous group in the lowlands of Bolivia, theTsimane’—on their associations between the physical conceptof weight and a variety of abstract attributes (e.g., importance,emotional state, moral worth). We find a strong relationshipbetween the associations of US and Tsimane’ adults, but little-to-no relationship between US children and either adult popu-lation. These results suggest that the property of weight playsa similar role in everyday thought across cultures, but that ittakes time to develop. Further, we found that these associationscould not be recovered from a simple semantic embeddinganalysis, suggesting that the cross-culturally shared connec-tions between physical and abstract attributes may be learnedthrough more complex experiences than language alone.

Learning in the Wild: Real-World Experiences Shape Children’s Knowledge Organization

The organization of knowledge according to relations between concepts is critically involved in many cognitive processes, including memory and reasoning. However, the role of learning in shaping knowledge organization has received little direct investigation. Therefore, the present study investigated whether informal learning experiences can drive rapid, substantial changes in knowledge organization in children by measuring the effects of a week-long Zoo summer camp versus a control camp on the degree to which 4- to 9-year-old children’s knowledge about animals was organized according to taxonomic relations. Although taxonomic organization did not differ at pre-test, only Zoo camp children showed increases in taxonomic organization at post-test. These findings provide novel evidence that informal, real-life learning experiences can drive rapid knowledge organization change.

The Effects of Social Task Setting on Time Perception

This study investigates the effects of the social setting on prospective time estimation, how time is perceived when a task is performed (i) alone, (ii) with a collaborative, or (iii) with a competitive partner. N=90 participants were tested (30 in each condition). Participants performed a concurrent Simon task for three different durations (15, 30 and 45 seconds) which was followed by a time reproduction phase. Results revealed a main effect of social condition. Reproduction ratios in dual conditions were smaller than in the single condition and also smaller in the competitive condition compared to the cooperative condition. The results provide first evidence that social condition affects time estimation: time “flies” when we work together, in particular when we compete with a partner, showing that cognitive and social processes are heavily intertwined.

Instruction type and believability influence on metareasoning in a base rate task

Task dependent conflict has been shown to reduce metacognitive judgements of confidence and prolong response times in various reasoning tasks. For this study a modified version of the base rate task was used to induce conflict while measuring response times and judgements of confidence. The aim of this experiment was to determine the influence of different instruction conditions (reasoning according to belief or according to mathematical probability) on fluency and metacognitive judgements. As expected, participants experienced higher levels of conflict when reasoning according to mathematical probability even though conflict effects were present in both conditions. Additionally, higher believability items mitigated conflict influence while reasoning in accordance with belief and increased it when reasoning in accordance with mathematical probability. These results enrich the growing field of metareasoning research and are discussed as such.

Text, images and diagrams as information providers

We studied the effect of adjunct displays on recall in an expository text (based on McCrudden, Schraw, Lehman, & Poliquin, 2007) in order to find out which means of display aided pupils in the last years of secondary school to recall information. We included four conditions in the experiment: text only, text and causal diagram, text and images and causal diagram only. Participants were checked for their recall of main ideas and causal sequences. Recall for main ideas did not vary significantly across conditions. Contrary to McCrudden et al. (2007), our results for the causal sequences revealed that participants who studied a causal diagram only could recall more steps from causal sequences than participants in any of the other conditions. We will interpret the findings in the light of the literature on redundancy effects, dual coding theory, and the causal explication hypothesis.

The Role of Linguistic Information in Learning Abstract Words:Evidence from Children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI)

Accounts of abstract word learning suggest that learning thesewords relies primarily on access to linguistic cues, such as thestatistical co-occurrence of words with similar semanticproperties. Thus, children with language impairment (LI),who by definition have impoverished access to linguisticcontext, should have disproportionate impairments in abstractword knowledge. Here, we compared verbal definitions andlexical decisions to both abstract and concrete words ofchildren with LI (ages 8 to 13) and both age-matched andvocabulary-matched typically developing (TD) peers. Relativeto age-matched peers, children with LI had significant deficitsin both tasks. Crucially, however, there was not greaterimpairment of abstract words. We conclude that that linguisticknowledge is not a sine qua non to learning abstract wordsand concepts and other mechanisms, which are notspecifically impaired in LI, are at play.

Facial Motor Information is Sufficient for Identity Recognition

The face is a central communication channel providing infor-mation about the identities of our interaction partners and theirpotential mental states expressed by motor configurations. Al-though it is well known that infants ability to recognise peoplefollows a developmental process, it is still an open questionhow face identity recognition skills can develop and, in par-ticular, how facial expression and identity processing poten-tially interact during this developmental process. We proposethat by acquiring information of the facial motor configurationobserved from face stimuli encountered throughout develop-ment would be sufficient to develop a face-space representa-tion. This representation encodes the observed face stimuli aspoints of a multidimensional psychological space able to as-sist facial identity and expression recognition. We validate ourhypothesis through computational simulations and we suggestpotential implications of this understanding with respect to theavailable findings in face processing.

Forgetting My Memories by Listening to Yours: The Impact of Perspective-Takingon Socially-Triggered Context-Based Prediction Error

The mind is a prediction machine. In most situations in which it finds itself, it has expectations as to what mighthappen. But when people’s expectations are invalidated by experience, the memories that gave rise to these expectations aresuppressed. The present research explores the effect of these prediction errors on listener’s memories during social interaction.We reasoned that listening to a speaker recounting experiences similar to one’s own would trigger prediction errors on the part ofthe listener that would result in the suppression of his/her memories. Study 1 offers evidence for the effect of socially triggeredcontext based prediction errors on listener’s mnemonic representations. Study 2 replicates these findings and shows that thiseffect is sensitive to a perspective-taking manipulation. Taken together, these findings provide evidence for a yet unrecognizedphenomenon by which our conversations shape the memories that we come to hold.

Rank Aggregation and Belief Revision Dynamics

In this paper we compare several popular rank aggregationmethods by accuracy of finding the true (correct) ranked list.Our research reveals that under most common circumstancessimple methods such as the average or majority actually tendto outperform computationally-intensive distance-based meth-ods. We then conduct a study to compare how actual peopleaggregate ranks in a group setting. Our finding is that individ-uals tend to adopt the group mean in a third of all revisions,making it the most popular strategy for belief revision.

Overcoming the Tragedy of Personnel Evaluation?

Human beings are essentially – by nature or second nature - members of groups. They contribute to these groups not just as isolated individuals but also through their interaction with others. Consequently, personnel evaluation in companies and organizations requires assessing not only evaluating indivi- dual performance but also the overall direct and indirect effect one has on a team. Others’ work may be improved or ham- pered by the presence of a particular employee. We investi- gate Two-level Personnel-Evaluation Tasks (T-PETs) with information on individual and group earnings, where an individual focus may lead to evaluate the overall best employee as being the worst. We have previously found a Tragedy of Personnel Evaluation where focus on direct individual impact did have such systematic effect. In two experiments, one on team size, the other on kinds of information provided, we explore the boundary conditions of this effect and suggest how it may be overcome.

Altruist vs Egoist Detection and Individual vs Group Selection in Personnel Management

In the Wason-Selection Task debate it has been suggested that people may be able to detect cheaters but not co-operators or altruists. This position has been challenged. Here we focus on a scenario that is more ecologically valid with regard to different strategies for detecting workers who negatively interact with others (here ‘egoists’) and positive interactors (here ‘altruist’). The results on altruist detection in two-level personnel evaluation tasks (T-PETs), with information on individual and team performance, suggested a disregard of the team performance and a resulting “Tragedy of Personnel Evaluation”. Experiment 1 transfers the idea of altruist detection in a personnel evaluation and personnel selection task (von Sydow & Braus, 2016) to egoist detection and explores whether there are analogous problems for egoist detection. Experiment 2 explores egoist and altruist detection in more realistic settings where individual and group-selection may affect our sampling of the interactor.

Rational and Semi-Rational Explanations of the Conjunction Fallacy: A Polycausal Approach

Conjunction fallacies (CF) have not only been a major obstacle in justifying the rationality of a Bayesian theory of belief update; they have also inspired a variety of theories on probability judgment and logical predication. Here we provide an overview of Bayesian logic (BL) as rational formulation of a pattern-based class of conjunction fallacies. BL is described here as a generalization of Bayesian Occam’s razor. BL captures the idea that probabilities are sometimes used not extensionally but intensionally, determining the probabilistic adequacy of ideal logical patterns. It is emphasized that BL is a class of models that depend on representations and the mea- nings of logical connectives. We discuss open questions and limits of BL. We also briefly discuss whether other theories of the CF may be good supplementary theories of CFs (and predication) as well, if linked to functional explanations.

Empathic Humans Punishing an Emotional Virtual Agent

Virtual agents have quietly entered our life in diverse everydaydomains. Human-Agent-Interaction can evoke any reaction,from complete rejection to great interest. But do humans im-plicitly regard virtual agents as pure machines, or beings on ananthropomorphic level? We asked participants to train an erro-neous virtual agent on a cognitive task and to reward or punishit. The agent showed human-like emotional facial reactions forthe experimental but not for the control group. We expectedparticipants from the experimental group to give less harmfulreinforcement and show more hesitation before punishing. Ad-ditionally, we hypothesised that participants with higher em-pathy show more compassion towards the agent and thereforewould give more positive reinforcement and feel worse whenpunishing. The results indicate that the agent’s expression ofemotionality is not the relevant factor for showing compassiontowards it. Conversely, human empathy seems to be an impor-tant factor causing compassion for virtual agents.

Exploitative and Exploratory Attention in a Four-Armed Bandit Task

When making decisions, we are often forced to choose between something safe we have chosen before, and something unknown to us that is inherently risky, but may provide a better long-term outcome. This problem is known as the Exploitation-Exploration (EE) Trade-Off. Most previous studies on the EE Trade-Off have relied on response data, leading to some ambiguity over whether uncertainty leads to true exploratory behavior, or whether the pattern of responding simply reflects a simpler ratio choice rule (such as the Generalized Matching Law (Baum, 1974; Herrnstein, 1961)). Here, we argue that the study of this issue can be enriched by measuring changes in attention (via eye-gaze), with the potential to disambiguate these two accounts. We find that when moving from certainty into uncertainty, the overall level of attention to stimuli in the task increases; a finding we argue is outside of the scope of ratio choice rules.

Actively Detecting Patterns in an Artificial Language to Learn Non-AdjacentDependencies

Many grammatical dependencies in natural language involve elements that are not adjacent, such as between thesubject and verb in ”the dog always barks”. We recently showed that non-adjacent dependencies are easily learnable withoutpauses in the signal when speech is presented rapidly. In this study, we used an online measure to look at the relationshipbetween online parsing and the learning performance from the offline assessment of non-adjacent dependency learning. Wefound that participants who showed current parsing of the language online also learned the dependencies better. However, thispattern disappeared when they are explicitly told where the boundaries are before parsing. Theories of non-adjacent dependencylearning are discussed.

A Plausible Micro Neural Circuit for Decision-Making

An intermediate level between neural circuits and behaviors is neural computations, various behaviors that animals exhibit following some basic control laws can be implemented by some canonical neural computations [Carandini, 2012]. To explore how the microscopic activity of neurons leads to macroscopic behavioral control strategy, we consider basic logic-like operations as some canonical computations in the brain. In this paper, firstly we designed the functional circuits for basic logic-like operations based on the known neurophysiological properties. Secondly, using basic functional circuits constructed a possible neural network for decision logic of animal’s behavior. This study provides a general approach for constructing the neural circuits to implement the behavioral control rules. Furthermore, this study will help us to establish a transitional bridge between the microscopic activity of the nervous system and the macroscopic animal behavior.

The Cognitive Reflection Test: familiarity and predictive power in professionals

The CRT is an increasingly well-known and used test of biassusceptibility. While alternatives are being developed, theoriginal remains in widespread use and this has led to itsbecoming increasingly familiar to psychology students(Stieger & Reips, 2016), resulting in inflated scores.Extending this work, we measure the effect of prior exposureto the CRT in a sample of oil industry professionals. Theseengineers and geoscientists completed the CRT, seven biastasks and rated their familiarity with all of these. Key resultswere that: familiarity increased CRT scores but tended not toreduce bias susceptibility; and industry personnel, evenwithout prior CRT exposure, scored very highly on the CRT -greatly reducing its predictive power. Conclusions are that thestandard CRT is not a useful tool for assessing biassusceptibility in highly numerate professionals – and doublyso when they have previously been exposed.

Desires influence 4- to 6-year-old children’s probabilistic judgments

Research on wishful thinking suggests that desires bias adult’s probability judgments. Previous research has yet toexplore if this extends to young children. In Experiment 1, 260 4- and 6-year-olds in the U.S. and Peru played a card game,where selecting a desirable card was unlikely. In Experiment 2, 200 4- to 6-year-old children were shown a bag of plasticeggs; a few contained desirable prizes. Children were asked to make predictions about what card / egg would be randomlyselected. Answers were compared to control conditions in which probability was comparable, but children had no reason todesire a specific outcome. In control conditions, children tended to state that the majority card/ egg would be selected. In theexperimental conditions, children were more likely to state that the desirable (and improbable) card/ egg would be selected.Results suggest that a desire bias extends to children as young as 4.

The Interaction of Bayesian Pragmatics and Lexical Semantics in Linguistic Interpretation: Using Event-related Potentials to Investigate Hearers’ Probabilistic Predictions

We contrast two views of how contextual influence on sentence meaning composition can be explained. The Semantic Similarity View maintains that discourse context affects sentence meaning mainly because of the semantic similarity between the words in the discourse context and the words in the sentence (as measured by Latent Semantic Analysis). The Free Pragmatic View, in contrast, defends the claim that also pragmatic aspects of the discourse context can affect sentence meaning composition. This effect can be quantitatively modelled by Bayesian Pragmatics. We introduce a Predictive Completion Task in which the hearer at every moment in a communicative situation has to generate a probabilistic prediction about how a discourse being uttered by the speaker is continued. We test the predictions of the two views in EEG using the well-established observation that the conditional probability of a word given a context is negatively correlated with the amplitude of its N400 component.

The Neural Mechanisms of Relational Reasoning: Dissociating Representational Types

The ability to reason about information is an essential human capability. It is less understood from the perspective of neuro- cognitive processes which can serve to constrain cognitive theories by implications from neuroscientific data. Despite some progress in the last decades, some disagreement about the experimental results and the cognitive processes of reasoning with abstract relations versus visuospatial relations persist. We conducted a cross-study meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies to determine the neural correlates of visuospatial and abstract relational reasoning. We analyzed 884 stereotactic data points from 38 studies and 692 subjects. We found that relational reasoning is mediated by the fronto- parietal network, especially the right precuneus and the left pars triangularis. Problems with abstract relations are processed by enhanced activation in the inferior parietal lobe, whereas visuospatial reasoning is promoted by prefrontal domains. Our results disentangle the neurocognitive mechanisms of different representational types of relational reasoning across study designs.

How the truth can make a great lie: An empirical investigation of the folk concept of lying by falsely implicating

Is it possible to lie despite not saying anyhing false? While the spontaneous answer seems to be ‘no’, there is some evidence from ordinary language that a lie does not require what is said to be believed-false. In this paper, we will argue for a pragmatic extension of the standard definition of lying. More specifically, we will present three experiments which show that people’s concept of lying is not about what is said, but about what is implied by saying it that way. We test three Gricean conversational maxims. For each one of them we demonstrate that if a speaker implies something misleading, even by saying something semantically true, it is still considered lying.

Eliciting Middle School Students’ Ideas About Graphs Supports Their Learningfrom a Computer Model

When middle school students learn science content withgraphs, the graphing and science knowledge may be mutuallyreinforcing: understanding the science content may help stu-dents interpret a related graph, and information from a graphmay illustrate a scientific concept. We examine this relation-ship between graphing and science by studying how studentslearn from interactive computer models with accompanyingdata graphs. The computer models provide an animated simu-lation that illustrates an unobservable phenomenon, while thedata graph tracks one or more quantities over time. This or-dering study, on middle school students learning about photo-synthesis, indicates that engaging with novel graph conceptshelped students interpret their data as they experimented withthe computer model. The study also provided some supportfor the opposite direction: experimenting with the model firsthelped students make sense of the graphs.

Counterfactual Conditionals and Normative Rules

Counterfactual thinking is the consideration of how things could have turned out differently, usually taking the form of counterfactual conditionals. This experiment examined the psychological mechanisms that transform counterfactuals into deontic guidance rules for the future. We examined how counterfactual thinking translates into deontic guidance rules by asking participants to infer these deontic conclusions from the counterfactual premises. Participants were presented with a vignette and a counterfactual conditional, and assigned to either a control condition or a suppression condition in which they were additionally presented with conflicting normative rules. The presence of conflicting norms reduced the likelihood of positive deontic conclusions being endorsed and increased the likelihood of negative deontic conclusions being endorsed. Future intentionality and regret intensity ratings were reduced in the suppression condition. The same conditions that affect normative inference also affect regret and future planning, suggesting similar cognitive mechanisms underlie these processes.

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation

A classic effect in the timing field is that “sounds are judged longer than lights” (Goldstone, Boardman & Lhamon, 1959). Recently, judgements for tactile durations have been found to fall between the two (Jones, Poliakoff & Wells, 2009). These modality differences are commonly interpreted within scalar timing theory as the work of a central pacemaker which runs faster for sounds, then vibrations, and slowest for lights (Wearden, Edwards, Fakhri & Percival, 1998). We investigated whether verbal estimates and temporal difference thresholds are correlated within each modality, but found this not to be the case. This suggests that differences in pacemaker speed may not be the main driver for modality differences in thresholds. In addition, we investigated sensory bias as an alternative to the pacemaker explanation, but this was found not to correlate with modality differences in timing.

Modeling cognitive load effects in an interrupted learning task: An ACT-R approach

Based on the established framework of Cognitive Load Theory, the presented research focuses on the inspection of cognitive load factors in an interrupted learning task. The task itself is inspired from basic cognitive research and demands participants to learn abstract symbol combinations of varying complexity. In addition, they have to deal with interruptions while performing the task. Experimental results indicate the influence of task complexity on how interruptions effect learning performance. However, questions on underlying learner cognition persist, rising the need for a more in-depth way of examination. For this purpose, a cognitive model within the cognitive architecture ACT-R is developed to clarify cognitive processes and mechanisms within different conditions of the task. Preliminary results from a first model for the easy task condition already indicate some fit between human and model data. Modeling work continues with adjusting the current model and implementing a model for the difficult task condition.

Does Associative Memory Play a Role in Solving Physics Problems?

Previous research has found that people frequently provideincorrect predictions about the path of moving objects when givenan idealised physics problem to solve. The aim of this research wasto explore whether these incorrect predictions are due to theapplication of an incorrect naïve physics theory, whether incorrectperceptions generated from past experiences lead tomisconceptions of how moving objects behave, or whether it is acombination of both. Thirty-one participants volunteered to takepart in the experiment which followed a two (experiencecongruent/incongruent with naïve physics theory) by two (carriedversus free-moving object) within-subject design. The dependentvariable was participant response (straight down or curvedforwards). Results of the study revealed that participants providedanswers both consistent and inconsistent with the naïve physicstheory. This suggests that responses were primarily elicitedthrough the retrieval of associatively-mediated memories of similarscenarios - some of which contain perceptual illusions. Possiblemethodological limitations and alternative theoretical explanationsare discussed, along with practical and theoretical implications foreducation and learning.

Sensorimotor Learning Modulates Automatic Imitation in Visual Speech

People automatically imitate observed actions, including speech. Automatic Imitation (AI) is linked to observation-execution associations in the mirror neuron system (MNS). AI is measured using interference tasks, in which prompts (say”ba” or ”da”) are paired with congruent or incongruent distracters (video of someone saying ”ba” or ”da”). Faster responses forcongruent than for incongruent prompt-distracter pairings signal AI. Observation-execution associations for speech actions arethought to be inflexible, unlike associations for manual actions, which have been shown to be flexible. We trained participantsto reinforce or abolish their AI response by providing them with compatible (say ”ba” for a video of someone saying ”ba”) orincompatible training (say ”ba” for a video of ”da”). After training, the AI response was reduced for participants who receivedincompatible training, thus showing that the MNS for speech actions is also flexible and subject to experience, like the MNSfor manual actions.

Whoa! Aww . . . Ohh . . . Hee! and Mmm: Infants’ nuanced distinctions about theprobable causes of emotional expressions

Can infants map diverse positive emotional expressions to their probable causes? Across two studies (includingone pre-registered experiment), we used a preferential-looking task to find that infants as young as 12-17 months (mean:14.8 months) successfully matched non-verbal vocalizations elicited by funny, exciting, adorable, sympathetic, and deliciousimages to their probable causes (Experiments 1 and 2). Do infants also posit unobserved causes of emotional expressions? Inboth exploratory and pre-registered experiments, an adult peeked into a box and made one of two distinct positive emotionalvocalizations (Experiment 3: “Aww!” or “Mmm!”; Experiment 4: “Aww!” or “Whoa!”). Infants reaching into the box retrievedeither a probable or improbable cause of the reaction. Infants were more likely to search again on incongruent trials. Theseresults suggest that infants make nuanced distinctions among emotions, and infer probable causes of emotional reactions.

Visual and Audio Aware Bi-Modal Video Emotion Recognition

With rapid increase in the size of videos online, analysis andprediction of affective impact that video content will haveon viewers has attracted much attention in the community.To solve this challenge several different kinds of informationabout video clips are exploited. Traditional methods normallyfocused on single modality, either audio or visual. Later onsome researchers tried to establish multi-modal schemes andspend a lot of time choosing and extracting features by differ-ent fusion strategy. In this research, we proposed an end-to-end model which can automatically extract features and targetan emotional classification task by integrating audio and vi-sual features together and also adding the temporal character-istics of the video. The experimental study on commonly usedMediaEval 2015 Affective Impact of Movies has shown thismethod’s potential and it is expected that this work could pro-vide some insight for future video emotion recognition fromfeature fusion perspective.

Teaching Versus Active Learning:A Computational Analysis of Conditions that Affect Learning

Researchers have debated whether instructional-based teach-ing or exploration-based active learning is better for decadeswith unsatisfying results. A main obstacle is the difficulty inprecisely controlling and characterizing the pedagogical meth-ods used and the learning conditions in empirical studies. Toaddress this, we leveraged existing computational models ofteaching and active learning to formalize the methods and thelearning process. We compared the two pedagogical methodsin a concept-learning framework and investigated their effec-tiveness under various scenarios. Our results show that whenthe learner and teacher are conceptually aligned, teaching isat least as effective as, and often much more effective than ac-tive learning, but when the alignment is broken, active learningcan yield moderate improvement over teaching. We concludeby discussing our results’ implications for the debate and theprospects of bringing computational models to bear on com-plex real-world problems that are resistant to simple experi-mental investigation.

Training Graph Literacy: Developing the RiskLiteracy.org Outreach Platform

Visual aids have been found to provide an unusually efficientmeans of risk communication for diverse and vulnerableindividuals facing high-stakes choices (e.g., health, finance,natural hazards). Research indicates the benefits of visual aidsfollow from scaffolding of cognitive and metacognitiveprocesses that enable independent evaluation andunderstanding of risk—i.e., risk literacy (see Skilled DecisionTheory; Cokely et al.,. 2012; in press). Here, we present a briefreview and progress report on the development of an onlineadaptive graph literacy tutor developed as part of theRiskLiteracy.org decision education platform. We begin witha brief review of theoretical foundations of the current tutorbased on graph comprehension theory. Next, we discuss keysteps in developing and validating our pseudo-intelligentadaptive tutor with emphasis on cognitive and psychometricitem analyses and transfer assessments (i.e., decision-makingbiases). Finally, we present recent changes in technicalimplementation of the RiskLiteracy.org platform (i.e., Pythonbased with a NoSQL database) that are designed to facilitateinteractive, yet brief (5 minute to 3 hour) and easier-to-developtraining and risk communication tutors. Discussion focuses onemerging opportunities including cognitive oriented usabilityanalyses that should help promote an effective, enjoyable, andinclusive user experience.

Empirical constraints on computational level models of interference effects inhuman probabilistic judgements.

Decades of research in decision making have established thatthere are some situations where human judgments cannot bemodelled according to classical probability theory. Yet if weabandon classical (Bayesian) probability theory as an overar-ching framework for constructing cognitive models, what dowe replace it with? In this contribution we outline a way to di-vide the space of possible computational level models of prob-abilistic judgment into a hierarchy of levels of increasing com-plexity, with classical Bayesian probability models occupyingthe lowest level. Each level has a unique experimental sig-nature, and we examine which level is best able to describehuman behavior in a particular probabilistic reasoning task.

Evidence for overt visual attention to hand gestures as a function of redundancyand speech disfluency

We investigated the effect of gesture redundancy and speechdisfluency on listeners’ fixations to gestures. Participantswatched a speaker producing a redundant or non-redundantgesture, while producing fluent or disfluent speech. Eyemovements were recorded. Participants spent little time on aspeaker’s gestures regardless of condition. Gestureredundancy and speech disfluency did not affect listeners’percentage dwell time to a speaker’s gestures. However,listeners were more likely to fixate to a speaker’s gestureswhen they expected the gesture to be non-redundant.Listeners were also more likely to fixate to a speaker’sgestures when the speaker was disfluent. Thus, listenersallocate overt visual attention based on the expectedusefulness of a speaker’s gestures, although evidence does notsuggest that they spend more time fixating on these gestures.Furthermore, listeners are sensitive to disfluency in aspeaker’s utterance and change how they attend to gesturesbased on qualities of the speech.

Physical problem solving:Joint planning with symbolic, geometric, and dynamic constraints

In this paper, we present a new task that investigates how peo-ple interact with and make judgments about towers of blocks.In Experiment 1, participants in the lab solved a series of prob-lems in which they had to re-configure three blocks from aninitial to a final configuration. We recorded whether they usedone hand or two hands to do so. In Experiment 2, we askedparticipants online to judge whether they think the person inthe lab used one or two hands. The results revealed a closecorrespondence between participants’ actions in the lab, andthe mental simulations of participants online. To explain par-ticipants’ actions and mental simulations, we develop a modelthat plans over a symbolic representation of the situation, exe-cutes the plan using a geometric solver, and checks the plan’sfeasibility by taking into account the physical constraints of thescene. Our model explains participants’ actions and judgmentsto a high degree of quantitative accuracy.

Three-Way Bindings in Associative Recognition

To avoid interference among similar memory traces it is re-quired to form complex memory structures that include mul-tiple components of the event, which helps one to distinguishone event from another. In a laboratory setting, these complexbinding structures have been studied through a paradigm whereone has to form a memory structure that includes two items andthe context together (i.e., three-way binding). However, de-spite the long history of the theoretical concept, its importance,and the existence of the laboratory paradigm, three-way bind-ing structures have only been examined in recall paradigms.Moreover, not all memory models consider the ability to formthree-way binding structures as a default. Therefore, the cur-rent study examined the use and formation of three-way bind-ing structures in an associative recognition paradigm. Resultsprovide evidence that three-way binding structures are usedduring recognition, which implies that it is critical for mem-ory models to properly represent them.

Emotional and Cognitive Interest: How Creating Situational Interest Affects Learning with Multimedia

Situational interest is the positive affect and sustained attention triggered by particular contexts (Hidi & Renninger, 2006). Some studies show interesting information enhances learning while others find it hinders learning, producing the seductive detail effect. Limited evidence suggests the seductive detail effect is weakened if emotionally interesting information is relevant to main ideas. The present research shows the seductive detail effect occurs only under certain conditions. Harp and Mayer (1997) proposed that generating cognitive, rather than emotional, interest is more effective for improving learning by cueing relationships among concepts for easier processing. Hidi and Renninger (2006) argue distinguishing between the emotional and cognitive might be artificial. Present research found benefits from cognitive interest but no support as to whether cognitive interest is necessarily a distinct type of interest from emotional interest. There were some challenges with operationalizing cognitive interest, as well as validating strategies utilized to manipulate cognitive interest levels.

“I won’t lie, it wasn’t amazing”: Modeling polite indirect speech

Why are we polite when we talk to one another? One hypoth-esis is that people expect others to choose what to say basedon their goals both to transfer information efficiently (an epis-temic goal) and to make the listener feel good (a social goal).In our previous work, we found that when these two goals con-flict, they sometimes produce white lies. In the current work,we expand on this theory to consider another prominent case ofpolite speech: indirect remarks using negation (e.g., “It wasn’tamazing”). With minimal extensions from our previous frame-work, our formal model suggests that a pragmatic speaker willproduce more indirect remarks when the speaker wants to beinformative and seem considerate at the same time. Thesepredictions were borne out in a language production experi-ment. These findings suggest that the conflict between socialand epistemic goals can account for a broad range of politenessphenomena.

The Structure of Young Children’s Numerical and Spatial Abilities

We conducted a study of 400 preschool children to determine whether spatial and numerical skills rely on commonprocesses. Children completed a battery of mathematical tasks as part of an ongoing preschool formative assessment develop-ment project. We created theoretically meaningful skills from these tasks and carried out item response theoretic analyses oneach skill. We extracted Rasch scores for each of the skills and carried out multiple factor analyses to determine whether oneor more factors best characterized spatial and numerical skills. Finally, we regressed factor scores on demographic variables,including age, gender, socioeconomic status, and verbal ability. We discuss how our results add to our understanding of theconnection between spatial and numerical processes and their implications for closing the achievement gap in early education.

Alternation blindness in the perception of binary sequences

Binary information is prevalent in the environment. In this study, we examined how people process repetition and alternation in binary sequences. Across four paradigms involving estimation, working memory, change detection, and visual search, we found that the number of alternations is under-estimated compared to repetitions (Experiment 1). Moreover, recall for binary sequences deteriorates as the sequence alternates more (Experiment 2). Changes in bits are also harder to detect as the sequence alternates more (Experiment 3). Finally, visual targets superimposed on bits of a binary sequence take longer to process as alternation increases (Experiment 4). Overall, our results indicate that compared to repetition, alternation in a binary sequence is less salient in the sense of requiring more attention for successful encoding. The current study thus reveals the cognitive constraints in the representation of alternation and provides a new explanation for the over-alternation bias in randomness perception.

Back to ABCs: Clustering Alphabetically, Rather than Semantically, EnhancesVocabulary Learning

Optimizing the study of vocabulary words for high-stakes tests such as the SAT or GRE prep can beproblematic, given that many words are semantically,orthographically, or phonologically confusable. Companiesmarketing test preparation programs make multiplerecommendations, such as clustering words on some basis,but little research has been carried out to examine what thatbasis should be. Across two experiments, we compare theefficacy of different types of clustering—categorical,alphabetical, and confusable--for the learning ofsemantically related words (Experiment 1) and confusablewords (Experiment 2). We demonstrate that, in contrastto most learners’ intuitions, an alphabetical sequence yieldssuperior learning.

Domain-General Learning of Neural Network Models to Solve Analogy Tasks– A Large-Scale Simulation

Several computational models have been proposed toexplain the mental processes underlying analogical reasoning.However, previous models either lack a learning componentor use limited, artificial data for simulations. To address theseissues, we build a domain-general neural network model thatlearns to solve analogy tasks in different modalities, e.g., textsand images. Importantly, it uses word representations andimage representations computed from large-scale naturalisticcorpus. The model reproduces several key findings in theanalogical reasoning literature, including relational shift andfamiliarity effect, and demonstrates domain-general learningcapacity. Our model also makes interesting predictions oncross-modality transfer of analogical reasoning that could beempirically tested. Our model makes the first step towards acomputational framework that is able to learn analogy tasksusing naturalistic data and transfer to other modalities.

Convincing Conversations: Using a Computer-Based Dialogue System to Promote a Plant-Based Diet

In this study, we tested the effectiveness of a computer-based persuasive dialogue system designed to promote a plant-based diet. The production and consumption of meat and dairy has been shown to be a major cause of climate change and a threat to public health, bio-diversity, animal rights and human rights. A system promoting plant-based diets was developed, comprising conversational, motivational and argumentational elements. 280 participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions, each representing a particular combination of motivational and argumentational modules. Male participants showed higher intention scores in the motivational conditions compared to the argumentation-only or control condition. Female participants scored higher overall, unaffected by condition. These results suggest that men and women are differentially sensitive to persuasive strategies regarding the adoption of a plant-based diet. It seems to be particularly worthwhile to use motivational - as opposed to merely argumentational - elements in a persuasive conversation

Pseudoneglect and development: Age-related spatial bias in bisection and drawing

The numerous studies on pseudoneglect have generated inconsistent results and disagreement concerning the underlying mechanisms. Most research supports the hypothesis that hemispheric lateralization is the main reason for the persistent leftward bias in spatial tasks. Findings on the influence of reading direction, handedness and participant age are largely contradictory. As a result of brain maturation adults usually perform with significant leftward bias. However, both hemispheric activation and scanning habits exert an influence on space representation, which varies across age groups. Preschoolers, middle school children and adults were tested on the line and word bisection tasks and on house-person-tree drawing tasks. The analysis of their performance produced results consistent with an explanatory account that the direction of the spatial bias shifts leftwards in the course of development.

Is Red Fire Warmer than Blue Fire? Colored Thermal Words in a Stroop Task

In many languages there are concepts for warm and cold colors. Research on color-temperature correspondenceand their interaction is quite scarce, and based mostly on subjective measures. It is still unknown whether and to what extentcolors bear the thermal information. The current study explored the relationship between warm and cold colors (red andblue) and thermal aspects of the word semantics (sun, snow), using the Stroop paradigm in a color categorization task. Itwas hypothesized that if colors activate the thermal meaning then Stroop effect should occur. The results suggested a color-temperature compatibility effect – faster responses when associated color and thermal meaning corresponded (e.g. sun presentedin red). This provides important information on the automaticity of thermal activation during word processing, and on thestrength of conceptual associations in color perception. It was suggested that words induced mental simulation of the thermalconcepts, together with the associated color.

The Redundancy Effect in Human Causal Learning: Evidence Against a Comparator Theory Explanation

The blocking effect, canonical in the study of associative learning, is often explained as a failure of the blocked cue to become associated with the outcome. However, this perspective fails to explain recent findings that suggest learning about a blocked cue is superior to a different type of redundant cue. We report an experiment designed to test the proposal that blocking is not a failure of association, but a performance effect arising from a comparator process (Denniston, Savastano, & Miller, 2001). Participants received A+ AX+ BY+ CY- training containing a blocked cue X and another redundant cue Y, before rating outcome expectancies for individual cues. These ratings were inconsistent with the association-failure view. After subsequent A- Y+ training, participants rated cues again. Ratings in the second test were inconsistent with the comparator theory. Our data suggest that neither perspective is likely to provide a complete account of causal learning.

Modeling Semantic Fluency Data as Search on a Semantic Network

Psychologists have used the semantic fluency task fordecades to gain insight into the processes and representationsunderlying memory retrieval. Recent work has suggested thata censored random walk on a semantic network resemblessemantic fluency data because it produces optimal foraging.However, fluency data have rich structure beyond beingconsistent with optimal foraging. Under the assumption thatmemory can be represented as a semantic network, we test avariety of memory search processes and examine how wellthese processes capture the richness of fluency data. Thesearch processes we explore vary in the extent they explorethe network globally or exploit local clusters, and whetherthey are strategic. We found that a censored random walkwith a priming component best captures the frequency andclustering effects seen in human fluency data.

The Impact of Decision Agency & Granularity onAptitude Treatment Interaction in Tutoring

In this study, we explored the impact of the decision agency(Student vs. Tutor) and granularity (Problem vs. Step) acrossstudents with different levels of incoming competence (Highvs. Low). Students were randomly assigned to four conditionsand split into High and Low groups based on their pre-testscores. All students used the same Intelligent Tutoring Sys-tem (ITS) called Pyrenees, followed the same general proce-dure, studied the same training materials, and worked throughthe same training problems. The only substantive differencesamong the four conditions were decision agency and granular-ity. That is: who decided to present an example or to solvea problem: the student or the ITS tutor; and was the deci-sion made problem-by-problem or step-by-step? Our overallresults showed that there were significant different impacts ofthe decision agency and granularity between High and Lowstudents on learning performance. More specifically, for Highstudents granularity was the more dominant factor in that steplevel decisions can be more effective than problem level deci-sions regardless of the decision agency; for Low students therewas a significant interaction effect in that: Low students ben-efit significantly more when they were making problem-leveldecisions than making step-level decisions, but no significantdifference was not found when the decisions were made bythe tutor. Much to our surprise, both High and Low groupsshowed strong decision-making preference for problem solv-ing over worked example at both problem and step levels.

Information Seeking as Chasing Anticipated Prediction Errors

When faced with delayed, uncertain rewards, humans andother animals usually prefer to know the eventual outcomesin advance. This preference for cues providing advance infor-mation can lead to seemingly suboptimal choices, where lessreward is preferred over more reward. Here, we introduce areinforcement-learning model of this behavior, the anticipatedprediction error (APE) model, based on the idea that predic-tion errors themselves can be rewarding. As a result, animalswill sometimes pick options that yield large prediction errors,even when the expected rewards are smaller. We compare theAPE model against an alternative information-bonus model,where information itself is viewed as rewarding. These mod-els are evaluated against a newly collected dataset with humanparticipants. The APE model fits the data as well or betterthan the other models, with fewer free parameters, thus provid-ing a more robust and parsimonious account of the suboptimalchoices. These results suggest that anticipated prediction er-rors can be an important signal underpinning decision making.

Interruptions Reduce Confidence Judgments: Predictions of Three Sequential Sampling Models

The relationship between confidence and accuracy has been modeled many times. This paper compares and contrasts three decision-making mathematical models (2DSD, Poisson, RTCON2) of confidence and investigates how each model predicts the effects of interruptions on accuracy, decision response time, confidence, and confidence response time.

Posters: Member Abstracts

Simulation and heuristics in flexible tool use

Humans are remarkably flexible tool users. We not only recognize a wide range of existing tools, but also producenew tools by seeing objects in new ways, or by making or repurposing objects to solve a problem confronting us. Here westudy the cognitive processes supporting flexible tool use, including deciding what makes a good tool, and how it should beused. Participants played a video game which requires selecting an object from a set of options and placing it in a virtualphysical scene in order to accomplish goals such as tipping another object over or launching it into a container. People appearto use a combination of simulation-based planning and experience-based heuristics: fast heuristics drive the initial selectionand placement of a candidate tool, and that solution can then be refined by several rounds of mental simulation interspersedwith trial-and-error experimentation to rapidly converge on goal-satisfying solutions.

Predicting Preschool-Aged Children’s Behavior Regulation from Attention Tasksin the Lab

One challenge in studying cognition over the lifespan is designing tasks that measure the same construct in dif-ferent age groups and relate reliably to real-world outcomes. The current study confronts this challenge by testing a newparadigm to assess attention in preschool-aged children for comparison with other measures. Children completed the new“Pop-the-Bubbles” paradigm plus Flanker and Visual Search tasks, for comparison with parental reports of behavioral regula-tion. Correlations between behavioral regulation and measures from both Flanker and Pop-the-Bubbles suggest that children’sability to ignore irrelevant stimuli in these lab tasks relates to their ability to behave appropriately in everyday situations. Fur-ther development of Pop-the-Bubbles for eye-tracking and a color version of Flanker are underway to test these relationshipsmore extensively in young children.

Why do we punish negligent behaviors?

Prior research suggests that negligent harms are punished because of the resulting negative outcomes. Under thisaccount, negligent but completely harmless acts should not be punished. An alternative possibility is that negligence is punishedas a way of modifying future thought and behavior. Across three studies we find support for this second proposal. Study 1demonstrates that punishment is assigned to negligent agents, irrespective of whether or not a harm actually occurs. Study 2demonstrates that non-negligent agents who cause harm are punished less than negligent agents who do not cause harm. Study3 shows that the punishment of harmful negligent actions is only judged to be successful when it results in the agent ceasing toact negligently, and not when it results in the harm ceasing to occur. Together, these results suggest that a primary function ofpunishment in cases of negligence is modify future thought.

When I say ‘Black Lives Matter,’ why do some people hear ‘Others Lives MatterLess’?

The statement ”Black lives matter” is commonly construed as implying other lives matter less, even though thestatement does not explicitly reference other lives. Bias is a common explanation for this construal. However, other factors maycontribute. We hypothesized that the linguistic structure of “Black lives matter” plays an important role. ”Black lives matter”takes the form of a generic, or statements in which a property is attributed to members of a set (e.g., “lions have manes”).Generics are often interpreted as implicit comparisons (e.g., “lions are more likely to have manes than other animals”). Wereport two experiments in which we find evidence that the statement “Black lives matter” is often construed as an implicitcomparative claim, similarly to other generics. This research contributes to our understanding of generics, while providing anovel explanation for why when I say ”Black lives matter,” some people hear ”Other lives matter less.”

The Use of Ambiguous Messages as a Strategy to Appeal to Multiple DecisionMakers

Messages that are tailored to specific audiences (matched messages) are typically more persuasive compared tomessages that are crafted for a general audience (Hirsh, Kang, & Bodenhauser, 2012). However, tailoring messages can havethe effect that messages are less persuasive for audiences for which they were not tailored (mismatched messages; Sillince,Jarzabkowski, & Shaw, 2012). Eisenberg (1984) introduced the concept of strategic ambiguity to appeal to multiple audiencessimultaneously. We systematically compared effects of matched/mismatched tailored messages with the effects of ambiguousmessages on multiple-criteria choice behavior. We found evidence that ambiguous messages can be used under certain con-ditions to simultaneously appeal to multiple audiences within the context of credit card choices. Using the financial controltypology developed by Shefrin and Nicols (2014) to define different audiences, the study (154 participants) provided somesupport for the use of ambiguity as a tool for tailoring messages to diverse credit-card holders.

Social wayfinding in complex environments

Wayfinders in a group can be influenced by various factors, including other group members and environmentalstructure, but social wayfinding is an underexplored topic. This experiment investigated differences in wayfinding decisionsbetween individuals and groups and their dependence on environmental structure. Participants navigated through a train stationwith or without market stalls, either as individuals or as groups. There was a significant main effect of environmental structureon task efficiency, and an inconclusive interaction between environmental structure and group membership on task efficiency(p=0.05). Because of heterogeneity of variance, we conducted targeted t-tests. T-tests revealed that groups were slower thanindividuals with market stalls (p=0.02) but not without (p=0.91). There was significant main effect of the environmentalstructure on number of turns. The main effect of group membership on number of turns and the interaction were not significant.We will analyze walked and Levenstein distance as wayfinding efficiency indicators.

Projecting space into the future: peripersonal space remaps in anticipation of anobject manipulation

Manipulation planning relies on anticipatory processes, aimed at achieving the desired goal state, such as a grasp.This implies that peripersonal space is remapped to the anticipated grasp posture on the targeted object. Vibrotactile-visualinteractions were probed at different times during a grasp-and-place task. Thumb or index finger were stimulated concurrentlywith a visual distractor on the to-be-grasped object. Object orientation (upright/upside down) afforded a thumb-up or thumb-down grasp, inverting the congruency between haptic and visual stimulation. Response times about which finger was stimulatedshow the expected crossmodal congruency effect already before motion onset, with shorter times when the visual distractor andthe future position of the stimulated finger overlapped. Moreover, eye-tracking data show that the tactile stimulation influencesthe gaze in anticipation of the upcoming grasp. Thus peripersonal hand space is mapped into the future, predictively mediatingbetween tactile and visual perceptions as a function of the final state.

Characterizing Human-Machine Teams with Process Algebras

We conceptualize human-machine (computer, robot) teams as concurrent processes. Such a conceptualizationmeans: (1) the human and machine agents have a common goal or mission; (2) each agent may have different subtasks withinthe goal space; (3) they do not have a shared memory, but (4) they do have a means of communicating with each other. Processalgebras, such as communicating sequential processes (Hoare, 1977), are formal languages for describing the ways in whichtwo concurrent processes interact through message passing across information channels. In this research, we enumerate theways in which human-machine interactions can be structured, such as strictly serial, parallel, and cascade-like architectures.We use process algebras to characterize the interactions in candidate architectures. We discuss design implications for activeand interactive machine learning systems.

Bottom-up attentional cueing in category learning in children

Young children tend to differ from adults in how they learn new categories. In comparison to adults (who relyon selective attention and tend to form explicit rules), children distribute attention widely, forming similarity-based categoryrepresentations. But, when attention is explicitly directed toward the rule with top-down feedback, children exhibit rule-based classification–though memory performance still indicates distributed attention. Little is known, however, how bottom-upattentional cueing affects the category representations that children form. In our experiment 4-year-olds learned to classifyalien creatures composed of binary features. A single “deterministic” feature perfectly predicted category membership, whileother features were probabilistically predictive. We manipulated the saliency of the deterministic feature, making it growand shrink. This manipulation was remarkably effective at facilitating category learning and rule-based classification, butrecognition memory still showed evidence of distributed attention. These results help elucidate the important role of attentionalprocesses in the development of categorization.

Infusing Cognitive Science Content in Teacher Preparation

Surprisingly, foundational knowledge about cognitive science (CS) is not included in all teacher preparationprograms or required for certification in all states. Here, I examine the impact of infusing CS content into teacher-candidates’ coursework by providing half of the pedagogy instructors with professional development on big ideas in CS (mem-ory/attention/transfer/problem solving/practice/expertise) and encouraging them to use the materials to deliver mini-lectures onthese topics and discuss their relevance to instructional practice. Control instructors did not receive PD or CS materials. Inboth experimental and control classes, CS knowledge was measured at the beginning and end of the semester; we also collectedlesson plans where teacher-candidates explained their reasoning for each instructional decision. We saw no CS knowledgeimprovement, but teacher-candidates exposed to CS reduced their use of folk reasons (e.g., buzz words such as learning styles,concrete thinking, active learning, etc.) when planning lessons compared with peers in control classes.

Using Analogical Processing to Categorize Musical Patterns

Participants often categorize musical melodies (“themes”) based on perceptual features (e.g. loudness, fastness) in-stead of structural or relational features (e.g. pitch, rhythm) (Lamont & Dibben, 2001; Ziv & Eitan, 2007). In the present study,we investigate whether within-category analogical comparison (Markman & Gentner, 1993) influences participants to cate-gorize musical themes based on relational features, a prediction from structure-mapping theory (Gentner, 1983). Participantscompleted a forced-choice triad task where they had to choose whether one theme (relational match) or another (perceptualmatch) best fit the target theme. In a “no-compare” condition (between-subjects), participants heard one target theme. In a“compare” condition, participants heard and compared two target themes. Initial results indicate that participants who com-pared two themes chose more relational matches. We found this result for Western Classical themes and popular music chordprogressions. These results and their implications are discussed with respects to analogical processing and musical categoriza-tion.

How infants map nonce phrases to scenes with objects and predicates.

When infants hear sentences containing unfamiliar words, are some language-world links (such as noun-object)more readily formed than others (verb-predicate)? What if the context renders verb-predicate and noun-object interpretationsequally plausible? We examined 14-15-month-olds’ capacity for linking semantic elements of scenes with simple bisyllabicnonce utterances. Each syllable either referred to the object, or the object’s motion. Infants heard the syllables in either a VS-or SV-consistent order. Learning was tested using 2AFC language-guided looking. Infants learned the nouns and verbs equallywell, showing no bias favoring nouns. In all conditions, infants learned the meaning of the utterance-final syllable, but not theinitial one, suggesting that noun or verb biases played a smaller role than utterance position when noun- and verb-learning wereequally supported by context.

No Tranfer of Training in Simple Addition

Several researchers have proposed that skilled adults may solve single-digit addition problems (e.g. 3+1=4, 4+3=7)using a fast counting procedure. Practicing a procedure often leads to transfer of learning and faster performance of unpracticeditems. Such transfer has been demonstrated using a counting-based alphabet arithmetic task (e.g., B+4 = C D E F) that indicatedrobust RT gains when untrained transfer problems at test had been implicitly practiced (e.g., practice B+3, test B+2 or B+1).Here we constructed analogous simple addition problems (practice 4+3, test 4+2 or 4+1). In three experiments (n=108) therewas no evidence of generalization for these items, but there was robust speed up when the items were repeated. The results areconsistent with direct retrieval of addition facts from long-term memory rather than a counting procedure.

Vowel Harmony as a Distributional Learning Problem

Vowel harmony is a class of phonotactic restrictions in which vowels in a language are divided into two or moresubclasses, and words must contain only vowels from only one such subclass regardless of intervening consonants. Languagesworldwide (Turkish, Finnish, Mongolian, Warlpiri, but not English) exhibit vowel harmony. The opacity of such potentiallylong distance alternations poses a challenge for the learner. Nevertheless, infants are sensitive to vowel harmony alternationsat as young as seven months. We present a computational model for vowel harmony acquisition. By normalizing transitionalprobabilities over the vowel tier, and making minimal assumptions about the phonology, we successfully determine which testlanguages have harmony processes and correctly categorize their vowels into harmonizing classes. Using universal typologicalpatterns to inform the search space, we find that phenomena which appear opaque can be captured by simple distributionallearning.

A comparative assessment of embodied and computational topic extraction

Word embedding algorithms like word2vec (Mikolov et al., 2013) have enabled advances in topic modelling bytraining shallow neural networks on the co-occurrence of words in corpuses of sentences. However, it is not clear how thisprocess reflects human cognition. This poster will compare the results of document classification using the word2vec skipgrammodel and the 20k sensorimotor word norms collected by the presenter and colleagues (Lynott & Connell 2013; Carney et al.,in prep.) (These latter norms establish how concepts are processed by way of perceptual and motor schemes, and thus offer auseful proxy for human conceptual classification.) The results of the comparison will generate insights into the different waysin which higher-order concepts are inferred, and allow systematic biases in concept formation to be identified. It will also allowfor machine learning processes to be finessed so as to more accurately reflect human-level modes of cognition.

Mapping hand to world; Development of iconic representation in gesture andhomesign

In both gesture and sign, objects and events can be represented by reproducing some of their features iconically.Iconic gestures do not typically appear until well into children’s second year of life, suggesting that the cognitive and/or com-municative resources required are not trivial. Here we investigate how manual iconicity develops in two different communica-tive systems. Using longitudinal video corpora, we compare the emergence of manual iconicity in 52 hearing children learninga spoken language (co-speech gesture) to a deaf child creating a manual communication system (homesign). We focus on theshape of the hand, asking how handshape use changes between age 1 and 5, and how handshape choice relates to semanticcontent. We find broadly similar patterns of handshape development in co-speech gesture and homesign. This suggests that thecognitive building blocks underlying children’s ability to iconically map forms to meanings are shared across vastly differentcommunicative systems.

One-shot word learning under high and low sentential constraints in adult L2learners of Chinese

New words were embedded in high- and low-constraint sentences and presented three times in a random order toadult learners of Chinese as a second language. The learners explained the meaning of each word in their native languages andtheir answers were scored by other native speakers. The overall accuracy was .47 with no effect of constraint or frequency.When the data were limited to those words the learners reported having no prior encounters with and those sentences theyreported comprehending, the accuracy was .45. The results demonstrated fast mapping of word learning in adult L2 learners butindicated that extended mapping was necessary to achieve ultimate attainment. The results are also consistent with Krashen’s(1982) “comprehensible input” and “i+1” hypothesis.

Comparing Human Use of Fast & Frugal Tree with Machine-Learning Tree

Previous studies have shown that the predictive accuracy of fast and frugal decision trees (FFTs) is comparable todecision trees generated by machine-learning (Martignon et al., 2008). FFTs are thought to be useful decision tools that arecognitively plausible to internalise, as opposed to complex machine-learning algorithms. Nonetheless, there seems to be a lackof behavioural studies in the literature to support such a claim. In this between-group experiment, we examined the human useof an FFT versus a C4.5 algorithm tree when completing a car evaluation task. Participants had to learn the rules of their giventree before making evaluations based on their memory. Preliminary results show that FFTs may indeed be easier to use, evenwhen the number of cues for both trees are the same. Interestingly, participants who were successful in using the C4.5 treeexhibited tree pruning strategies, resulting in a heuristic similar to an FFT.

Incorrect responses salience affects strategy use in a figural analogy task

Previous studies of multiple-choice analogy problems suggested that some people use a more efficient but also harderconstructive strategy (they build the complete representation of analogy), whereas others tend to use a less effective but simplerresponse elimination. We tested whether salience of incorrect options (five per figural analogy problem) affected strategy use.Salient options in 18 problems missed many features from the (sixth) correct option; options in 18 non-salient problems missedonly few features. When controlling for working memory capacity, eye tracking yielded strongly correlating patterns of datathat suggested, in line with previous reports, large individual variance in strategy use. However, participants overall spent50% less time analyzing salient than non-salient options, suggesting that salience promoted the constructive strategy. Thisconclusion was supported by pupil size significantly predicting accuracy on problems with salient options, but not on thosewith non-salient options (which additionally yielded lower accuracy).

Learning Object Names from Visual Pervasiveness: the Visual Statistics Predict

Recent analysis of a corpus of infant-perspective head-camera images found an extremely right-skewed frequencydistribution of objects present in 8- to 10-month-old infants’ visual environments (Clerkin, et al., 2017). Furthermore, theobjects most pervasively present in these scenes have names normatively acquired first by learners of English. New analysesshow that the names for these objects occur only sparsely in infants’ environments, and object name frequency is not correlatedwith object visual frequency. Therefore, we designed a simple associative model simulating word-object co-occurrence in orderto investigate how visual pervasiveness without high-frequency naming could lead to learning of word-object correspondences.With random sampling from distributions reflecting the actual frequency of words and objects in infants’ environments, we findthat the most frequent objects have a distinct advantage over less frequent objects in their conditional probability. This suggestsvisual experience with objects may be the principal predictor of early word-referent learning.

Relations Between Intuitive Biological Thought and Scientific Misconceptions

Students enter educational settings with complex and well-established intuitive conceptual understandings of theworld, which have important educational consequences. In biology, intuitive thinking can be characterized in terms of cogni-tive construals (anthropocentric, teleological, and essentialist thinking, Coley & Tanner, 2015). We examined relations betweenintuitive thinking and biological misconceptions, and how formal biology education might influence such relations. 137 bi-ology and non-science majors completed measures of anthropocentric, teleological, and essentialist thinking, and indicatedagreement/disagreement with common misconceptions and explained their responses. Teleological thinking (but not anthro-pocentric or essentialist thinking) predicted teleological misconceptions. Anthropocentric and teleological thinking (but notessentialist thinking) predicted anthropocentric misconceptions. Agreement with essentialist misconceptions was unrelated tointuitive thinking. Similar patterns for non-majors and majors suggests formal biology education may have little influence onrelations between intuitive reasoning and misconceptions. These findings demonstrate a clear impact of intuitive thinking onlearning biology at the university level.

A picture falls under many categories: How ancient mathematical marks becameextinct

The development of mathematical marking conventions from prehistory to the present is characterized by a trendfrom conventions with more iconic relationships to concrete structures of the physical world (such as more pictorial ancient landsurveying marks) to marking systems with less-iconic relationships to physical structures (that represent numbers, operations,infinity, and other more abstract concepts). We propose how certain constraints of perception-cognition induced conventionsthat made more-iconic (pictorial) marks controversial. These became too conceptually ambiguous to convey more abstractconceptual categories during the formalization of mathematics: Iconic properties of ancient proto-mathematical conventionsrecruited lower level perceptual capabilities developed to perceive-act in a concrete world of occluded surfaces-edges andwere suitable for conveying concrete structures (such as landforms during surveying). However, these were too conceptuallyambiguous to convey more abstract conceptual categories that emerged when mathematics was formalized because a (pictured)concrete structure can fall under many possible conceptual categories

ANCHORING is amodal: evidence from a signed language.

Modern linguistic theory posits the existence of universal constraints. But whether these constraints concern lan-guage structure, generally, or speech, specifically, is unknown. To address this question, here we ask whether the constraintsidentified in spoken languages transfer to sign languages. ANCHORING (McCarthy & Prince, 1993) is a putatively universalconstraint on reduplication. ANCHORING requires that the final element of a suffixed reduplicant match the final element ofthe base (e.g., pana ‘chase’––>panana, ‘run’ not panapa). Here, we examine whether ANCHORING is likewise operative in asigned language. In our experiments, native ASL signers rated novel reduplicated forms: either ones consistent or inconsistentwith ANCHORING (i.e., ABB vs. ABA, where A and B are syllables). Results showed that signers reliably favored ABB formsover ABA. These findings show for the first time that ANCHORING constrains a sign language. This conclusion is consistentwith the existence of amodal linguistic principles.

Don’t forget to bind: Memory binding and interference in development

This work investigates the development and causes of memory interference effects. Specifically, we measuredproactive and retroactive interference effects in children and adults when learning multiple sets of contingencies, as well asindividuals’ memory binding for the same contingencies. We measured proactive interference by examining memory for asecond set of contingencies after learning a first set, and retroactive interference by examining memory for the first set ofcontingencies after learning the second set. We measured memory binding by presenting participants with partial informationabout each contingency and measuring their accuracy and pattern of errors when asked to identify the completed contingency.Results indicate that both children and adults experienced substantial interference effects, but children were more prone tointerference and substantially worse at memory binding. Additionally, individuals’ memory binding abilities were predictiveof the magnitude of interference effects, suggesting that memory binding is an important mechanism modulating memoryinterference.

Developmental Changes in Visual Scene Statistics

Mature visual experience is tuned by inputs to the developing visual system. However, little is known about thelow-level statistics of available visual input as infants interact with the world in rapidly changing ways. Recent studies of thecontents of infant-perspective scenes (sampled from a corpus of over 5 million head camera images) indicate that these contentschange dramatically over the first year of life. Faces, ceilings, wall edges, and high-contrast patterns characterize younger babies(below 3 months), while more crowded images characterize older babies. These differences suggest possible developmentalchanges in lower-level visual statistics. After analyzing a sample of infant-perspective scenes from 4- to 10-week-old infants,and from 28- to 34-week-old infants, we found that mean Feature Congestion and Subband Entropy—measures of visual clutterin natural scenes—increase with age. The full analyses include spatial frequency, orientation, contrast, and clutter measuresacross 1,821,021 frames.

Speed and accuracy trade-off of semantic composition involving highlighting and

In a Speed-Accuracy-Tradeoff (SAT) paradigm we investigated how adjective type and polarity modulate the on-line semantic composition of noun phrases (NPs). 22 German speakers read sentences like “The tradesman — buys — areal diamond”. Enriched adjectives (“real/fake”) highlighted or adjusted the noun’s meaning, whereas non-enriched adjectives(“white/flawed”) simply specified a property. Adjectives had positive (“white/real”) or negative polarity (“flawed/fake”). Uponthe display of critical NPs, participants indicated by a series of key presses if the sentence was correct. For the SAT responsefunction we computed the (i) asymptote (response accuracy as d’), (ii) rate (response speed) and (iii) intercept (point when ac-curacy departs from chance). Accuracy was significantly lower for semantically enriched vs. non-enriched NPs, suggesting thathighlighting and adjusting certain properties during composition is costly. Polarity affected temporal dynamics with negativeNPs showing a slower rate than positive NPs, indicating that negative information is processed in more depth.

A Neurodynamical Model of How Prior Knowledge Influences Visual Perception

Recent behavioral studies showed that prior knowledge can directly influence visual perception. In the current work,we offer an explanation of the observed findings based on the adaptive resonance theory (ART). The ART neural network wasdesigned to solve the problem of catastrophic forgetting during learning in non-stationary environment. In the ART, stability oflearning is achieved by matching bottom-up sensory signals with top-down expectations. Resonant state that corresponds withconscious perception develops in the network when the bottom-up and top-down signals are closely aligned. On the other hand,mismatch produces global reset signal that clears the traces of erroneous top-down expectations. Therefore, prior knowledge caninfluence conscious perception only when it already closely matches with sensory signals. We performed computer simulationswith real-time implementation of the ART circuit that confirm our analysis. Simulations also showed how observed behavioralfindings arise from response bias.

Couples Emotion Dynamics During Conversations Involving Stress andEnjoyment

While conversing face-to-face, romantic partners are thought to form a coupled and co-regulatory system, uninten-tionally shaping each other’s emotional states on a moment-by-moment basis. What has been less explored, however, are theways in which this coupling is modulated by high-level interpersonal factors, such as discussing topics that are stressful forone or both partners. We provide an initial exploration by examining the emotion ratings of 42 romantic, heterosexual couplesduring conversations involving stress or enjoyment. Ratings were generated via continuous dials (sampled every second) asparticipants watched video playback of their interactions. The resulting time series were assessed for time-lagged patterns ofemotional coupling using cross recurrence quantification analysis. Initial results show that for topics that involved a mutualsense of stress or enjoyment, overall coupling was high, but this coupling was largely disrupted once the stress was moreasymmetrically experienced.

Information Signatures in Children’s Language Environment

In auditory statistical learning, children are sensitive to the transitional structure of their language environment. Vari-ability and stability of utterances in the language environment are important properties of statistical learning but are currentlyunderstudied across laboratory and naturalistic research contexts. In this study, we quantify variability and stability in the lan-guage environment of children as measured by amount of information within the temporal structure of caregivers’ utterances. Inthis work we present a new method for understanding information signatures in the temporal structure of parent-child free playcontexts and document information signatures of caregiver utterances at multiple timescales. Our results suggest informationsignatures of parental utterances increase across development (9-24 months), but decrease within individual play sessions (5-6minutes). We speculate that the dynamics of information signatures varies across multiple timescales. Possible implications ofthe observed information signatures inherent in caregivers’ naming of objects to their young children are explored.

The acquisition of verb morphology in Polish and Finnish: Model and experiment

Usage-based approaches suggest that language acquisition is a function of the statistical properties of the input. Wecompare predictions from neural network models with results of two elicited-production experiments on verb inflection withchildren in the morphologically complex languages Polish and Finnish. Three-layer neural networks were trained to produceperson/number-inflected present-tense verb forms in Polish and Finnish from phoneme representations of verb stems usingfrequency information from child-directed speech corpora. Simulated acquisition in both languages was affected by tokenfrequency and phonological neighbourhood density (PND) as well as an interaction such that low-frequency forms benefitedmore from PND than high-frequency forms. Suffix errors showed overgeneralisation and substitutions of low-frequency formswith higher-frequency forms. The model predictions are consistent with our empirical findings, except for the frequency XPND interaction. We discuss the experimental and simulated data and their implications.

What makes a joke funny: Analysing joke humor through single-word ratings.

The appreciation of humor is a universal phenomenon and a key aspect of cognition. It has been studied in thecontext of jokes, where the incongruity in expected and observed context results in the perception of humor. The present studyexamines how the humor appreciation of single words relates to the humor of the whole joke – is a joke simply a sum of itsparts? Using a novel dataset of single-word humor ratings, collections of jokes from the JESTER database were analyzed. Amultiple regression analysis showed joke length and individual word arousal were the best predictors of joke funniness. Longerjokes with fewer individually arousing words were found funnier. Individual word humor did not contribute to the humor ofthe overall joke. These findings suggest the cognitive aspects of humor are likely driven by broader semantic context, whereasappreciating humor on a per-word basis links to separate factors.

What’s on your wandering mind? The content of mind wandering during text-and film comprehension

What do we think about when our mind wanders? We asked 88 students to read an instructional text and watch afilm (each 20 minutes) and report whenever they found themselves zoning out. Each time they did, we asked them to reporttheir thoughts and what, if anything, triggered them. We then categorized these thoughts (1208 in total) based on their content,and found that in contrast with previous studies, only 17% involved prospection whereas 33% consisted of autobiographicaland semantic memory retrieval. This discrepancy might be driven by the rich content of stimuli: 71% of autobiographicaland semantic retrieval was explicitly triggered by the text or film, compared to 28% of prospection. Latent semantic analysisrevealed that memories were more similar to their triggers than prospective thoughts, suggesting that a substantial proportionof mind wandering is driven by the content of our environment.

Interaction with a robot changes human motor behavior

Social judgments about other people are often made based on visual appearance. In this study, we investigatedwhether visual appearance of an interaction partner influences action coordination in social interactions. In a novel interactiveaugmented reality setup participants interacted (i.e. carried out a high-five) with a life-sized 3D avatar that was either human-looking or robot-looking. Importantly, the kinematics of the avatars were identical for both appearances. We examined whethermotion trajectories of a high-five action and other motion trajectory parameters such as velocity, radial error, synchrony, andvariability were modulated by the visual appearance of the avatar. Results showed that participants carried out the high-fivefaster and applied different motion trajectories for the human-looking than for the robot-looking avatar. These findings suggestthat visual appearance does not only influence social judgments but also the immediate behavior towards the interaction partner.

The differential effects of transmission and interaction on linguistic variation

Variation in natural language is constrained: languages tend to lose competing variants over time, and where vari-ation persists, its use tends to be conditioned on grammatical or sociolinguistic context. We had adult participants learn andcommunicate with artificial languages exhibiting unpredictable variation in plural marking. Using an iterated learning pro-cedure, the languages produced by participants were used as training languages for other participants. We passed on eitherthe language produced during a post-training recall test (Recall condition) or the language used while communicating withanother participant (Interaction condition). We found that alignment during interaction leads to the elimination of variability:in Interaction chains, one plural marker typically came to dominate. However, in Recall chains, variation became conditionedon linguistic context, rather than being eliminated. This suggests that the pattern of restricted, conditioned variation in naturallanguage reflects the combined influences of biases in learning, recall and interaction.

Production of morphologically complex words as revealed by a typing task:Morphological influences on keystroke dynamics

In a production by typing task, with extraneous factors (e.g., length) controlled, measures such as latency to initialkeystroke as well as mean inter keystroke interval typically vary systematically according to the word’s lexical properties.Conventionally, lexical effects in production tasks get interpreted as evidence of cascaded processing between central andperipheral levels. We compare mean and distribution of keystroke latencies within the same stem as it undergoes affixation insets such as DEPRESS, DEPRESSION, DEPRESSIVE. Novel is the comparison of stems that differ with respect to number ofaffixes like SUPER, SUPERIOR, SUPERIORITY. Results provide new insights into the ways in which morphological structurecan influence purportedly peripheral motor processing.

Probability matching as a cognitive basis of cultural drift

In the field of cultural evolution, cognitive agents are either seen as perfect imitators who reproduce cultural variantsveridically (e.g. Boyd & Richerson 1985) or as imperfect imitators who transform the variants as they replicate them (e.g.Sperber 1996). In this poster, I explain how the transformative view of cognition applies to not only to the generation ofvariants, but also to the way we learn frequency distributions. Probability matching is a widely-observed human behaviorwhere learners reproduce a frequency distribution over variants with a small amount of error and is equivalent to Wright-Fisher drift when the variance in error is binomial/multinomial. However, humans and learning algorithms can produce errordistributions that are non-binomial/non-multinomial, which constitute a broader class of drift processes than those that exist ingenetic evolution or in perfect-imitator models of cultural evolution.

Is gender-fair langauge needed? How grammatical gender influencesrepresentations of discourse referents

The use of gender-fair language is an important measure to boost gender equality. However, there is wide-spreadscepticism as to the usefulness of avoiding male bias in language, even in gendered languages. For instance, in German allnouns carry grammatical gender, and role names are considered generic, even when their gender is masculine. We used asentence-picture matching task to test whether male references in language induce gendered representations. After presentinga sentence with a role name, a picture of a person was shown. In 48 trials, the factors gender of the role name (masculinevs. feminine) and sex of the person in the picture (woman vs. man) were crossed. The results of 40 participants showed thatwomen after masculine referents were more readily accepted than men after feminine referents, but reaction times increased.Thus, readers interpret some masculine forms as generic, but only with considerable cognitive effort.

Introducing a New JavaScript Framework for Professional Online Studies.

New possibilities such as online crowdsourcing (Amazon Mechanical Turk), open data repositories (Open ScienceFramework), and online analysis (Ipython notebook) offer rich possibilities to improve, validate, and speed up research. How-ever, until today there is no cross-platform integration of these subsystems. Furthermore, implementation of online studies stillsuffers from the complex implementation (server infrastructure, database programming, security considerations etc.). Here wepresent LabVanced, a JavaScript framework that constitutes methodological innovation by combining three essential aspects foronline research. With our framework studies can be implemented in an intuitive graphical user interface without programming.Second, the framework takes care about participant recruitment and third, it outlines options for data visualizations and statisti-cal analysis. Additionally, the framework can be used for sharing not only the recorded data, but also the study design and theanalysis. In summary, we introduce a new powerful JavaScript framework for improving and accelerating online research.

How to communicate uncertainty in severe weather forecasts?

Communicating uncertainty to lay audiences is as challenging as indispensible if people are to understand medicaltest results, gains from financial investments, or weather warnings.Compared to risk communication in the medical domain, there is so far only limited evidence on how to best communicateuncertainty for continuous quantities, such as financial returns or wind speeds (Spiegelhalter et al., 2011).The poster presents results from a longitudinal study investigating this question within a real-life setting. We implementeddifferent representations communicating probabilistic weather forecasts within an online information system operated by theGerman National Weather Service. The system is used by fire brigade coordination centers throughout Germany to prepare forsevere weather conditions.By analyzing web usage and search behavior, we investigate which representations users rely upon under real operationalconstraints. We link the analysis to tests which representations are best understood and could thus aid emergency managers intheir decisions.

Developing Visual Closure in Infancy

Visual closure is the ability to complete a picture from partial information. In children it is a requisite in manyskills such as fluent reading, and is also used in many tests of colour vision. Here we present a test of visual closure in infantsacross two age groups, 1;4 and 1;7, both to test their abilities in visual closure, but also as a prototype for a colour vision testfor younger infants. The results of the study show evidence of development in visual closure abilities across those two ages,suggesting that visual closure is a perceptual ability that continues to develop in the second year of life. The results of this studyare discussed in terms of perceptual development in infants and toddlers, and have consequences for both medical and scientificunderstanding of visual closure in children.

Executive Functions and Academic Achievement in a High-Poverty Sample

Research exploring cognitive theories of executive function (EF) report positive associations with academic out-comes, but whether such general cognitive theories generalise to when children are exposed to social or economic povertycontexts require more in-depth investigating. This study explores associations between EF and academic achievement for anethnic minority sample aged 8–10 years, from high poverty, urban backgrounds. EF skills were measured using stop-signal (in-hibition), continuous performance (sustained attention), task-switching (cognitive flexibility), spatial span (working memory)and Tower of Hanoi (planning). In addition, we included a popular standardized test of academic ability commonly used byschools to measure literacy, numeracy and science skills and the Raven’s Progressive Matrices task to measure general cognitiveability. EF is differentiated in this sample and is linked to academic achievement. The role of important mediators like cognitiveability are considered in the context of children with high-poverty urban backgrounds.

Neural Phase Synchrony on Understanding Meanings of Symbols

The establishment of symbolic communication system, i.e., making a shared meaning system from meaninglesssignals, is studied in experimental semiotics (Galantucci, 2005). Local neural activities within a brain region during a symboliccommunication task (Konno et al., 2013), where two participants try to establish a symbolic communication system fromscratch, has been studied (Li et al, 2015). It is, however, not certain how information bindings between different brain regionsis involved in a cognitive process associated with the establishment process. We analyzed EEG phase synchronization, as ameasure of functional connectivity, of participants engaged in the symbolic communication task. We found the recruitmentof fronto-occipital synchronization at 40 Hz frequency (gamma band), when a symbolic message was displayed, became fastwhen establishing a symbolic communication system. This finding suggests that frontal-occipital information binding by phasesynchronization becomes efficiently used in the course of mutual understanding of symbolic messages.

Jumping in Japanese: Converting linguistic instructions into physicalperformances

This study explores the difficulties in physically realizing linguistic instructions concerning the action of jumping.We carried out a questionnaire on the understandability and physical feasibility of various jumping actions in Japanese, andthen conducted an experiment in which participants were asked to jump according to these instructions. After the physicalperformances the participants were asked to rate the easiness of the actions in a second questionnaire, and the results of thetwo questionnaires were compared. The results show that the understandability of the instructions and the participants’ beliefsabout the physical feasibility of the instructions were closely correlated. However, the results of the two questionnaires did notcorrelate. The results suggest that although participants believe they can convert jump instructions into physical performancesif the instructions are easy to understand, there are some gaps between the understandability of the linguistic instructions andthe physical realization of them.

The Price of Fear: Developing a behavioural assessment of fear-related avoidanceincorporating dynamic response measures.

In economics, “willingness to pay” reflects subjective value which has been employed to price goods, and morerecently, negative outcomes. The current project proposes a protocol for the behavioural assessment of fear-related avoidancebased on how much an individual is willing to pay to avoid their fears.The proposed protocol consists of a “card game” interface in which participants make choices in several stages. Duringbaseline, participants chose between two decks that provide differential point rewards. Across a series of experimental blocks,feared stimuli (e.g. a spider image) were presented in addition to rewards when the richer deck was chosen. Rewards were thenmanipulated, in a staircase fashion, to establish the value of the feared stimulus. Mouse and eye movements were tracked inan attempt to track cognitive processes during decision-making and avoidance. Preliminary results indicate sensitivity of theprotocol, and strengths and weaknesses will be discussed.

On the road to . . . somewhere? Change-blindness in event description tasks isinformative about the interrelation between visual perception and languageplanning

The visual processing of complex event stimuli and the planning of utterances to describe them happen rapidlyand partly overlap in time, posing a challenge to researchers on vision and language: How exactly do the processes interact?As a test case we investigate how sudden content-changes in visual scenes affect speakers of different languages. In a novelapproach, we elicit event descriptions from naturalistic video stimuli of motion events consisting of two segments (240ms each),each followed by a mask (80ms). A potential change-blindness situation regarding the presence/absence of the goal of motionis created. We exploit typological differences between French and German regarding the verbal encoding of goal-orientation.Analyses of the linguistic data (content and timing) reveals a language-specific effect regarding how subjects accommodate toseemingly unnoticed changes (e.g., distribution of hesitations, temporal onsets of words). Furthermore, we find differences inovert change detection frequency depending on conditions.

Mental computations underlying morphosyntax acquisition

Research in theoretical linguistics has shown that human languages require abstract and highly detailed grammaticalrepresentations. However, we understand surprisingly little about the mechanisms through which these representations areacquired. What kinds of statistical relationships would learners need to compute to construct representations like those positedby linguistic theory? We created miniature languages containing patterns found in natural languages and also patterns notfound in natural languages. We showed that complex word-order contingencies are acquired only when they correlate withmorphological patterns like those in natural languages. We then asked how learning changes when the statistical evidence forthese patterns is manipulated. These experiments illuminate the nature of learners’ computations and the units over which theyare performed.

Computational Modelling of Embodied Semantic Cognition: A Deep LearningApproach

perceptual symbol systems hypothesis describes how semantic knowledge is grounded insensorimotor experience. According to the theory, knowledge is acquired through sensorimotor simulations. This challengesthe classical view supported by the disembodied cognition hypothesis, which generally favours an abstract and symbolic sys-tem. We propose a unified perspective, in which, the embodied cognition hypothesis, with a particular focus on the semanticdomain, is provided with a mechanistically tractable computational framework based on the parallel distributed processing(PDP) paradigm. A critical difference between the current approach and previous mechanistic accounts of embodied cognitionis that the current approach avoids using hand-coded representations and instead, relies on an agent-based simulation withenvironmental interaction for the creation of situated inputs and outputs, supplemented with supervised and unsupervised deeplearning mechanisms, from which semantic cognition emerges.

The Perceived Duration of Vast Spaces

Experiencing awe may make us believe we have more time (Rudd, Vohs, & Aaker, 2012). Awe can be evoked byencountering a vast experience (Keltner & Haidt, 2003), for example an endless ocean or large mountains (Klatzky, Thompson,Stefanucci, Gill, & McGee, 2017). Vast environments may lead to distortions in perceived time that are reported after awe expe-riences. Participants reproduced the perceived duration of images of natural environments that varied in vastness and estimatedthe degree awe they would experience in those spaces. Results show that as actual duration increased, perceived duration of theimage decreased, whereas estimated awe increased. The perceived duration of highly vast images was underestimated less thanother images. Participants reported they would experience more awe in highly vast images compared to low and medium vastimages. These findings suggest that distortions of time associated with awe may be related to the vastness of the environment.

The role of spatial skills in mathematics cognition: Evidence from children aged5-10 years

While there is evidence of associations between spatial skills and mathematics, relatively few studies explore theseassociations in children aged 5-10 years. I will present findings from longitudinal and cross-sectional studies to highlight theimportance of spatial skills as both longitudinal and concurrent predictors of mathematics. First, secondary data analysis of theMillennium Cohort Study indicates that spatial performance at both 5 and 7 years is a significant predictor of mathematics at age7 (N = 12099). Second, cross-sectional findings from children aged 5-10 years (N=156), suggest that spatial skills explain 10-12% of the variation in standardised maths performance and approximate number sense, even after accounting for vocabularyskills. That is, spatial scaling was a significant predictor of mathematics for all age groups, while the role of mental rotationand mental folding varied with age. These findings have implications for the design of mathematics interventions customisedfor specific developmental stages.

Investigating the predictions of a memory-based account of statistical learning

Statistical learning (SL) refers to the ability to extract statistical regularities from the environment. Many researchersbelieve that SL arises as a consequence of the way that information is stored and accessed in memory (Thiessen, Kronstein,& Hufnagle, 2013). Accordingly, manipulations that influence memory should have similar effects in SL experiments. In thecurrent study, participants were presented with artificial languages that varied along two dimensions known to impact memory:number of distractors in the input and timing of presentation (e.g., spaced vs. massed). Participants’ performance was clearlyinfluenced by these manipulations; for example, the ability to segment a word (e.g., ”dupona”) differed as a function of whetherthere was one frequent competitor (e.g., ”dugalo”) or several less frequent competitors (e.g., ”dugalo,” ”dufalu,” ”dumiso”).Experimental results were compared to two memory-based computational models of SL (PARSER and TRACX). Implicationsof the experimental results and model comparisons will be discussed.

Motor Fluency Effects on Causal Judgment: The Role of Grip-StrengthAsymmetries and Spatial-Numeric Associations

Human understanding of causation may be grounded in our experience of physical forces in the world. We investi-gated whether right-handers, who exert greater force with their right than left hands, judge candidate causes on the right sideas more causal. In two experiments, subjects simultaneously learned about a moderately effective and an ineffective causeon a trial-by-trial basis. Subjects rated the moderately effective cause as more causal when it appeared on the right side ofspace. This effect was also present in subjects’ trial-by-trial predictions, but the effect reversed with a left-right reversal in thespatial-numeric mapping of the causal judgment scale. The results are not consistent with the notion that our understanding ofcausation is grounded in our ability to exert force. However, they are consistent with influences of motor fluency and polaritycorrespondence on judgment.

Comparison of directed gaze during vocalizations in bonobo and human infants

A crucial step in language evolution was likely joint attention with alternating gaze between vocalizing individualsand an object. This triadic interaction likely formed a foundation for labeling of objects. We have argued that vocalizationsused for “social glue” – flexible low intensity and low arousal vocalizations given during e.g. grooming, keeping in contact withthe group, etc. – are a probable source of raw material for first labels. It is critical that these vocalizations be socially directed,by gaze contact. We longitudinally investigated directed gaze during vocalizations in low arousal interactions during the firstyear in three bonobo mother-infant pairs and compared them with 9 human mother-infant pairs. We found that bonobo infantsdirected their gaze to a conspecific during vocalizations only 8% of the time while human infants directed it 44% of the time.

How does of initial inaccuracy benefit cross-situational word learning?

Both children and adults are able to extract several intended word-referent mappings from a series of scenes con-taining multiple words and objects. Known as cross-situational learning, this ability is thought to be an important means ofacquiring language. Proposed models of this ability range from hypothesis-testing accounts to associative accounts, but mostformal models assume learners store one or more feasible word-referent mappings per experience, and that the correct map-pings emerge through consistent co-occurrence. These theories would all predict that presenting unambiguous evidence for acorrect pair would benefit learning, but recent evidence indicates the reverse is true: giving unambiguous evidence for incorrectpairs improves subsequent cross-situational learning (Fitneva and Christiansen, 2015). With some nuances, we replicate theseresults, and show why future models may need to include an error-driven learning mechanism to explain word learning.

On the Detection of “Alternative Facts” in Environmental Messages: The Effectsof a Sequential versus a Simultaneous Presentation Format

Reasonable rational information processing is important in people’s in everyday decision-making. A number offeatures affect how environmental messages are processed, including the presentation format and the reliability of the informa-tion source. One way to measure the importance assigned to the source reliability is to frame the question in terms of Bayes’theorem (Hahn & Harris, 2009). In two online experiments, we investigated how people process environmental messages in aBayesian integration task where the participants rate the probability of an energy crisis. The information about the prior, like-lihood ratio, and source reliability were presented either sequentially or simultaneously. The results showed that, as prescribedby Bayes’ theorem, participants integrated the sentences multiplicatively. However, with sequential presentation they assignedmore weight to source reliability, and this effect remained when the source reliability was presented next to last, suggesting thatparticipants assigned more weight to the source regardless of its position.

Exploring inductive bias of visual scenes

When people encode a representation of a scene, they do not necessarily represent the exact locations and orientationsof the constituent elements. Instead, people rely on preexisting inductive biases to simplify their encoding of new sceneconfigurations. We investigated people’s inductive biases in their memory for configurations of simple 2D shapes (such ascircles, triangles, etc.) using a serial reproduction paradigm (Bartlett, 1932). This paradigm establishes an iterative process inwhich information is transmitted through a chain of people (like the ”telephone” game). In our experiment, we asked peopleto memorize configurations of simple shapes (which were either generated at random or by other participants) and then askedthem to reproduce those configurations. In analyzing the final generation of reproductions, we found that people have strongpreferences for the scale of individual shapes, as well as the alignment, distance, overlap, and relative rotation between pairs ofshapes.

Choosing while Losing: The Effects of Valence and Relative Magnitude onDecision Dynamics.

Framing decision options as gains or as losses affects how we evaluate those options. The current study assessedthe effects of gain- and loss-framing on the acquisition of outcome values across decisions and on the dynamics of computermouse responses to those decisions. In a series of 36 decisions per block, four arbitrary symbols were presented, two of whichwere assigned high points (e.g., 20) and two of which were assigned low points (e.g., 5). Participants (N=86) learned to choosehigh values and avoid low values when values were positive and to choose low values and avoid high values when they werenegative. Loss-framed outcomes (i.e., negative valence) were learned faster and more reliably. Response trajectories followingacquisition were slower, more curved and exhibited greater vacillation when choosing between two poor outcomes. Theseeffects were stronger when poor outcomes were negatively valenced.

The Relationship between Anxiety, Mind Wandering and Task-switching: ADiffusion Model Analysis

The current study aims to examine the mechanisms underlying the negative impact of anxiety on task-switching. Todo so, we employed a stochastic diffusion model analysis along with a thought-probe technique in task-switching paradigm.Across 152 participants, we found state anxiety was associated to higher switch costs in nondecision time but not drift rateparameter of diffusion model, implying that the locus of task-switching impairment in anxious individuals is pertinent to theefficiency of task-set reconfiguration but not proactive interference processes. Furthermore, we found boundary separationparameter – which quantifies conservative decisional styles – heightened as a function of anxiety, supporting the existence ofcompensatory strategy in anxious individuals. Lastly, we found that impaired performance by anxiety was not attributed tothe frequency of worrisome thoughts during task-switching. These findings elucidate several theoretical assumptions on therelationship between anxiety and task-switching.

Do people behave dishonestly easily?

This study examines whether dishonest behaviors occur easily. In 60 trials, 100 undergraduate students viewed20 dots on a square divided into right and left sides and had to decide which side contained more dots within one second(developed by Gino et al., 2010). In with-reward condition, participants received 0.1 point for each left decision and 1 pointfor each right decision, and they received more sweets depending on points. Therefore, this asymmetrical payment structuretriggered motivation to dishonestly report more right-side dots, even when there are actually more left-side dots. The resultsdemonstrate that participants decided at greater frequencies that more dots were on the right side in with-reward conditionthan in without-reward condition, indicating dishonest behaviors occurred. Furthermore, participants with greater right-sidefrequencies in with-reward condition showed lower points on a morality scale. These results suggest dishonest behaviors occureasily and are related with a decline in morality.

Hierarchical Processing of Response Production and Categorisation

Early research on categorisation suggested that verbalizable and nonverbalizable category-learning are qualitativelydifferent. Toward this end, the implementational-level model (COVIS–COmpetition between Verbal and Implicit Systems) ofcategorisation assumes that category-learning involves separate but parallel sub-systems. Specifically, for verbalizable tasksabstract category-labels are learned by a hypothesis-testing sub-system, while for nonvertbalizable tasks response position islearned by a procedural-learning sub-system. However, recent research has revealed that: 1) regardless of category structure,reversal learning is easier than learning novel categories; 2) qualitative difference between verbalizable and nonverbalizabletasks disappears when automaticity has developed; and 3) control of automatic categorisation is different from both proposedsub-systems. These challenges suggest a fundamental revision of the mechanisms of categorisation. Contrary to the separate,parallel-processing sub-systems theory, we argue that categorisation involves hierarchical-processing sub-systems of response-production and category-label association. This framework, when combined with Supervisory Attentional System theory, mayfacilitate the unification of human categorisation.

Where are you? The Effect of Uncertainty and its Visual Representation onLocation Judgments in GPS-like Displays

Two experiments revealed how non-experts interpret visualizations of positional uncertainty on GPS-like displaysand how the visual representation of uncertainty affects their judgments. Participants were shown maps with representations oftheir current location; locational uncertainty was visualized as either a circle (confidence interval) or a faded glyph (indicatingthe probability density function directly). When shown a single circle or faded glyph, participants assumed they were locatedat the center of the uncertain region. In a task that required combining two uncertain estimates of their location, the mostcommon strategy – integration – was to take both estimates into account, with more weight given to the more certain estimate.Participants’ strategies were not affected by how uncertainty was visualized, but visualization affected the consistency of theirresponses. The results indicate that non-experts have an intuitive understanding of uncertainty and that the best visualizationmethod is task dependent.

Semantic Bootstrapping in Frames: A Computational Model of SyntacticCategory Acquisition

Semantic Bootstrapping in Frames: A Computational Model of Syntactic Category AcquisitionAccording to the semantic bootstrapping hypothesis, children map certain (prototypical) semantic concepts to syntacticcategories (e.g., objects as nouns, actions as verbs), which are then used to learn other elements of language. We report acomputational model of syntactic category acquisition that combines semantic bootstrapping with the distributional learning oflanguage. The model has access to a small set of “seed” words, with labeled categories. It then iteratively constructs syntacticframes from the seeds; sufficiently frequent frames are used to categorize non-seeded words which then contribute to theconstruction of additional frames, including frames that incorporate category information. The model is online and effective.Simulation on child-directed English corpus shows that with only 100 seed words, classification precision exceeds 70%.

Tangible rhythm: Sensorimotor representations of metrical structure and learningmusical rhythm with gesture

When we listen to music, we can mentally control how we perceive the beat. This ability is thought to be subserved bysensorimotor imagery, having top-down effects on attentional-allocation and perception. Here, we examine whether imagined“up and down” gestures can support an internal generation of metrical accent in rhythmic sequences. We also examine howthis type of motor imagery interacts with either metrically congruent or incongruent auditory imagery. This is explored usingEEG with a frequency-tagging approach, quantifying the strength of metrical accent with the amplitude of beat-related SSEPs.Gesture supports our ability to think and learn by fostering an alignment between sensorimotor representations and moreabstract conceptual structure. Therefore, the imagined gestures may act as a bridge between perceptual and action-orientedunderstandings of metrical structure and the more abstract conceptual ones that musicians struggle with in their training. Theseimagery strategies may then be beneficial to music education.

Eye movement-based probabilistic models for physical scene understanding

Humans make prediction about physical environments and future events through inference. Previous research hasproposed that a common sense engine implementing probabilistic programming is used to build an internal model of theenvironment, and simulations of that internal model are used for inferences. Battaglia et al.(2013) have demonstrated anapplication of this formulation in physical scene understanding and stability judgment in the case of block tower. Here weaugment this formulation by including the subjects’ eye movements as a process of sampling the environment, and propose thatthe underlying common sense model guides gaze toward sampling the features of the space with relevant information for thejudgments about stability. We compare a base probabilistic model with one that takes the statistics of the saccades into account,and argue that the additional information improves the model predictions about subjects’ judgment.

Data Driven Eye Gaze Path Segmentation

The first stage of analyzing eye-tracking data is commonly to code the data into sequences of fixations and saccades.This is usually automated using simple, predetermined rules for classifying ranges of the time series into events, such as “ifthe dispersion of gaze samples is lower than the threshold, then code as a fixation.” More recent approaches incorporateadditional eye-movement categories in automated parsing algorithms, particularly glissades, by using time-varying, data-driventhresholds. We describe an alternative approach using the beta-process auto-regressive hidden Markov model (BP-AR HMM).The BP-AR-HMM offers two main advantages over existing frameworks. First, it provides a statistical model for eye movementclassification rather than a single estimate. Second, the BP-AR-HMM uses a latent process to model the number and natureof the types of eye-movements and hence is not constrained to predetermined categories. We present comparisons betweenBP-AR-HMM parsing and standard analyses on multiple datasets.

Case Markers Facilitate Abstraction of Syntax among Mandarin-speakingpreschoolers

In light of the studies that investigate when and how English-speaking young children exhibit adult-like abstractionof syntax, this study explores these issues by examining Mandarin-speaking preschoolers ranging from 2- to 5-year-olds’comprehension of the Mandarin SVO-, ba-, long-passive, and short-passive constructions using the forced-choice pointingparadigm. The results indicated that at the age of 2, Mandarin preschoolers exhibited abstraction of syntax in these fourconstructions. These results went against the predictions of accounts derived from the structure mapping account and from thecompetition model. Instead, Mandarin ba- (used in the ba-construction) and bei-markers (used in the long- and short-passiveconstructions) play an important role in Mandarin-speaking young children’s demonstrations of abstraction of syntax.

An Exploratory Study on Remote Associates Problem Solving: Evidence of EyeMovement Indicators

Remote associates problems (RAP) have been widely used to measure creative processes. However, studies haverarely explored the RAP processes. The main purpose of this study was to record eye movements while solving RAP. Theresults show that: (1) The mean fixation duration increases throughout the problem-solving process, which indicates that moreproblem solvers encounter impasses. This result supports the “impasse encounter” phase of insight. (2) During the initial periodof problem solving, individuals display more regression counts in the fixation region than in the key region, which supportsthat the impasses are caused by inappropriate initial representation. (3) During the middle period of the process, the timeindividuals spend gazing at the key region increases, while the time spend at the fixation region decreases. This supports the“impasse resolution and insight” phase of insight.

Processing of Filler-gap Dependency in Island Constraints and its Relation toWorking Memory for Non-native Speakers of English

Whether non-native speakers’ on-line processing can be native-like remains a hot issue. Recently many have shownthat qualitatively native-like processing is attainable, especially for learners with high proficiency. However, most of the studiesrecruited learners who had been immersed to the English-speaking country. The current study investigated processing of filler-gap dependency and island constraints for Chinese learners of English as a second but foreign language. We also attempted tolook into individual differences by taking different variables into account. The results showed that native-like active gap-fillingstrategy positively correlated with L2 proficiency, native-like island effect negatively correlated with age of acquisition, butneither one correlated with working memory capacity. These findings lent more support to the grammar-based account forisland effects, though future studies adopting more precise measure of working memory would be needed. The study alsocalled for further investigation into L1 background on processing islands in an L2.

Linguistic processes in translation: Eye-tracking reveals differential effects ofphrase order and lexical choice

What are the processes underlying the judgments of translation? And what is the role of language proficiency?This study addresses these questions by examining how Chinese-English bilinguals evaluate poetry translations. Participantswere shown haikus in Chinese and the corresponding English translations and were asked to rate the translation quality. TheEnglish translations ranged from literal to free style and differed in two source text factors — phrase order and lexical choice.Results indicated an interaction between translation style and language proficiency, with the high proficiency bilinguals givingfree translations higher ratings. Furthermore, the analyses of eye movements revealed that, (a) in contrast to low proficiencybilinguals, high proficiency bilinguals tended to integrate discourse information regardless of intra-text re-ordering, and (b)among the good quality translations, the phrase order effect was more prominent than the lexical effect. These findings suggestthe interplaying roles of language proficiency and linguistic factors in translation.

How reactivation strength affects memory updating

Memory reactivation induces plasticity, rendering reactivated memories susceptible to interference. The currentstudy examined whether the method and strength of reactivation modulates retroactive interference effects. Two days afterlearning AB word pairs, memory for these pairs was either not reactivated, moderately reactivated (presentation of A cues in anunrelated task), or strongly reactivated (restudy of AB pairs or cued recall of B targets). Immediately afterwards, participantseither learned AC word pairs, DE word pairs, or performed an unrelated distractor task. Cued recall of target words wastested two days later. Strong reactivation before learning new material protected memory from retroactive interference andintrusions, whereas moderate reactivation resulted in both. This finding suggests that strong reactivation enhances event-baseddistinctiveness, counteracting memory modification. Results are discussed in reference to the testing effect literature and thereconsolidation account, and implications for educational practice are outlined.

An fNIRS Hyperscanning Study on Brain-Brain Interactions of a Dyad during aJoint Sentence Reading Task

Existing studies in cognitive neuroscience predominantly focus on a single participant’s behavioral and brain re-sponses. Lack of an interactive context for joint action particularly limited social neuroscience studies to simulated socialcontexts. Advances in portable brain imaging technologies have made it practical to simultaneously monitor the brain activityof two or more people in an interactive context to investigate neural correlates of social interaction. In this study, the rela-tionship between behavioral synchrony and inter-brain coherence is investigated during simultaneous reading of matching andmismatching sentences in different auditory conditions. A dual-fNIRS hyperscanning setup was used to obtain simultaneousrecordings of hemodynamic activity from the prefrontal cortices of the participants while they jointly read-aloud the sentencesdisplayed on their screens. The results suggest that the level of inter-brain coherence in the right superior cortex tends toincrease depending on the level of behavioral synchrony among the participants.

Pre-term infants exhibit impaired prediction and learning in Audio-Visualassociation paradigm

Forming reliable predictions about upcoming events are both essential to and the product of successful learning.Using fNIRS recording of cortical hemodynamics, we measured infants’ prediction of upcoming visual events that were pre-ceded by auditory cues in infants who are at-risk for poor development due to premature birth and their full-term peers. Wecompared prediction and learning across groups by fitting their occipital cortex response (which we assumed to reflect themagnitude of the prediction error) to a reinforcement learning model with a dynamic learning rate. We found that preemies hada lower learning rate than full-terms. These findings shed light on the origins of the developmental difficulties associated withprematurity.

Endpoints and Midpoints in Event Perception

Events unfold over time, i.e., they have a beginning and endpoint. Previous studies have illustrated the importance ofendpoints for the perception and memory of various events (Lakusta & Landau, 2005, 2012; Papafragou, 2010; Regier & Zheng,2009; Strickland & Keil, 2011; Zacks & Swallow, 2007). However, this work has not compared endpoints to other potentiallysalient points in the internal temporal profile of events (e.g., midpoints). Building on the “picky puppet task” (Waxman &Gelman, 1986), we presented 4-to-5-year-old children and adults with a puppet that liked clips of events containing brief screenblanks that disrupted either the midpoint or the endpoint of the event. Both children and adults learned the puppet’s preferencesbetter (as evidenced by their extension to novel events) when the puppet liked midpoint compared to endpoint interruptions.These findings suggest a bias for event endpoints that is present from an early age.

Dynamic and multiplexed networks for working memory

Working memory (WM) provides the neurobiological infrastructure for human cognition. Dominant models positthat prefrontal cortex (PFC) supports WM by coordinating control over distributed memory representations. In two studies,multimodal electrophysiology data reveal that PFC control over WM is rhythmic, fundamentally dynamic, and not altogethernecessary. Direct brain recordings (n=10) demonstrate that PFC and medial temporal lobe (MTL) theta-band rhythms directa complex system of higher-frequency neural activity across regions, uncovering initial support for bidirectional PFC-MTLinteractions related to WM demands. Then, data from patients with unilateral PFC damage (n=14) challenge dominant modelson the central role of PFC (note 8% accuracy decrease in patients). In healthy controls (n=20), delta-theta-band rhythms precessfrom PFC toward parieto-occipital sites, concurrent with alpha-beta-band rhythms precessing in the opposite direction. All PFCeffects are diminished with unilateral damage, revealing an independent posterior WM mechanism. These results reveal thatrapid, parallel processing governs WM.

Improving Perceptual Reasoning in School Children through Chess Training

Perceptual reasoning is the ability that incorporates fluid reasoning, spatial processing, and visual motor integration.Several theories of cognitive functioning emphasize the importance of fluid reasoning. Tasks that require fluid reasoning involvethe process of manipulating abstractions, rules, generalizations, and logical relationships. A pretest–posttest with control groupdesign was used, with 43 (28 boys, 15 girls) children in the experimental group and 42 (26 boys, 16 girls) children in thecontrol group. The sample was selected from children studying in two private schools from South India, which includedboth the genders. The experimental group underwent weekly one-hour chess training for one year. Perceptual reasoning wasmeasured by three subtests of WISC-IV INDIA. Pre-equivalence of means was established. Statistical analyses revealed thatthe experimental group shows statistically significant improvement in perceptual reasoning compared to the control group. Thepresent study establishes a correlation between chess learning and perceptual reasoning.

Measuring Demand Avoidance with the Demand Selection Task: Challenges andOpportunities

When given the chance to choose between two tasks, one will more likely choose the easier, less demanding task.This effect has been shown in various domains and referred to as the law of minimum effort or demand avoidance. Kooland colleagues (2010, 2013) designed the demand selection task (DST) and showed that most of their participants exhibitedclear demand avoidance. We attempted to replicate and extend their results in a series of three studies. Here we argue thatDST confounds demand detection and demand selection, which weakens its ability to reliably measure demand avoidance indifferent populations. In our first study, most participants did not show reliable demand avoidance and those who showed ithad higher working memory capacity. The following two studies aimed to de-confound the two processes. We define a newmeasure of demand avoidance that affords a more robust estimation of demand avoidance in different populations.

Optimizing Mathematic Learning: Effects of Continuous and Nominal PracticeFormat on Transfer of Arithmetic Skills

Should we give learners a lot of practice with a few problems, or a little practice with a variety of problems? Thebest practice set depends on the way people are learning. We describe two models people employ when learning arithmeticproblems. We show that features of the task environment influence model use. When problems are presented in a purelysymbolic format, people learn an item-specific model. When the task format linked problems to representations of magnitudes,people learn a continuous model. We also test the effects of different practice sets on learning. In both formats people learnedthe practice sets well with a few repeated examples. With a continuous magnitude format people showed better transfer with awide variety of practice problems. Variety led to poor learning in the symbolic format. In ongoing research we are attemptingto identify the optimal practice set for each type of learning model.

Priors, informative cues and ambiguity aversion

Ambiguity aversion, or the preference for options with known rather than unknown probabilities, is a robust findingwithin the decision making literature (see Camerer & Weber, 1992, for a review). There are some suggestions this aversenessis due differences in the inferred prior distribution (G ̈uney & Newell, 2015). In this study we investigated the relationshipbetween prior distributions and information cues on decision making and participants’ judgments of underlying distribution.We used three different prior cues; a positive underlying distributional cue, a negative underlying distributional cue, and aneutral cue. We also used five different information cues which varied both the bias of the information and the degree ofambiguity. Whilst we found that both prior and information manipulations had the expected impact for participants’ judgmentsof underlying distributions, they only impacted the decisions participants made in some cases. There were also interestinginteractions between the prior and information manipulations.

Moral Judgments in Trolley Like Dilemmas: An Eye-Tracking Study

Previous research suggests that participants may be susceptible to confirmation bias after making decisions in moraldilemmas. We manipulated the type of moral dilemmas (personal or impersonal) and the framing of the question promptingparticipants to respond (emphasizing saving five people or sacrificing one person). The actors in the dilemmas were representedby a series of silhouettes. Eye tracking data revealed that both manipulations had an effect on participants’ gaze. Furtheranalysis of utilitarian choices has shown that there were no framing effects of the prompting question when the dilemmas wereimpersonal. The data suggests that participants’ subsequent gaze patterns are sensitive to both how the situation is describedand the framing of their hypothetical actions. Taken together, our results provide some support to the claim that confirmationbias may arise after making moral decisions.

Word retrieval decline in midlife: a voxel-based morphometry study

There is currently little understanding on whether significant word retrieval difficulties appear during midlife and ifso, whether they relate to decrease in grey matter (GM) density that accompanies aging. To answer this question, we studiedretrieval of proper names in 125 cognitively healthy middle-aged persons (49.7, ±3.2) comparing their performance on atip-of-the-tongue (TOT) task with that of 86 young persons (25.4, ±3.5) from the Cam-Can data repository (http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/datasets/camcan/). The middle age (MA) group was worse in word retrieval (U = 23950.5, p = 0.003) and hadless GM volume in a range of left fronto-temporal areas relative to the young group, but there were no statistically significantcorrelations between volumes of the regions known to be implicated in word retrieval and MA’s TOT scores. Thus, midlifeword retrieval decline is not associated with GM volume reduction; more likely it is due to changes in connectivity.

The relationship between verbal route descriptions and personal characteristics ofempathy

Empathy is important for good verbal navigation instructions. The present study examined the relationship betweenverbal route descriptions and personal characteristics of empathy using a verbal navigation task and the Japanese InterpersonalReactivity Index (IRI-J). In the verbal navigation task, participants were presented a maze map and were instructed to provideverbal route instructions to reach a goal, to a person who is lost in the maze. Then, the participants answered a questionnaireabout their own navigation abilities and responded to the IRI-J. The descriptive data were objectively evaluated with reference tothe following three points: types of spatial description (survey or route), consideration for the other person’s point of view, andunambiguity of instruction. We analyzed the relationships between the descriptive traits and scores on the two questionnaires,and found that the three points were good predictors for some empathy-related factors measured by the IRI-J.

Explanatory Completeness: Evidence from Causal Chains

Explanations have no bound in principle, but in practice, people prefer explanations that are complete (Zemla etal., 2017), and the explanations that they generate are bounded (Miyake, 1986). We tested reasoners’ ability to assess whethersome explanations are incomplete. Participants in three experiments received explanations, i.e., chains of causal events, e.g., Acauses B causes C. Their task was to choose questions relevant to links in the chain. Some explanations contained ”breaks” inthe chain, whereas others did not. Participants in three studies were able to detect the breaks, and preliminary data suggest thatthey assess explanations with breaks as less complete than those without breaks. Many participants also chose to ask questionsabout the initial event in a causal chain (e.g., A in the chain above), suggesting that such initial events are themselves seen asincomplete. The studies reveal a novel pattern in reasoners’ ability to formulate explanations.

Different alternative explanations can render different information relevant toexplaining an event

Scientific reasoning includes deciding whether information is relevant to explaining an event. In some cases, seeinginformation as relevant requires having a background theory or explanation that can make sense of the information. Collegestudents were shown a possible explanation for an event, along with two pieces of possibly relevant information (Info1 andInfo2), and one of two possible alternative explanations (Alt1 or Alt2). Info1 was seen as more relevant when Alt1 rather thanAlt2 was presented; Info2 was seen as more relevant when Alt2 rather than Alt1 was presented. In addition, relevance ratingsof the information increased as did initial ratings of the Alternative. People from different backgrounds might bring with themdifferent alternative theories that can hinder the understanding of why some information is relevant and other information not. Inaddition, finding the initial alternative compelling might enable people to better assess the relevance of additional information.

One-shot Learning and Classification in Children

People can often generalize concepts from just a single example, while machine learning algorithms typicallyrequire hundreds. Lake, Salakhutdinov and Tenenbaum (2016) studied this ability in the domain of handwritten characters,and proposed a model for one-shot learning of new concepts based on inferring compositionally structured generative models,and transfer (or learning to learn) from familiar concepts. Lake et al showed that their model fit well with the classifications anddrawings of adults, but provided no direct evidence for the role of learning to learn which presumably occurs mostly in childrenlearning to draw. Here we study the drawing and classification abilities of children ages 3-5, asking whether their ability toclassify novel objects and handwritten characters is related to their ability to infer an appropriate motor program for drawing ortracing characters. Preliminary results suggest at least a weak relationship between these abilities, independent of age.

Mentioning atypical properties of objects is communicatively efficient

What governs how much information speakers include in referring expressions? Atypical properties of objects aremore likely to be included in referring expressions than typical ones. E.g., speakers are more likely to call a blue banana a “bluebanana” and a yellow banana a ”banana”. A unified account of this phenomenon is lacking. When should a rational speakermention an object’s color? Reference production is modeled within the Rational Speech Act framework. Utterances (“banana”,“blue”, and “blue banana”) are taken to have a graded semantics: rather than assuming all bananas are equally good instancesof “banana”, we empirically elicited object-utterance typicality values for all possible utterances. Pragmatic speakers selectutterances proportionally to the probability that a literal listener using a graded semantics will select the intended referent. Weevaluate the proposed model on a dataset of freely produced referring expressions collected in an interactive reference gameexperiment via the web.

Why Would ’Same’ Go With ’Same’? Exploring New Factors Required ForRelational Reasoning

Relational Match to Sample (RMTS) is a common test of relational reasoning involving matching cards based onthe relations “same” and “different”. Children below the age of five fail RMTS, even with corrective feedback. Given thatsuccess on RMTS depends on the ability to represent and compare ”same” and ”different”, such failure has been interpreted asindicative of the absence of these abilities (Penn, Holyoak & Povinelli, 2008; Hochmann, Mody & Carey, 2016).In the current studies three, four and five-year-old children were provided explicit instructions on RMTS. Results showsuccess in all groups, including three-year-olds - two years earlier than previous work. This suggests the ability to representand compare ”same” and ”different” emerges significantly earlier than spontaneous success on RMTS, undermining previousinterpretations. More generally, this work begins to explore the nature of the development which allows existing relationalreasoning capacities to be spontaneously deployed in RMTS.

When reading is harder than a mother kucker: Top-down effects of the taboo-nesson novel word pronunciation

When pronouncing novel/unknown words, readers often use prior experience with similar, neighbor words. Com-parison to neighbors can be helpful for unknown or novel words (wug is like pug), but it can also lead to errors (pint is notlike mint). We investigate whether pronunciation can be affected by top-down influences, specifically the perceived taboo-nessof a known neighbor. While orthographic similarity typically biases novel-word pronunciation to be similar to a known word,taboo-ness might bias pronunciation away from a likely one. Adults read aloud words from three lists– novel words that wereneighbors to taboo words, novel words that were neighbors to benign words, and known control words. All known neighborsand controls were frequency matched. Results show differences in the correspondence between pronunciation of novel wordsand known neighbors depending on the relative taboo-ness of the known neighbor. Findings suggest that perceived taboo-nesshas top-down influences on reading.

Effects of Variable Response-Stimulus Interval (RSI) On Sequence Learning

The objective of this study was to investigate the effects of varying Response-to-Stimulus interval (RSI) on sequencelearning by systematically varying it across three different groups (Group1: 0-300ms, Group2: 400-700ms and Group3: 800-1100ms) and to assess the implicitness and explicitness of the knowledge acquired through such learning. Serial Reaction timetask followed by generation task and recognition task were used for this purpose. Results of the SRT task showed learning in allthe three groups and the results of the free generation task and recognition task revealed that the sequence learning was implicitin Groups 1 and 2 while it was explicit in Group3. These results were discussed in the context of a recent theoretical frameworkthat proposes conditions in which a switch from implicit to explicit knowledge acquisition is facilitated.

Object Understanding: Exploring the Path from Percept to Meaning

This research addresses the generation of meaningful interpretations of real-world perceptual stimuli. Accordingto a widespread framework we will call the features-first view, a stimulus is initially encoded via semantically-laden, symbol-like properties that are compared to stored category representations to find the best match. Alternative theoretical perspectiveschallenge the features-first view, but there has been no direct empirical test. In our experiment, participants were shownphotographic images of everyday objects and asked to judge as quickly as possible whether a provided verbal descriptivematched the picture. We tested different levels of delay between image and descriptive and found evidence that basic-levelcategory labels were verified faster than clearly manifested descriptions of physical or functional properties. Accordingly,people know the category of the stimulus before knowing its semantic properties. The present evidence suggests that thecategory is used to achieve a property-level description of the meaning of the stimulus, not vice-versa.

Effect of Touch-produced Sounds on Surface Texture Perception

Texture is an important source of information for distinguishing surface properties. We are able to perceive varioustextural properties of surfaces from tactile or visual inputs. However, it is unclear how touch-produced sounds influence thevarious surface texture perceptions. In this study, we examined whether the touch sounds produced by different surface texturesinfluence the various surface perceptions. Consequently, the surface textures with high height and wide interval resulted inrough, bumpy, soft and cool perceptions and the surface textures with the low height and narrow interval resulted in smooth,flat, hard and warm perception. Also, there were statistically significant differences in these measures between two surfacetexture groups. Furthermore, significantly positive correlations were found in “rough – smooth”, “bumpy – flat”, “sticky –slippery”, “wet – dry” and “unpleasant – pleasant” measures between touch-produced sounds and actual touch. This indicatethat the touch-produced sounds influence various surface perceptions.

Identifying Causal Direction in the Two-Variable Case

One of the key characteristics of human cognition is the ability to learn causal structure from data. An influentialthread of research into causal learning relies on causal graphical models as a theoretical foundation, and emphasizes the roleof prior knowledge, interventions, and statistical independence as tools with which people learn causal structure. What if thesesources of information are all absent, as in the problem of identifying causal direction from observations of just two variables?Most work has either ignored this problem or asserted that it is inherently unsolvable. However, recent machine learningalgorithms can sometimes infer causal directionality in this setting, by exploiting simple assumptions about the relationshipbetween causes and the noise observed in their effects (Mooij, et al 2016). We investigate whether humans are able to exploitthese assumptions or others in order to infer the causal connection between two statistically dependent variables.

Intolerance to uncertainty is associated with diminished exploration

Across diverse cognitive and behavioral domains, humans confront a fundamental tension between exploiting currentknowledge about the environment and exploring the environment in order to acquire new knowledge. Individuals differ idiosyn-cratically in how they balance this explore/exploit tradeoff, although the sources of these individual differences have not beensystematically studied. In the current study, we sought to do so, in terms of trait-level affective phenotypes. Specifically, weinvestigated whether intolerance to uncertainty (IU), characterized by a negative disposition toward uncertainty, predicted bothrandom and directed exploration in a two-armed bandit task which manipulated decision horizon. We found that greater IU wasassociated with diminished exploration, both random (p<0.001) and directed (p<0.05). These results suggest the importanceof explicitly considering affective states and dispositions in human decision-making and also have psychiatric implications, tothe extent that IU is a transdiagnostic dimension central to a range of anxiety-related disorders.

Interplay between semantic and emotional information in visual scene processing

We examined whether and how image’s semantics and emotion content interact during visual processing. In eachtrial, we briefly presented two emotional or neutral images (a scene context and an object), manipulating the semantic con-sistency and the emotional consistency of the pair. Participants categorised one image semantically or emotionally. Semanticcategorisation was overall better than emotional categorisation, but reduced in emotional compared to neutral images, and es-pecially in negative images. Emotional categorisation was better for positive than neutral or negative images; moreover, it wasfacilitated by emotional consistency and, for accuracy in context images, by semantic consistency. Our results show easinessof semantic compared to emotional categorisation. They suggest that semantic and emotion processes are interdependent, al-though emotional influence on semantic processing seems stronger than the counterpart, with in particular an interfering effectof aversive images. Conversely, image’s attractiveness seems beneficial when evaluating the quality of the emotional content.

The effect of binaural beats on inhibition

A binaural beat is the perceptual experience that occurs when two tones of slightly different frequencies are presenteddichotically, creating the experience of a third tone corresponding to the difference in frequencies. Many temporary cognitiveeffects have been linked to the presentation of a binaural beat, including increased working memory capacity. In the presentstudy, a version of the flanker test was used to investigate the effect of short-term alpha wave binaural beat stimulation oninhibition processes specifically. Participants were presented with 10 minutes of either mid-alpha range binaural beats combinedwith a recording of waves or only the sound of waves. After this, participants completed a flanker test. The difference betweenreaction times of congruent and incongruent trials on the flanker task was significantly lower in the binaural beats conditionthan the wave condition, suggesting that even brief exposure to binaural beats aids in the inhibition of irrelevant stimuli.

Perception of others: Representation of immigrant groups in newspaper articles

Immigrants always have difficulties in integrating into a local society, and sometimes these difficulties come fromthe fact that they are subconsciously regarded as someone from the outside. How they are represented in the main stream mediacould reflect people’s perceptions towards the ‘otherness’ of different social members. We analyzed the representation of 29US immigrant groups in newspaper articles in 2 related studies. The favorability of an immigrant groups is highly associatedwith its perceived social distance (reflected through usage of concrete language) in our research. To further understand whatcaused the positive or negative image of immigrants, we applied Latent Dirichlet Allocation to identify topics associated withimmigrant groups. We also investigated into how these news topics differ in terms of lingual social distance and favorability.The results provide both qualitative and quantitative insights in how the image of immigrants are reproduced in social media.

Modelling the dynamics of integrating context into perception: in good and inpoor readers

Individuals implicitly learn the statistics of environmental stimuli. We used ”contraction bias”, the tendency toperceive stimuli closer to the estimated mean of similar previous stimuli, to characterize the dynamics of these implicit inferenceprocesses. Using a simple auditory discrimination task we found that listeners build a rich representation of the distribution ofpast stimuli, and yet over represent very recent events. This combined pattern allows both learning of the stable environment,and flexibility to fast changes.We further characterized populations who have difficulties in acquiring specific expertise, i.e. specific developmental dis-orders, focusing on reading (dyslexia) and non-verbal communication (high functioning ASD, autism spectrum, individuals)disability, respectively. We found that the pattern of their perceptual inference differs from controls’. Both underweight pre-vious events. However, dyslexics’ implicit memory decays fast and they underweight earlier events, whereas ASD individualsunderweight recent events. This pattern parallels, and perhaps underlies, their strengths and weaknesses.

The Effects of Familiarity and Typicality on Naming Objects and Faces

It is found that when we name an object or a face, we often use basic level name (e.g., dog) rather than a nameat superordinate level (e.g., animal) or subordinate level (e.g., Labrador). In addition, although abundant evidence generallysuggested that both familiarity and typicality influence object recognition, how each of the two factors involves categorizationin terms of naming is not fully investigated yet. The present studies were performed to examine the familiarity and typicalityeffects on naming either an object or a face. Names for basic, superordinate, and subordinate levels were prepared for testingthe speed and correctness of object/face identification. As a result, familiarity, not typicality, induced a down-shift pattern fornaming. In contrast, typicality led to overall faster responses. The findings of the study indicated that familiarity and typicalityhave dissimilar effects on categorization by naming.

The Effect of CSAL AutoTutor on Deep Comprehension of Text in Low-LiteracyAdult Readers

It is well documented that reading strategies of low-literacy readers are suboptimal when text requires deeper levelsof comprehension. Deep comprehension demands causal or goal-oriented reasoning and functional conceptual knowledge.Alternatively, shallow comprehension entails recall of definitions or text features without necessitating a coherent understandingof the text. The Center for Adult Literacy (CSAL) AutoTutor is an interactive intelligent tutoring system designed to foster deepand shallow comprehension in low-literacy readers. The present work represents the first empirical study of the effect of CSALAutoTutor on comprehension type in low- and high-literacy readers. Community members and students interacted with CSALAutotutor and then were assessed on recall ability for the structure (shallow) and meaning (deep) of sentences from lessontext. Preliminary analysis suggests CSAL AutoTutor promotes comparable deep level comprehension in low- and high-literacyreaders. Implications for CSAL AutoTutor as a literacy intervention and future goals are discussed.

Adaptive response priors in context-dependent decision-making

Context (such as our location or current goal) informs everyday decisions, both by predicting stimuli and determiningrelevant responses. How do we develop priors that are general enough to apply in various contexts yet specific enough tomaximize reward in a given context? We investigated this using the AX-CPT, a task in which a cue determines which buttonto press for a probe that appears seconds later. We manipulated the frequency of the probe given the cue across participantsand built a diffusion model to estimate how the cue informs participants’ priors for the decision. We found that participants’context-dependent priors were closer to each other and less extreme than those predicted by a model that maximizes rewardrate given the true stimulus frequencies. However, participants’ priors were optimal given their subjective frequency estimates,which showed that they averaged response probabilities across cues when the cues made sufficiently similar predictions.

Comprehension of Chinese Classifiers in Preschool Children

The present research aimed to investigate children’s comprehension of Chinese classifiers. Sixty-five Chinese-speaking children between the ages of 4 and 6 recruited in Taiwan participated in the experiment. The results indicate thatchildren can make generalization based on their understanding of classifiers instead of solely relying on classifier-noun associ-ations. The results also show that the participants performed equally on both shape-based and feature-shared classifiers, whichsuggests that children not only use shape salience to learn Chinese classifiers, but are also sensitive to other relations betweenobjects classified by the same Chinese classifier. Besides, the complex patterns in the results imply that in spite of the exposureto classifiers, the semantic transparency between classifiers and objects varies considerably in both semantic types of classifier,which might be the primary reason that some classifiers are more difficult for children to acquire.

Cumulative response probabilities: Estimating time course of lexical activationfrom single-point response times

An aim of research on spoken word recognition is to characterize the influence of various lexical characteristics(e.g., word frequency, neighborhood size) on lexical access. Dynamics can be coarsely estimated from single-point measureslike naming or more directly assessed using time course measures like fixation proportions over time in the visual worldparadigm (e.g., Tanenhaus et al., 1995). We propose that cumulative response probabilities (CRPs) over time may allow anew characterization of the activation dynamics of lexical access from single-point measures. We assume that the timing ofresponses in a naming task reflects probabilistic sampling of underlying continuous activation dynamics that can be recoveredby CRPs. We applied CRP analyses to visual word recognition data collected for 40,481 words from 472 participants (theEnglish Lexicon Project; Balota et al., 2007) and report initial efforts to validate this new approach.

Guardian and Daily Mail Readers’ Implicit Attitudes to Immigration

The implicit association test (IAT) measures bias towards often controversial topics (race/religion), while newspaperstypically take strong positive/negative stances on such issues. In a pre-registered study, we developed and administered an im-migration IAT to readers of the Daily Mail (typically anti-immigration) and Guardian (typically pro-immigration) newspapers.IAT Materials were constructed based on co-occurrence frequencies from each newspapers’ website for immigration-relatedterms (migrant) and positive/negative attributes (skilled/unskilled). Target stimuli showed stronger negative associations withimmigration concepts in the Daily Mail corpus compared to the Guardian corpus, and stronger positive associations in theGuardian corpus compared to the Daily Mail. Consistent with these linguistic distributional differences, Daily Mail readersexhibited a larger IAT bias, revealing stronger negative associations to immigration concepts compared to Guardian readers.This difference in overall bias was not explained by other variables, and raises the possibility that exposure to biased languagecontributes to biased implicit attitudes.

How and when does the syllable become a reading unit? Developmental evidencein French children

French beginning readers might rely on syllables during reading acquisition. However, no in-depth developmentalstudy has been carried out to determine how and when the syllable becomes this prelexical and segmental unit used in the timecourse of reading acquisition. We recruited 800 French-speaking children distributed in grade 1-5. We used a lexical decisiontask in a visual masked priming paradigm and a visual identification task. We manipulated the initial syllable frequency, theinitial bi/trigram frequency, and the initial syllable structure (CV; CVC). Our main results describe a clear developmental course.The syllable-based effects are early (G1) and sustainably observed (G5), and primarily depend on the syllable frequency. FromG2, we found the systematic, automatic use of the syllable as prelexical and segmental unit but the syllable frequency hasfacilitatory syllable-based effects in the task with lexical access, while it has inhibitory effects in the task without lexical access.

Epistemically Suspect Beliefs can be partly explained by individual’s propensitytowards contradiction

Studies on epistemically suspect beliefs (ESB) have suggested that individual’s analytic cognition suppresses un-warranted beliefs, however, our previous studies also showed that an inhibitory effect of analytic cognition was higher amongWesterners than Easterners. Rather, intuitive cognition was a common predictor of beliefs between two cultures. Among sev-eral cultural differences in cognitive style, we suspect that tendency towards dialectic thinking, i.e., tolerance for contradictionmay contribute cultural differences on ESB. The present study aimed to explore this possibility and investigated the associationbetween beliefs and other cognitive measures including individual’s cognitive abilities, thinking dispositions, personality traitsand propensity towards dialectic thinking. The results showed that the ESB resulted from our intuitive cognition for the mostpart, and that the effect of culture diminished whilst controlling individual’s tendency towards dialectic thinking and style ofcausal cognition. The cultural difference in a relationship between beliefs and cognitive style was discussed.

Practicing an auditory working memory task recruits lower-level auditory areas ina task-specific manner

We studied the impact of the trained auditory task on the pattern of behavioural improvement, and its relation tothe underlying neural mechanisms. Specifically, we asked whether training with tone retention and manipulation (workingmemory, WM) transferred to pitch discrimination and vice versa, and whether training modified the brain areas that underlietask performance. Training substantially improved performance, but did not transfer across tasks, even when using the samestimuli. Pre and post training fMRI scans revealed that WM training enhanced activity in bilateral auditory cortices, butnot in frontal areas that are initially associated with higher cognitive functions. These results suggest that training-inducedimprovement is associated with back-tracking along the reverse hierarchy in a task specific manner, as predicted by the ReverseHierarchy Theory of perceptual learning (Ahissar & Hochstein, 2004). Thus, low-level areas are recruited, but there is nogeneral upgrade in WM or in auditory skills.

Do Relationality and Aptness Influence Conventionalization?

The conventionalization of figurative comparisons is one source of lexical evolution. For example, anchor onceonly meant a device for mooring a ship, but may now be used to describe any source of stability or confidence. Our goalis to understand this process. Following the Career of Metaphor framework, figurative mappings are interpreted through astructure-mapping process, rendering common structure salient. As figurative terms become conventionalized, (1) the figurativesense becomes associated with the base term; and (2) there is shift from simile form to metaphor form. In two studies weinvestigated psycholinguistic properties that may influence this process: relationality and aptness. We use relative preferencefor the metaphor form as an estimate of degree of conventionalization; by determining the preferred form for a set of figuratives,we find evidence that both aptness and relationality influence this process. We speculate that figurative comparisons may giverise to new relational terms.

Mutual Exclusivity Revisited – When Pragmatics overrides Novelty

Children typically apply a novel label to a novel object, rather than to a familiar object; a phenomenon called MutualExclusivity (Markman et al., 2003). A recent explanation is that children tend to associate novel stimuli together (Horst et al.,2011). We show that pragmatic factors may override novelty. In our study two-year-old children first played with a novel objecttogether with E1. Then E1 left the room and E2 brought another three novel objects for the child to manipulate on his/her own.Finally, E1 came back and requested the child to give her the ‘Bitye’. Most children chose the first object, with which theyhad a common history with E1, even though it was the least novel. This suggests that children understand a novel word byconsidering to which object the speaker is most likely to have intended to refer.

Search Your Feelings, Luke: Emotional Fluency Predicts Well-being andEmotional Intelligence

How we feel reflects a combination of recalled and recognized emotions. All existing self-report measures are basedsolely on recognized emotions. To understand the influence of recalled emotions, we developed a new method to recover humanemotional states based on emotional free association, in a task we call the emotional fluency. The present work investigated thedifferences between recall and recognition in human emotional states. We compared the emotional fluency task with self-reportmeasures, including PANAS, WEMWBS, and the Emotional Intelligence Scale. Using language statistics computed from theemotional fluency task, we developed multiple models for predicting self-report measures. We find that while recalled emotionscan predict recognized emotions, they highlight important problems with existing recognition measures, including emotionalcoverage and the difference between availability and accessibility. We also investigate the search process in emotional memory,supporting the role of unbiased memory sampling and higher emotional intelligence and mental well-being.

Children’s reasoning about data sets

When reasoning about several numbers, past work has shown that adults mentally summarize data sets and reasonbased on set characteristics such as the mean and variance (Morris & Masnick, 2015). In the current study, we asked 10- and12-year-old children to look at two columns of numbers (framed as the distances two golfers drove a golf ball, when doingso repeatedly), and to choose which golfer hit the ball farther. We examined reaction time, accuracy, and eye movements, inaddition to self-reported strategy use. We found children reasoned using some of the same summary characteristics as adults,though less consistently, and had more varied strategy uses. For example, some children focused only on one number in eachset, a pattern not seen in adults. These findings suggest that instruction building on these intuitions may help develop children’snumerical cognition skills.

Attention Modulation Effects on Visual Feature-selectivity of Neurons inBrain-inspired Categorization Models

Most Brain-inspired Visual Object Recognition Models(BVORMs) do not consider local and global reciprocal con-nections in visual pathway. We addressed this weakness and implemented an attention modulation mechanism based on feed-back connections in BVORMs, where feature-selectivity is shaped and modulated by categorization of objects based on theirvisual features. This modification is inspired by the top-down neuromodulatory signals that make changes in post-synapticactivities of the feature-selective neurons. We also incorporated an implicit memory unit in BVORMs to accumulate recentHebbian synaptic plasticity’s of the neurons in each task. This mechanism guides the top-down feature-based attention modula-tion to retrieve the interrelated feature-selectivity pattern for each task.HMax and CNN models were used as two BVORMs andtested on a visual categorization problem: natural versus artificial objects in CALTECH-256. Based on experimental results,our proposed modifications not only increased their biological-plausibility but also significantly improved their categorizationaccuracies compared to the original models.

Simple and Complex Working Memory Tasks Allow Similar Benefits ofInformation Compression

Because complex span tasks were designed to create a demanding concurrent task, the average span is usually lower(4 ± 1 items) than in simple span tasks (7 ± 2 items). One possible reason for the higher span of simple span tasks is thatparticipants can take profit of the spare time to chunk a few stimuli into 4 ± 1 groups. It follows that the respective spans of thesetwo types of tasks could be equal (at around 4 ± 1) when regularities are absent. We therefore predicted an interaction betweentask and chunkability, supporting a single higher span for a simple span task using chunkable items. However, observation ofthe spans in the non-chunkable vs. chunkable series refuted the idea that chunking is important solely in simple spans. Indeed,information compression processes contributed to performance levels to a similar extent in simple and complex span tasks.

Causal asymmetry and the intuitive physics of collision events

In the Michotte (1963) launching scenario, an object (X) moves toward a resting object (Y), eventually colliding withit. In the moment of contact, X stops und Y starts moving - creating the strong impression that X caused Y’s motion and that Xexerted a force on Y (but not vice versa). These asymmetries contradict the (symmetrical) laws of Newtonian mechanics, whichare at the heart of the popular “noisy Newton” theories of intuitive physics. As an alternative, we propose that inferences inphysical scenarios operate over pre-Newtonian representations that are based on the asymmetrical concept of impetus, a motiveforce that keeps objects moving and that is transferred and reflected in object collisions. We present a formal model of impetusand show that, unlike noisy Newton theories, it provides an explanation of asymmetrical judgments. Other related findings canalso be modeled (e.g., biases in mass judgments).

The development of interpersonal regret and its relation to prosocial choice

We examined whether children feel regret when their failure to make a prosocial choice negatively affects a peer.Five-to-six-year-olds and 7-to-9-year-olds played a game in which they completed a sticker sheet to win a prize. Children thendecided whether to donate a spare sticker to another child; most children did not donate. Children discovered that the next childdid not have enough stickers to win a prize, and rated their emotions. At this point, children did not know whether the nextchild could have been able to win the prize if they had donated the sticker in question. This counterfactual information wasthen provided, and children rated whether they felt happier, sadder, or the same as before. Only the 7-to-9-year-olds’ responsessuggested that they experienced interpersonal regret. We also showed that experiencing interpersonal regret in the sticker taskresulted in children making more prosocial choices in a separate task.

Interactive and embodied repair: Displaying, recognizing, and negotiatingmisalignment in an emerging language context

As problems of understanding arise in conversational interaction, we must find a means to indicate to our interlocutorthe reason for our misunderstanding. However, we are simultaneously constrained by social interactive practices that limit facethreat and adhere to epistemic rights. Thus, the challenge is to communicate our own misunderstanding - as specifically aspossible - while avoiding explicitness. This challenge may be increased in contexts of language emergence in which alignmentis necessary to promote communicative efficiency and conventionalization. Participants in novel communication tasks reliedon certain gesture-driven other-initiated repair strategies to gain interactive alignment. The embodied display of cognitive andinteractive misalignment cues the interlocutor to repair in a way that reflects their own understanding of the repair initiation andtrouble source. The breakdown of intersubjectivity - and its subsequent re-building - is observed in the negotiation of evolvingsignal-meaning matches through interactive repair sequences.

Misalignment increases abstraction of referring expressions

A central finding in dialogue research is that interlocutors rapidly converge on referring expressions which becomeprogressively contracted and abstract. However there is currently no consensus on which mechanisms underpin convergence:The interactive alignment model (Pickering and Garrod, 2009) favours alignment processes, the grounding model (Clark, 1996)prioritizes positive feedback, while Healey (2002) demonstrates the importance of miscommunication in identifying differencesof interpretation.To investigate convergence we report a variant of the maze-task in which both participants are given misaligned instructions:One participant is primed with instructions that conceptualize the maze as consisting of horizontal vectors (e.g .”4th row, 2ndsquare”); the other is primed with instructions that conceptualize the maze as consisting of vertical vectors (e.g .”3rd column,2nd square”). Compared with a baseline, misaligned dyads converged on more abstract referring expressions. We argue thispattern is due to participants interactively combining their perspective with that of their partner.

The spontaneous creation of systems of conventions

We used a non-linguistic experimental paradigm to explore the instantaneous creation and adaption of novel commu-nicative systems of conventions. Groups of participants played a computer game, in which they sent and interpreted minimalsignals to obtain shared rewards within a virtual scene. Within groups, trials manipulated the space of possible signals thatcould be sent, and the set of meanings to be expressed (the range of cases for the locations and quantities of rewards). Betweengroups, initial conditions were manipulated through early exposure to different sets of communicative cases.We observed participants spontaneously develop systems of conventions that were adapted to the full range of signal-meaningmappings encountered. Groups favoured systems optimised to their particular initial learning environment. These systemsbecome entrenched and transferred to new signal-mapping environments to which they were not adapted.

Refining the cognitive semantic web: The tensor method to represent thetopographic emplacement of different word categories

A central problem concerning the organization of the cognitive semantic web is to understand how different cate-gories of words are stored in the brain with a well-defined topographical organization. This topography is a natural constructionthat plausibly is strongly related with the syntactic and semantic organization of natural languages. An eloquent experimentalevidence of the existence of a continuous semantic representation of object and action categories in the human brain has beenpublished by Huth et al (Neuron 76:1210, 2012). One of the ways to explain the emergence of a topographical organization inthe brain cortex using neurocomputational models, is by means of Kohonen’s self-organizing maps. Here we show that thesetopographies can be operationally represented with associative memories spatially organized by tensor contexts. We illustrateformally and numerically this fact. In addition, we show that, consistently with evidence from pathology, different semanticcategories can be specifically damaged.

Picture book reading in the lives of 18-30 month old children: A diary study

Picture book reading is a common activity in the lives of many children. This work describes the frequency andcharacter of picture book reading in American homes. Seventy-seven monolingual English-speaking families with childrenbetween the ages of 18-30 months took part in a 5-day diary study in which caregivers recorded details about picture book read-ing activities. This sample is characteristic of some laboratory samples but less nationally representative; 92.2% of caregiversheld a college degree. Relative to previously reported averages, caregivers reported reading to children more often (6.8x/day),reported beginning reading at a younger age (2.2 months) and reported more books in the home (111.1 books). Caregiversreported both reading the book text and discussing the pictures with their children. These numbers suggest an extremely highupper-limit to the amount of language input some children receive from picture book reading. Consequences for languageenvironments and language development will be discussed.

Resemblance among similarity measures in semantic representation

Similarity measures, extent to which two concepts have similar meanings, are key to understanding how conceptsare represented, with different theoretical perspectives relying on very different sources of data from which similarity canbe calculated. Experiential/embodied theories use verbal features or property ratings; distributional/relational ones use co-occurrence. Similarity may also be estimated from tasks like word-association, priming, and other rating studies. Often thedifferent theoretical perspectives are placed in opposition; here we test the extent to which similarity representations basedon different measures converge. We used Representational Similarity Analysis and multidimensional scaling on 31 similaritymeasures. Similarity in age-of-acquisition and word-length were related to similarity in naming and priming; and affectivesimilarity and co-occurrence were also related. More importantly, representational resemblance was shown among embodied,distributional and association-based representations, demonstrating that different data sources are employed in a similar way inbuilding meaningful conceptual representation.

Effects of Question Format on Test-Taker Cognition

Technology-based, interactive test questions are common in large-scale assessments, yet how alternative questionformats influence test-taker cognition is not well understood. In a series of studies, we investigated test-taker performance onisomorphic questions using alternative presentation layouts and modes of responding. Adult participants solved math prob-lems in three formats, each of which regularly appear in many large-scale assessments: 1) forced-choice (explicit True-Falseoptions) presented in a table format, 2) check-all-that-apply (implicit True-False options) presented in a table format, and 3)check-all-that apply presented as separate questions. Participants’ solution time and affirmative selection rate suggested dif-ferent cognitive processes for the question formats, particularly when they were uncertain of their answers. We propose acognitive model to account for the results and predict the impact of alternative question formats on test-takers. We discuss howprinciples of cognitive science and human-computer interaction provide direct implications for designing assessment questionsand understanding test-taker cognition.

Magnitude of metaphor and its effect on reasoning about immigration

Metaphor is replete in discourse about immigration. Recent work shows that metaphoric framing can influenceattitudes toward immigration (e.g., Landau et al., 2009). However, we know little about how and when specific information inthe source domain drives this effect. Our study takes a novel approach, examining how varying intensity of information in thesource domain frame influences attitudes toward immigrants and immigration in the U.S. We analyze various metaphors butwe focus especially on intensity effects in the conceptual metaphor IMMIGRATION IS FLUID TRANSFER. For the FLUIDTRANSFER source domain, we investigate how varying intensity of flow (e.g., rate) influences attitudes about immigration,including whether immigrants should have access to social services and what type of wall should be built, if at all. Our resultsmake a valuable contribution to metaphor research by revealing what information within the source domain has the most (orleast) robust effects on reasoning.

Investigating the Impact of Sleep on Eyewitness Memory

How sleep impacts the accuracy of identifications that eyewitnesses make from lineups is unknown. For a com-prehensive understanding of eyewitness performance, two types of eyewitness ID accuracy are considered: discriminability(the ability to distinguish innocent from guilty suspects) and reliability (the probability that the identified suspect was the of-fender). The well-known role sleep plays in memory consolidation should apply to an eyewitness’s ability to discriminate, butnot necessarily their reliability. That is what we investigated in a large-scale forensically-relevant experiment. We compareddiscriminability and reliability from sleep (sleep occurs between witnessing a crime and lineup test) and wake (remains awakebetween crime and lineup) conditions. Furthermore, theorists have long been using signal-detection models to understandrecognition memory, but its use is new to the field of eyewitness ID research. Thus, we compared signal-detection models withdifferent decision rules. Our findings shed light on the impact sleep has on eyewitness IDs.

Impact of testimony and prior knowledge on children’s beliefs about categoryhomogeneity

Previous work has shown that preschoolers—in comparison to older children and adults—tend to view categoriesas homogeneous, generalizing properties of individuals broadly to all category members (e.g., this dax has wings, so all daxesdo). Here, we explore whether the testimony used to describe category individuals as well as children’s prior knowledge ofcategories attenuates their homogeneity expectations. Using a novel induction task, 4 to 7-year-olds were asked to predict thedistribution of properties among members of familiar/unfamiliar animal categories based on a single exemplar. Exemplars wereintroduced as “special” to half of participants. Preliminary findings (N = 71) suggest that prior knowledge may contribute tobeliefs about category homogeneity: responses for familiar animals varied appropriately given the real-world prevalence of eachproperty whereas children overestimated the property’s prevalence for unfamiliar animals. The complete dataset will speak tohow language choice in testimony shifts children’s beliefs about homogeneity.

Learning to Consider Alternative Causes: Can Practice Make Us More Aware ofOur Imperfection?

In hindsight bias, upon learning an outcome, one is overly confident that one would have “known it all along.”Several researchers have been able to neutralize hindsight bias by prompting participants to consider alternative outcomes,but can we learn to avoid bias for novel outcomes, without prompting? Foresight participants read brief summaries of fivepsychology studies, and learned the mean performance of one group in each study. They estimated the other group’s perfor-mance—reflecting their sense of the effect size—stated possible causes, and then learned the other group’s mean performance.Hindsight participants learned both groups’ mean performance at the outset, then indicated what they would have estimated.We asked whether (1) participants would show superior estimation and/or consideration of alternative causes for novel stimulione week later, and (2) whether Foresight participants would benefit more given the feedback they received on the accuracy oftheir estimates.

Bidirectional effect of emotional contagion for pain during face-to-face interaction

The automatic contagion of emotion is considered crucial in interpersonal communication. In face-to-face interac-tions, people could be both the receiver and sender of emotional content. Thus, contagion may have bidirectional influenceson the emotional states of individuals. However, many studies have mainly dealt with unidirectional contagion, such that theexpression of pain in a target entails a reaction of pain in the observer. In this study, we demonstrated bidirectional emotionalcontagion in the experience of thermal pain during interaction. Firstly, we showed that the physiological responses of dyadmembers were correlated with each other when they could interact compared to when they were impaired to see each other.Further, we demonstrated that individuals showed higher or lower physiological responses when their partners experiencedstronger or weaker stimuli respectively. Thus, people can develop similar physiological responses through interactions, and thiseffect seems to induce a change in the responsivity to stimuli.

Agent’s symmetry elicits egocentric transformations for spatial perspective-taking

Spatial perspective-taking is an ability to understand in which direction an object is located relative to an agent (e.g.,another person or a chair). Previous studies showed that left/right judgments prompted an egocentric transformation strategy(i.e., mental rotation of the self) whereas front/behind judgments prompted other strategies (e.g., tracing a line of sight). Toexamine whether the symmetrical shape of an agent could affect the choice of strategies, we used as an agent a cuboid whichhas a prong on one of its sides. We labeled the prong side as the front (Experiment 1) or right (Experiment 2) of the agent,about which participants made left/right and front/behind judgments. The results revealed that egocentric transformations weremore favored for judgments about directions along symmetrical than asymmetrical axes of the agent, regardless of whether thejudgment was about left/right or front/behind. This suggests similar processing underlies left/right and front/behind judgments.

Robot as Moral Agent: A Philosophical and Empirical Approach

What is necessary for robots to coexist with human beings? In order to do so, we suppose, robots must be moralagents. To be a moral agent is to bear its own responsibility which others cannot take for it. We will argue that such anirreplaceability consists in its having an inner world — one which others cannot directly experience, just as pleasure and pain.And personality of a moral agent, which is to be irreducible to a mere difference of traits or features of individuals, is firmlyrooted in such an inner world.We will support our theses by referring to our experiment in which humans and robots interact with each other doing acoordination task. This experiment will provide an empirical analysis of the human-robot relationship with regard to learningmechanism, moral judgement, and the ascription of the inner world.

Iconicity vs. Systematicity in Artificial Language Learning

A foundational assumption in linguistics has been that words and their meanings are arbitrarily related; however, thisposition has been challenged recently. Experiments have shown that both systematic (where similar objects have similar labels)and iconic (words ‘resemble’ the objects they label) associations between words and objects facilitate learning. However, thesetwo literatures remain confounded: the degree to which increased learnability is driven by iconicity rather than systematicityhas not been disentangled. Here we present the results of two studies testing the differences in learnability between artificiallexica that are either conventionally systematic, or both systematic and cross-modally iconic. In the first study we find that bothconventional and iconic systematic lexicons are equally learnable, but iconic mappings provide an early learnability advantage.In the second study we find that the presence of sound-symbolic associations for one dimension can interfere with the learningof conventional associations on another dimension.

The space and time of contamination: Complete, continual, spreading effects

People sometimes report feeling “totally” different (complete affectedness) and that “they’ll never be the same again”(continual affectedness) after negative events. It’s been proposed that complete, and continual negative effects characterizecontamination or impurity. Meanwhile, whether impurity is a legitimate moral domain apart from harm has been debated inmoral psychology. We address these matters using novel approaches from cognitive linguistics. First, according to a prominenttheory of verb semantics, verbs that convey impurity (contaminate, taint) belong to a class that implies complete affectedness(the “fill” class), such that contaminated entities are seen as completely contaminated. Second, people rated perpetrators equally,and highly, “contaminated”, “contaminating”, and “injuring”, whereas victims were rated straightforwardly “injured” (Turk;n=126, replicated twice). For ”contaminated” perpetrators, the taint carried on – they were continually ”contaminating”. Insum, impurity is distinct from harm: the process underlying impurity, contamination, involves inferences of complete, continualnegative effects that spread.

Construction of design activity index based on the value of artifact

Digital personal fabrication refers to the creation of products using ICT-tools by individuals. In order to supportthe users of such tools who are untrained in design, it is necessary to develop a support system that makes it possible to useexpert knowledge on designing products. For this study, we selected 26 items from the values that are considered importantfor design (e.g., unique, modern; Inomata et al., 2016), and investigated the design activities for realizing these values. Eightyprofessionals in design participated in the survey. Many design activities concerning shapes and colors were observed asways to realize the values. In addition, various activities such as improvements on materials and motifs or advices to satisfy thepractical design activity were observed. We created an index of the frequently used activities to realize each value and discussedits potential as an actual design support tool.

Indirection Explains Flexible Tuning of Neurons in Prefrontal Cortex

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is broadly seen as supporting cognitive flexibility - quickly adapting behavior in responseto changing circumstances. Some PFC neurons appear to actively maintain rule-like information associated with the currenttask, with firing changing with task context. Some PFC neurons, however, have been found to exhibit activity related to specificstimulus features or action options, but the tuning of these neurons appears to dynamically change with task shifts (Duncan,2001). Short-term synaptic plasticity has been proposed as the primary mechanism for rapidly adapting the response profilesof these cells. Using a computational cognitive neuroscience model of hierarchical structure in PFC (Kriete, Noelle, Cohen, &O’Reilly, 2013), an alternative account is offered in which flexible neural tuning arises not from fast synaptic change but from afrontal representational scheme involving neurons that encode references to other PFC areas rather than directly encoding taskrelevant sensory/motor information.

Five-Year-Old Children Transfer a Metacognitive Strategy to a Novel Task

Previous work has demonstrated that interventions like 1) giving in-the-moment performance feedback and 2) pro-viding a strategy rule can improve children’s metacognitive learning. However, there is little evidence to suggest that thislearning transfers to a novel task. We trained 5-year-olds’ metacognitive control in a task requiring participants to select theeasier of two games to acquire the highest amount of points. Compared to a control group who received no training, childrenwho were trained to control behavior (by selecting an easier dot discrimination task) showed greater evidence of transfer to anovel task (by selecting an easier line length discrimination task). This suggests that the learned strategy rule (i.e., to select aneasier task) was not stimulus-specific, and was abstract enough to apply to a novel task with new stimuli. In sum, 5-year-oldswere able to learn a strategy rule and spontaneously apply the strategy to a novel task.

Do forgiving God primes strengthen support for state sanctioned punishment?

Do forgiving God primes strengthen support for state-sanctioned punishment?Laurin et al (2012) found that beliefs in powerful, intervening Gods (both in general and when made salient) reduce people’sendorsement of state-sanctioned punishment. In light of this, we investigated whether the manner in which God intervenes (viaforgiveness or punishment) influences people’s endorsement of state-sanctioned punishment.Across four studies we explored a) whether priming participants with a forgiving God and b) whether salient, forgiving Godbeliefs increase endorsements of state-sanctioned punishment. The rationale being that a forgiving God will lead people toview punishment as a responsibility that lies with them rather than one outsourced to God. Our results revealed no evidence foreffects of forgiving God primes or salient forgiving god beliefs on endorsements of state-sanctioned punishment. We discussthe implications of these findings for extant theories of religious prosociality and proportionality-based accounts of morality.

Effects of motives of search and prior experiences on online browsingperformance: Considerations from searchers cognitive load

The present study aimed to develop effective education methods of online search for unskilled college students. Inthe preparatory stage of the study, an experiment using simple browsing tasks was conducted to examine the effects of importantfactors of searching focusing on cognitive load. Under two conditions (Casual and Formal) promoting different motivations,search result lists were displayed to fifty-nine college students to look for two types of information: seeking statistical data (taskA) and seeking views and opinions to answer open questions (task B). Analyses of each task using two factors (the conditionsand their presentation orders) revealed that in task A, only when the Casual condition was first, the participants performedbetter in the Formal condition. In task B, only when the Formal condition was first, browsing time in the Casual condition wasshorter. We assume that these effects are associated with the workload of browsing.

Anticipatory Active Inference from Learned Recurrent Neural Forward Models

We demonstrate that inference-based goal-directed behavior can be done by utilizing the temporal gradients in re-current neural network (RNN). The RNN learns a dynamic sensorimotor forward model. Once the RNN is trained, it can beused to execute active-inference-based, goal-directed policy optimization. The internal neural activities of the trained RNNessentially model the predictive state of the controlled entity. The implemented optimization process projects the neural activ-ities into the future via the RNN recurrences following a tentative sequence of motor commands (encoded in neurons akin torecurrent parametric biases). This sequence is adapted by back-projecting the error between the forward-projected hypotheticalstates and desired (goal-like) system states onto the motor commands. Few cycles of forward projection and goal-based errorbackpropagation yield the sequences of motor commands that control the dynamical systems. As an example, we show that atrained RNN model can be used to effectively control a quadrocopter-like system.

Application of fuzzy logic in dyslexia user modelling to design customizingassistive technology

Cognitive psychology studies phenomena that cannot be directly observed. Scientific knowledge about the brain isextensive, but there is still a lot to be understood about its functions. Cognitive functions are weakened in dyslexic children; thisis reflected in highly individual problems regarding the reading skills. Reading is a process which consists of decrypting graphiccharacters (perceptual level) and understanding the meaning of words (cognitive level). These levels cannot be separated.An approach – fuzzy logic – is used in order to address this issue and create a model of the dyslexic user, based on whichtechnologies can be individually tailored to a particular dyslexic. We discusse the possibilities of the use of the mathematicalapparatus for the categorisation of users with regard to their ”black box”. Further, we focuse on the development of newassistive technologies targeted at specific attention disorders, reading disorders, as well as information processing disorders.

An Exploratory Study of the Influence of Pretend Play on Children’sSelf-Regulation and Language Skills

Recently, there has been increased interest regarding how pretend play contributes to children’s cognitive develop-ment. This study examines the efficacy of a pretend play intervention on self-regulation and language skills of 4- to 5-year-oldsand explores parents’ perceptions about children’s engagement in pretend play. The small-scale intervention includes eight 30-minute sessions over 6 weeks, in groups of five children. Each session included: (1) shared storybook reading; (2) role-playing;and (3) review. During shared story-book reading the children were read two books with explicit phonological awareness andvocabulary instruction for 18 words in each book. Role-playing included providing the children with props, which allow forengagement in pretend play activities. Several measures were used pre- and post-intervention to evaluate children’s self- reg-ulation and language skills. The improvements that occurred in the intervention are considered alongside other cognitive andeducational factors to better understand the role of pretend play in educational settings.

Interpreting nonsignificant findings in psychological research

In this study, we examined the current practice and alternative methods for interpreting nonsignificant findings inpsychological research. The traditional null-hypothesis testing presents a challenge for researchers to interpret nonsignificantfindings. We reviewed the abstracts of all empirical articles published in three high-esteem psychological journals in 2015and selected those which referred to a nonsignificant result (N=134).We found that the majority of the statements interpretedthe results only within the sample, yet in 23% the authors inferred from the results to the absence of an effect. Bayes factoranalyses on these statistics indicated that the support of these results for the null-hypothesis is strong only in 4%, moderate in70% and anecdotal in 26%. The results revealed that Bayes factor analysis can help researchers in interpreting nonsignificantresults and also highlight that psychological studies with traditional sample sizes are unlikely to present strong evidence for thenull-hypothesis.

Perceived control in bounded-rational decision-making

The amount of control perceived by an agent governs their ability to learn. Bounded rationality, or the idea that weare limited by the amount cognitive work we can perform, provides an appealing framework within which perceived controlcould be formulated. When modeling the world, the bounded-rational agent balances the trade-off between the utility andcomplexity of this constructed model in order to choose an optimal policy. Here, we present a novel formulation of behavioralcontrol, bounded inference, which explicitly models control as the perceived constraint experienced by an agent during theinference process, employing a version of the free energy functional with an additional boundedness parameter as the variationalprinciple of this constrained optimization. The utility of bounded inference is demonstrated in simulations that capture variouscharacteristics of dysfunctional behavioral patterns as observed in a range of psychiatric disorders for which control beliefsplay a central role.

Applications of Cognitive Science to Enhancing Scholarly Communication

Learning from and building on the accomplishments of scholarly publications is often difficult. To address thischallenge, this work leverages well-replicated cognitive science phenomena to promote people’s understanding of researchfound in journal articles. It forms the conceptual groundwork for a digital platform through which users can author and learnfrom interactive multimedia documents that communicate research more effectively. One of the many recommendations is toreduce the split-attention effect by integrating text and graphics in figures. Doing so may help readers understand complexvisuospatial representations. Encouraging active processing via comprehension questions and responsive simulations of ex-perimental procedures embedded in articles may boost learning even more. To promote the creative extension of research,evidence-based brainstorming prompts that trigger analogical reasoning and episodic specificity induction should be adopted.If scholarly communication is centered on scientific principles like these, then the dissemination and dynamics of science mayboth advance.

Novel metacognitive problem solving task

Metacognition is important for decision making, problem solving and learning. Despite the widespread interest inmetacognitive skills and their development, it is challenging to measure metacognitive skills in children. Some excellent quali-tative and observational measures exist, but use metrics that are different from traditional metacognition tasks for adults. Somemeta-cognition tasks of memory have been developed for children, but these only offer a narrow range of the skills involvedin metacognition. Here, we compared performance on a meta-memory task for children with a new task of metacognitionfor problem solving. Our sample includes about 800 children aged 8-10 years who were part of a larger study exploring thedevelopment of thinking skills. The results indicated similarities and differences between the memory and problem solvingtasks, suggesting that the new task could be a bridge between existing qualitative and quantitative measures of metacognitionin children.

Comparing comparison indices: Assessing the validity of different magnitudecomparison measures across presentation formats and age groups

Magnitude comparison tasks are used to assess the precision of numerical representations. Recent research, how-ever, questions the validity of different measures of magnitude comparison. We investigated the validity of five performancemeasures: overall RT, overall accuracy, numerical ratio effect (RT), numerical ratio effect (accuracy), and Weber fraction.Kindergarten and university students completed symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude comparison tasks and a math skill mea-sure. For children and adults, we calculated Chronbach’s α separately for each presentation format. All values were in theunacceptable range, indicating that the different indices were not measuring the same construct. For children, a multiple re-gression predicting KeyMath scores from symbolic and non-symbolic indices showed that only non-symbolic overall accuracyand symbolic overall RT were predictors. For adults, a multiple regression predicting French Kit scores showed that only thesymbolic numerical ratio effect (RT) was a predictor. No index demonstrated predictive validity across formats or age groups.

Language input and development during a year in an early intervention classroom

By the time they are three-years-old, children raised in poverty hear 30 million fewer words than their socioeconomi-cally advantaged peers. This word gap predicts later school readiness outcomes and sets the stage for achievement gaps that canfollow the child through life. Although parent speech has become a subject of increasing study and intervention, less is knownabout speech in childcare settings. We conducted a longitudinal study in an early-intervention classroom for 2-3-year-old chil-dren from low-income, at-risk backgrounds. We examine the relationship between language input from teachers and peers andchildren’s language skills over one year. Results show that vocabulary knowledge influences children’s talkativeness in theclassroom, and talkativeness and the amount of language they hear positively relates to increases in their language abilities.Our application of automated measurement provides new insight into the dynamics of the classroom language environment andconsequences for language development in at-risk children.

Early Visual Evoked Potentials (VEPs) in Infant Siblings of Children with ASD,ADHD and Age-Matched Controls

Atypicalities in sensory perception are observed in individuals diagnosed with ASD and ADHD but have rarely beencontrasted in experimental studies. In the visual domain, superior performance on visual search tasks and hypersensitivity toflickering lights have been cited as evidence of unusual sensory profiles.To measure a reliable visual response, black-and-white checkerboards were presented under free-viewing conditions to threegroups of 10-month-olds: siblings of children with ASD (N=47), ADHD (N=21) and controls (N=18). Continuous EEG wasrecorded and VEPs time-locked to checkerboards presentation computed.Analysis of VEPs amplitude and latency revealed statistically significant group differences in the first 200ms post-stimulusonset. Early components were enhanced in amplitude (P100) and delayed in latency (P100-N100) in at-risk infants comparedto controls (p<.05).Atypical VEPs to low-level information might index a domain-general aberration in at-risk populations. The nature of thisatypicality will be further investigated by analyzing its association with background EEG.

How does social touch modulate arousal states? An investigation in earlydevelopment.

Caregiver-infant interaction through touch was shown to have long-term effects on child’s cognitive development,but the mechanisms are poorly understood. Our aim is to investigate how affective touch (slow gentle caressing) affects arousalstates in young infants. Previous work showed that slow-touch decreases heart rate in 9-month-old infants.We tested two groups of 6-months-old (n=26) and 9-months-old infants (n=23). We measured heart rate and saccadic reactiontime while infants performed a visual orienting task, where speed of re-orienting from a central fixation to a peripheral targetwas measured. During the experiment, infants received either slow or fast-touch on their back in blocked trials. We found noeffects of touch on heart rate in either age-group, and only marginal effects of slow-touch on reaction times in 9-month-oldinfants. We are currently testing 2 months-old infants to investigate if these effects are observed earlier in life; these new resultswill be discussed.

Hand, spoon or toothbrush? Towards the understanding of the neuralunderpinnings of affective touch in 5 months-old infants.

It is known that affective touch leads to broad cortical activations including posterior STS, key region of the so-cial brain. Our goal is to discover if a similar pattern of activation can be observed in 5-months-old infants, or whether thedevelopment of this cortical specialization results from extensive postnatal experience.Over two studies we used functional-Near-InfraRed-Spectroscopy (fNIRS) to compare social touch (a human caress) tonon-social touch (a caress performed with a spoon in study1 -n=22- or with an electric toothbrush in study2 -n=17-).In study1 we found similar patterns of activation to the social and non-social stimulus. In study2 we report broad responsesto the non-social stimulus, but, to our surprise, we found no activations to the human caress.In light of these results we conclude that it is possible that at this age discrimination between social and non-social touch inthe posterior temporal lobe is still undergoing specialization.

Do we see things better when we know grammar?

Language affects perception. But how? Recent findings (Boutonnet & Lupyan, 2015; Bocanegra, Poletiek &Zwaan, submitted) suggest a dissociation between perception that is mediated as compared to not mediated by language.One explanation is that language –that is combinatorial in nature- stresses the separate features of objects. We investigatedthe effect of combinatorial (two words) and non-combinatorial (one word) labels on the perceptual separation of features invisual recognition. Participants were trained to categorize meaningless objects with two dimensions: shape and height. Eachcategory had either a one word name; or a two words name reflecting its features. Participants then were tested on new objects. Combinatorial labels enhanced categorization performance as compared to single labels. This suggests that language, bydecomposing objects into parts, might drive dimension separation in vision as well.

Patterns of Cortical Activation Correlate With Speech Understanding AfterCochlear Implantation

Cochlear implantation is a standard intervention for deafness, yet the ability of implanted patients to understandspeech varies widely. To better understand this variability, we used functional near-infrared spectroscopy to image auditorycortex activation in response to different classes of sound and compared that to behavioral measures of speech perception. Bothcontrol and implanted participants with good speech perception exhibited greater cortical activity to natural speech than tounintelligible speech. In contrast, implanted participants with poor speech perception produced pronounced cortical activationacross stimulus classes. Moreover, the ratio of cortical activation in response to normal speech relative to that of scrambledspeech directly correlated with their comprehension scores, though not with auditory threshold, age, side of implantation,or time after implantation. Because implanted adults with low speech perception scores produced indistinguishable corticalactivation across stimulus classes without preferential response to speech, we interpret this as demonstration of compensatoryprocessing effort.

Encouraging Fruit and Vegetable Consumption Through Intuitive TheoryBuilding

Although routinely informed of the benefits of fruits and vegetables, Americans eat far short of the recommendedamounts. Instead of just telling people that fruits and vegetables are healthy, providing a compelling causal and teleologicalexplanatory framework could increase both people’s conviction about their health benefits and commitment to increasing theamount and variety of fruits and vegetables they consume. Our brief intervention: (1) emphasizes that fruits and vegetables havethousands of health-promoting phytochemicals, well beyond just vitamins, (2) describes clear causal mechanisms by whichthese foods ensure cellular health, (3) draws an analogy between the benefits of plant-based foods and the power of plant-derived medicines, and (4) explains that plants contain abundant nutrients because they must manufacture these chemicals fortheir own survival. This novel intervention improved understanding and increased participants’ intentions to eat more fruits andvegetables, illustrating how intuitive theories can shape motivation for behavioral change.

Scientific Reasoning Ability in Middle Schoolers related to MasterMind DiscoveryStrategy

A study, investigating the relationship between scientific reasoning and the capacity to discover the strategy to playan hypothetico-deductive game (MasterMind), posits that students being able to discover Complex Strategies (vs. GeneralStrategy, Feedback Related, No Strategy) were also, on average, performing higher on our measure of scientific reasoning,itself composed of evaluative, experimental and scientific knowledge measures. In addition to bridge the discovery of complexstrategies with higher SR ability, the finding also suggests the necessity to integrate rule discovery exercises in curriculum to 1-practice while 2- recognize valid reasoning procedures. Finally, inquiring about the middle schooler’s capacity to recognize themost effective strategy, will help to assess the class level as a whole. Such assessment will help the teacher identify some needsand target effective lessons to explain and facilitate the transfer of CoV strategies to novel situations, as suggested by “real life”problem demands.

Neural responses decrease while performance increases with practice: A neuralnetwork model

Why do neural responses decrease with practice? We used a predictive neural network model of sentence processing(St. John & McClelland, 1990) to simulate neural responses during language understanding, and examined the model’s correlateof neural responses (specifically, the N400 component), measured as stimulus-induced change in hidden layer activation, acrosstraining. N400 magnitude first increased and then gradually decreased over training while comprehension performance atthe output steadily rose with practice. These results fit the developmental trajectory of N400 amplitudes. Importantly, theyalso address the reduction of neural activation with practice. In the model, the reduction is due to continuous adaptation ofconnection weights over training. As connection weights between hidden and output layer grow stronger, less hidden layeractivation is necessary to efficiently modulate the output. This shift of labor from activation to connection weights might be animportant mechanism contributing to the reduction of neural activation with practice.

Age-related top-down and bottom-up guidance on eye movements when searchingin real-world scenes

Efficient selection of targets is crucial in everyday activities across the lifespan. Studies reporting age-related declinehave, however, typically utilised arrays of simple, unrealistic objects. Using real-world scenes, we investigated how reliability ofscene semantics (consistent vs. inconsistent targets), target template specificity (name vs. precise picture) and target perceptualsalience influence oculomotor search behaviour in older vs. young viewers. Aging resulted in slower search considering initialsaccade latency, time and number of fixations to locate the target, and verification of object-template matching. No groupdifferences emerged in accuracy and in search facilitation due to a pictorial template or a semantically consistent target. Targethigh salience enhanced efficiency in both groups, with stronger effects in older viewers. Aging seems therefore to lead to anoverall search speed reduction not due to specific deficits in utilisation of scene semantic guidance or in target recognition, andpossibly reduced by enhancing target perceptual guidance.

The Long and Short of It: The Role of Verb Stem Vowel Duration in SentenceProcessing

When native English speakers say active and passive sentences, verb stems are longer in passive sentences thanin their active counterparts (Stromswold et al., 2002; Rehrig et al., 2015) because phrase-final lengthening and polysyllabicshortening cause the verb stem vowel to be longer in passives (Aveni et al., 2016; Mayro et al., 2016). Eye-tracking and gatingstudies of unaltered sentences revealed that listeners are able to predict whether a sentence is active or passive prior to hearingthe inflection on the verb (Stromswold et al., 2002; 2016). To examine whether listeners use vowel duration in online sentencecomprehension, we lengthened the vowel in half of the active verb stems and shortened it in half of the passive verb stems.Reaction times were longer for sentences with altered verb stem vowels (p < .001), consistent with listeners using verb stemvowel duration as a predictive cue in online comprehension.

Recycling or Trash Bin? Modeling Consumers’ Recycling Behavior in a FieldStudy

What affects people’s behavior when they dispose items? The distance hypothesis predicts that the number ofmisplaced items is a function of the distance of an appropriate bin. We categorized and mapped bins at 140 locations onthe campus of a major research university in the Midwest and calculated the distances between adjacent bins. The distancehypothesis predicts that users dispose more recyclables in single, isolated trash bins than in trash bins that are paired withrecycling bins. Likewise, it is expected that more trash items can be found in isolated compared to paired recycling bins. Weconducted a field study that involved systematic comparisons of matched locations and focused on behavioral data that wereobtained through systematic audits of trash and recycling bins. The study provided partial support for the distance hypothesis,which was supported for certain items. The role of item difficulty and weather conditions will be discussed.

Is Neurocomputational Self-Organization a Core Mechanism of AGI Systems?

Artificial General Intelligence (AGI) is a term that describes a variant of a Strong AI revival in the mind sciences.Irrespective of its definition limits, and leaving aside the non-scientific metaphysical or philosophical aspirations, AGI studiesthe feasibility and implementation aspects of artificial systems that would have the capacity of domain non-specific (domain-general) human-level intelligence.The importance of self-organization in natural neural systems as well as in neuromimetic computational systems, especiallythe class of Self-Organizing Map (SOM) neural networks, has been extensively demonstrated and supported in the literature.Neurocomputational self-organization exhibits unique characteristics, including non-deterministic epigenetic (post-genetic) be-havior, which enable direct functional and structural comparisons with the neocortex more than most existing relevant compu-tational mechanisms. If the problem of artificial general intelligence is approached from a biologically relevant computationalstandpoint then SOM mechanisms are currently a very strong candidate as a core component of a computational AGI system.

Does a present bias influence exploratory choice?

Balancing exploration and exploitation is difficult, and across a wide variety of situations under-exploration ofuncertain alternatives appears prevalent. We propose that one possible cause of under-exploration is present bias, wherebyimmediate rewards (like those gained from exploitation) loom larger than future rewards (like those gained from exploration).This possible cause of under-exploration is not addressed by past studies, in which choices generally yield token rewards thatare converted to money at the end of the experiment, removing the inter-temporal aspect of the decision-making process. Toaddress this issue, we developed an exploratory choice task with immediately-consumed rewards. We then tested whetherwhether imposing a temporal delay before the consumption of rewards increased exploration by decreasing present bias, andreport on our results.

Everyday object affordance enhances automatic inhibitory control: an ERP study

High affordance stimuli are associated with an enhancement in the activation of the corresponding motor programs.Such over-activation of motor programs may imply a decrease in performances based on inhibitory control. However, recentdata suggest that high affordance stimuli are associated with a widespread privileged neural activation that goes beyond motorrepresentations. In this case, we can expect that high affordance objects will be associated to a higher level of flexibility inan oddball task with Go-NoGo procedure. By measuring ERPs, we observed that, in the case of high affordance objects,the amplitude of the N200 is decreased when the inhibition of the motor response is more difficult. Data suggests that highaffordance objects facilitate inhibitory control, probably due to a higher activation of automatic attentional resources.

Metacognitive Monitoring of Internal and External Storage and Retrieval

The ability to monitor our cognitive performance (i.e., metacognitive monitoring) is central to efficient functioning.Research investigating this ability has focused largely on tasks that rely exclusively on internal processes. However, our day-to-day cognitive activities often consist of mixes of internal and external processes. For example, we can offload memory demandsonto external media (e.g., computers, paper). In the present investigation, we expand research on the metacognitive monitoringof performance to this domain. Specifically, we examine participant’s ability to accurately monitor their performance in tasksthat require them to rely on only their internal processes (e.g., short term memory to remember a series of letters) and tasks thatrequire them to rely on both (e.g., paper and pencil to remember a series of letters). Results suggest that the former results insuperior monitoring relative to the latter. Implications for understanding metacognition in more distributed cognitive domainswill be discussed.

From Concrete Examples to Abstract Relations: A model-based neuroscienceapproach to how people learn new categories

The ability to form relational categories for objects that share few features in common is a hallmark of humancognition. However until recently, neuroimaging research largely focused solely on how people acquire categories defined byfeatures. In the current electroencephalography (EEG) study, we examine how relational and feature-based category learningcompare in well-matched learning tasks. Building on a previous functional magnetic resonance imaging study by our labo-ratory, we capitalise on the rich temporal information offered by EEG. Focusing on the neural dynamics of how people learncategory memberships over individual trials in an experimental task, we investigate how these single trial dynamics modulatecomputational estimates from decision-making modelling frameworks. Specifically, by sorting participants’ individual trialsby their position in the experimental sequence we observe striking relationships between EEG dynamics (e.g., frontal thetaoscillations and P300 component) and feature-based and relational categorisation behaviour.

Progress in building a machine that can ask interesting and informative questions

Asking creative questions is a hallmark of human cognition. In comparison, machine learning systems that attemptto mimic this ability are still extremely limited (e.g., current chatbots ask questions based on preprogrammed routines). In thepresent work, we developed a computational model of question generation. Based on a corpus of questions collected from onlineparticipants playing an information-seeking game, we designed a “grammar of questions.” The grammar is powerful enough torepresent all human questions we collected and thus defines the “question space.” Given a particular context (game scenario),people are more likely to ask (generate) some questions that others. Our computational model predicts these likelihoods, thatis, a probability distribution over the question space. In addition, the model can generalize to novel contexts. Key modelingredients are informativity, compositionality, and length of a question.

Shifting backward to say what’s front? Spatial referencing of dorsal objectarrangements

When referring to spatial arrangements of two objects in the visual field, German native speakers prefer reflectionas a subtype of the relative frame of reference. Whether this preference transfers to objects in one’s back and whether a mentalturn has to precede such dorsal references (turn hypothesis), has recently been explored in studies implementing questionnaires.However, the results hardly supported the turn hypothesis and rather suggested backward projection as an alternative strategy fordorsal references. To test the two assumptions more rigorously, a series of experiments implemented dorsal object arrangementsin interview situations and induced dorsal perspectives via turning, shifting or reflecting the actual view of participants. Acrossexperiments and conditions, backward projection consistently emerged as the preferred referencing strategy and only a smallproportion of dorsal references accorded with the turn hypothesis. Participants’ retrospective descriptions supported this patternand suggested backward projection to be involved in dorsal referencing.

The time course of Intentional Binding

Environmental stimuli caused by actions (i.e., effects) are perceived earlier than stimuli not caused by actions. Thisphenomenon is termed intentional binding (IB), and serves as implicit measure of sense of agency. We investigated the influenceof effect delay and temporal predictability on IB, measured with the classic clock procedure as the bias to perceive the effectas temporally shifted towards the action. For short delays, IB increased with delay (Experiment 1: 200 ms, 250 ms, 300 ms)and this initial increase declined for longer delays (Experiment 2: 100 ms, 250 ms, 400 ms). These results extend previousfindings showing IB to decrease with increasing delays for delay ranges of 250 ms to 650 ms. Further, the indication that IB,that is, sense of agency, might be maximal for different delays depending on the specific characteristics and context of actionand effect, has important implications for human-machine interfaces.

A positive attitude increases subjective life expectancy

Subjective life expectancy (SLE) has been related to psychological variables, such as optimism. Based on previousstudies where positive attitude was related with longer lifetime, the present study examined whether modifying participants’attitude would influence their SLE. Therefore, 50 participants were randomly assigned either to a positive or to a neutral attitudegroup. During one week, participants of the positive (neutral) group, had to choose the three most accurate positive (neutral)sentences (among 22) to describe their day. After this week, they had to estimate the probability of being 60, 70, 80, or 90years old (traditional measure) and to situate themselves on a line representing their lifetime (spatial based measure). Resultsshow that 1) a more positive attitude increased SLE more than a neutral one, 2) the spatial based measure was sensitive to theintervention and 3) both measures correlated positively with participants’ optimism.

Probability matching in choice behavior influenced by virtual rewards

We recognize the amount of ”reward” according to our choices. In repeated binary choice tasks, human behaveaccording to the theoretical basis of ”probability matching” (Shanks et al., 2002), which has been advocated in several studies.However, the quality of reward may influence their choice-behavior. It is acknowledged that the sensitivity of values for gains orlosses differs among individuals because of risk aversion (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). We conducted a series of experimentsto investigate how participants’ choices change when the ratio of hit-items was set up. Virtual rewards, -3/0, -3/+3, and 0/+3point for each choice, were given to participants. The results showed that the choice-ratio of the weighted correct side washigher in conditions involving losses, suggesting that participants’ choices indicate risk aversion even though rewards werevirtual. Our results suggest that probability matching can be found only when people implicitly recognize their choices haveno loss.

ASR Systems as Models of Phonetic Category Perception in Adults

Adult speech perception is tuned to efficiently process native phonetic categories, causing difficulties with certainnon-native categories. For example, Japanese has no equivalent of the distinction between American English /r/ and /l/ and na-tive speakers of Japanese have a hard time discriminating between these two sounds. Here, we ask whether standard AutomaticSpeech Recognition (ASR) systems trained on large corpora of continuous speech can make correct quantitative predictionsregarding such non-native phonetic category perception effects. By training an ASR system on language L1 and evaluatingit on language L2, we obtain predictions for a native L1 speaker tested on L2 phonetic contrasts. Using a variety of L1 andL2, we show that ASR models correctly predict several well-documented effects. Beyond the immediate results, our evaluationmethodology, based on a machine version of ABX discrimination tasks, opens the possibility of a more systematic investigationof computational models of phonetic category perception.

Which test to perform? Modeling utility of medical tests: information gain,patient risk and financial costs

In medical diagnosis, as in many cognitive domains, asking the right questions is crucial. Medical tests differ notonly in the type of information they provide, but in their financial costs and physical risks to a patient. We develop a modelthat combines informational and cost constraints, describing specific medical scenarios of a patient with realistic symptoms.We then define a finite number of existing medical tests that are available in this situation. The tests differ in their sensitivityand specificity concerning different possible underlying diseases as well as in their financial costs and the physical risks theypose to a patient. Combining these, we compare the utilities of the different tests if performed alone as well as if performed incombination. We show how purely informational considerations are not adequate for the analysis of such a scenario; test costsand patient outcomes must also be taken into account.

Intuitive system control: Challenging the standard model of dynamic decisionmaking

Dynamic decision making (DDM) is usually operationalized in a way that subjects explore and control computer-simulated dynamic systems consisting of interconnected variables. Most authors in the field agree on a “standard model ofDDM” that assumes that decision makers have to learn the causal structure of the system through appropriate explorativebehavior before they can bring the system to given goal states. This strategy draws heavily on cognitive ressources, such asworking memory. The standard model predicts that performance in DDM, as well as structural knowledge should be severelyimpaired when a second cognitive task has to be executed while exploring the system. An experiment with a dual task as themain factor revealed no differences in knowledge and performance between the conditions. Participants in both conditionsappeared to rely on rudimentary structural knowledge and adopted intuitive strategies. We interprete the findings within a dualprocessing framework.

Learning Temporal Generative Neural Codes for Biological Motion Perceptionand Inference

We introduce a modular recurrent neural architecture, which learns distributed, generative temporal models of bio-logical motion. It encodes modal visual and proprioceptive (angular) biological motions separately by means of autoencoders,structuring respective postures, motion directions, and motion magnitudes separately. The submodal encoders are interdepen-dent by predicting each other’s next autoencoder states temporally. As a result, distributed attractor states can develop fromself-generated motions. We show that the architecture is able to synchronize its activities across modalities towards overallconsistent action-encoding attractors. Moreover, the developing spatial and temporal structures can complete partially observ-able actions, e.g., when only providing visual information. Furthermore, we show that the network is capable of simulatingwhole-body actions without any sensory stimulation, thus imagining unfolding actions. Finally, we show that the network isable to infer the visual perspective on a biological motion. Thus, the neural architecture enables embodied perspective takingand action inference.

The Sufficiency Principle: Predicting when children will regularize inconsistentlanguage variation

Children exposed to inconsistent language variation regularize this variation in their productions (Hudson-Kam &Newport, 2005). Existing demonstrations of regularization observe this behavior when the signal-to-noise ratio is greater-than-or-equal-to 40%, but whether regularization occurs when the dominant form is less widespread has not been investigated. Arecent computational model, the Sufficiency Principle, quantifies when a pattern is widespread enough to generalize (Yang,2016): Let R be a generalization over N items, of which M are attested to follow R. R extends to all N items iff: N-M

Impaired phonological processing of lexical tones in Cantonese speakers withcongenital amusia

Congenital amusia is a lifelong musical disorder. It has been found that tonal-language speakers with amusia areimpaired in lexical tone perception. But it has also been found that tonal-language experience compensates the deficit in certainscenario, reducing prevalence rate of amusia in speakers of a highly complex tonal-language – Cantonese. Thus it remainsunclear whether lexical tone perception, especially its phonological processing, is impaired in Cantonese-speaking amusics.This study investigated the categorical perception of a continuum of lexical tone stimuli and pure tone analogues in Cantonese-speaking amusics and controls. The amusics showed reduced discrimination peak across the categorical boundary comparedto controls in lexical tone condition, suggesting impaired categorical perception; in pure tone condition, the amusics showedinferior performance on both between- and within-category discriminations, suggesting a deficit in auditory pitch processing.These findings indicate that phonological processing of tone is impaired in Cantonese-speaking amusics, despite possiblecompensation effect.

The influence of word-order harmony on structural priming in artificial languages

Structural priming occurs when interlocutors copy the syntactic structure of their partners’ utterances, and is di-agnostic of their underlying representations. We trained adult participants on an artificial ‘alien’ language in which nounsappeared with adjectives or numerals in two-word phrases; participants then used that language to communicate with an alieninterlocutor. Input languages had variable word-order with the two modifier types tending to appear on the same side of thenoun (harmonic) or on different sides of the noun (non-harmonic). Participants in all conditions acquired the dominant or-der of their input; however, structural priming only occurred within modifier types (e.g. encountering Numeral-Noun primedNumeral-Noun order only, not Adjective-Noun), even for participants exposed to harmonic input where both modifier typespatterned the same way. This suggests that the abstract representations tapped by structural priming in rapidly-learnt artificiallanguages encode distinctions that are not based purely on distributional properties of the input.

Recently rewarded task-irrelevant stimuli do not distract 2-year-olds during visualsearch

In adults, stimuli associated with reward capture attention, even when task-irrelevant, resulting in distraction (Awhet al., 2012). Here we examine whether rewarded stimuli capture attention in 2-year-old children. Toddlers (N = 46, mean age:28;10, range: 19;16 - 36;18) performed a visual search task where the target switched between blocks. Search arrays consistedof the current target, a previous target, and six feature conjunction distractors. On each trial, the current target was cued, andfollowing a fixed search period, rotated as a reward. We used a Tobii T120 eye-tracker to record toddlers’ eye-movements.Following a target switch, toddlers fixated the current target before the previous target, despite the previous target’s recentreward history F(1, 44) = 31.183, p < 0.001). Our study is one of the first to investigate the early development of reward-basedattentional selection.

Disambiguating Disfluencies: What Do Speech Disfluencies Tell Us About SpeechProduction?

Speech disfluencies occur frequently in spontaneous speech but their source is unclear. Disfluencies can take severalforms, most commonly as verbalized disfluencies such as “um”, “uh”, and “so”, as well as silent pauses. In the presentexploratory study we examined the relationship between disfluencies as distinct entities, individual differences in workingmemory capacity, and linguistic markers of complexity. We found that disfluencies diverge in their relationship with thesevariables. The “um” disfluency was most closely related to working memory capacity and linguistic complexity. The “uh”disfluency was associated with infrequent word production. The “so” disfluency predicted of the number of words produced.Silent disfluencies were not related to working memory capacity. However, micro-pauses were related to word production,and macro-pauses were negatively correlated with the “so” disfluency. Results are discussed in terms of potential relationshipsbetween disfluencies and speech production processes.

Phonological Competition during Spoken-Word Recognition in Infants and Adults

An ongoing debate concerns whether spoken word recognition happens in an incremental or continuous manner(Marslen-Wilson & Zwitserlood, 1989; McClelland & Elman, 1986). In the current study, participants (31 adults and 49 infantsaged 24-30months) were presented with four images while they heard a sentence like “Look at the cat”. Among the imageswas one object that rhymed with the spoken word, one object that shared its onset and two phonologically unrelated objects.Growth curve analysis of eye-tracking data revealed that adults preferentially fixated onset competitors over unrelated objectssoon after word onset but did not preferentially fixate rhyme competitors. Fixations of the onset competitors were modulatedby the degree to which the onsets of the three remaining competitors were phonologically similar to the spoken word. Infantsshowed no preference for either type of phonologically related competitor. The absence of a rhyme effect contradicts continuoustheories of spoken word recognition.

Word order rules in business name binomials

Naming practices offer a window onto linguistic processes of productivity that rely on input from interacting streamsof information. Previous studies have looked at proper personal names and binomial combinations of proper personal names toshow that phonological features such as rhythm, semantic features such as gender, and corpus features such as word frequencyplay an important role in naming and ordering of names. In comparison to personal names, business names tend to be morediverse in terms of constituent structure, often incorporating binomial constructions that may or may not consist of proper namesthemselves. In this study, we investigate whether the ordering of binomials in business names reflects the features identifiedin previous work, with a focus on the following: syllable count, metrical stress, animacy, concreteness, word frequency, andbinomial frequency. We report here on an initial analysis of data from the Yelp Dataset Challenge.

The time course of colour guidance in realistic scene search

Colour is a source of attentional guidance and object segmentation when viewing a scene. In an eye-tracking study,we examined its role during search of targets placed in consistent or inconsistent locations within realistic scene contexts. Boththe target template and the whole scene were presented in full colour or grayscale. Colour presence did not influence earlysearch, considering latency, direction or gain of the first saccade, but affected later phases, with longer scene scanning andmore fixations required to locate the target in the grayscale condition, which also lengthened verification of template-objectmatching. These effects were enhanced in inconsistent scenes. Our results suggest that observers may not utilise colour cueswhen initiating scene inspection during search but also that colour information modulates efficiency of the search process interms of attentional selection and object recognition, in particular when the context of the scene does not provide reliablehigh-level guidance.

Perceptual decision making from correlated samples

The optimal perceptual decision making strategy for weighting serially presented information depends on the degreeof sample dependence. Uniform weighting produces optimal estimates from independent samples, but increases in autocorre-lation should be matched by increasing and symmetric overweighting of early and late samples in order to maintain optimalperformance.In the current experiment, participants (N = 30) observed briefly presented sequences of eight dots and were asked to estimatetheir center of mass by dragging the cursor. The autocorrelation of the series was manipulated in two distinct blocks (either 0or .7). Preliminary results show that the weight assignment to uncorrelated inputs did not differ significantly from the optimaluniform allocation. In contrast, in the high-dependence block participants used different weighting profiles - overweightingthe first and/or last samples of the sequence. This suggests that humans flexibly adapt to changes in statistical structure in thepredicted direction of optimality.

Optimality of visual search under ambiguous stimuli

A hallmark of optimal decision making is that cues are weighted by their reliability. Previous studies have reportedevidence for reliability weighting in several human perceptual decision-making tasks in which sensory noise was the onlypossible source of errors. Here we use a target detection task to test whether optimality generalizes to situations where stimulusambiguity is an additional source of uncertainty. Target and distractor orientations were drawn from distributions with differentmeans and the level of ambiguity was varied through the amount of overlap between the two distributions. In line with previousstudies, we found clear evidence for sensory reliability-weighting, regardless of the level of ambiguity. However, using a richerset of models than before, we also found that the estimated weights deviated from the optimal ones. Finally, we found no effectof ambiguity level on task efficiency, which suggests that subjects optimally accounted for this source of uncertainty.

It is new, but will it be good? Context-driven exploration of novel options

How do people decide whether to try out novel options? We argue that they utilize contextual information toefficiently generalize from learned functional relations in order to decide between known or novel options. In a contextualmulti-armed bandit task, in which rewards are a noisy function of observable features, we assess participants’ preferences fornewly introduced options. We show that participants preferably choose a novel option if its features indicate high rewards,but shun the option if its features indicate low rewards, a behavior that can only be explained by functional generalization.Moreover, we assess people’s preferences for novel options that have medium rewards to test whether they prefer options lesssimilar to experienced options, consistent with choices guided by uncertainty. Given that novel options normally come withobservable features, we argue that contextual learning is a parsimonious yet powerful explanation of behavior in the face ofnovelty.

Mathematical Symbol Recognition in Children

In early mathematics development the development of symbolic skills is critical to math learning. Our mathematicalsystem is based on Arabic number symbols which do not provide any semantic meaning relevant to the number words andsymbols. In order to succeed in math one must be able to recognize and understand the meaning of numeric and other math-ematical symbols. Little is known about the development of these symbolic skills. The current study examines 4 -7-year-oldchildren’s understanding and recognition of number and arithmetic symbols. The youngest children made significant errors inidentifying numbers as well as confusing letter symbols with number symbols. Results reveal a developmental progression ofnumeric symbol recognition.

Speaking in English, sorting in Chinese: interaction in L2 can reinforce existingcategories in L1

How does interaction affect categorization, and how might this vary between native and non-native speakers? Whenpeople use shared labels to categorize objects, they categorize more similarly to each other. We investigated whether interactionleads non-native speakers to categorize in the same way as native speakers. In six rounds, L1-English and L2-English/L1-Mandarin speakers individually categorized dishware using labels (BOWL, PLATE), then discussed their categories or anunrelated topic after each round. L2 speakers’ categories shifted following category-relevant interaction with L1 speakers, buttheir categories did not become more L1-like. Unexpectedly, category-relevant interaction reduced alignment within pairs andwithin language groups; however, this effect was weaker in the L2 than L1 group. Hence, L2 speakers showed a strongertendency than L1 speakers to use categories that were similar to other speakers from their language group. This suggests thatinteraction in an L2 can reinforce L2 speakers’ categories in their L1.

The effect of overt language use on category induction

Successfully solving a problem should help people solve similar problems, but such generalization is often surpris-ingly limited. We investigated generalization performance when people explicitly verbalized solutions to open-ended category-induction ”Bongard problems”, compared to tacitly indicating that they had found a solution. In a Bongard problem people arepresented with an array of items falling into two classes, and have to induce the basis for the classification by working out what(sometimes quite abstract) feature of the items is relevant, from a vast set of possibilities. We measured objective performanceby testing people with new items, and observed how explicitly vs. tacitly expressed solutions affected generalization acrossconcretely similar or abstractly similar problems. For the concretely similar problems, explicitness boosted transfer of correctsolutions. For the abstractly similar problems, there was no evidence of transfer, though there was a general positive effect ofexplictness.

Respecting UP and Despising DOWN: Emotional and Body-based Image inJapanese Verbs

The aim of this study was to examine the image-schematic representations that arise from sentences referring toconcrete/abstract action in Japanese verbs. We used a free positioning task that required the participants to draw the position ofan object in a sentence referring to an agent’s concrete/abstract action and a simple rating task that investigated the agent’s needfor body movement and emotional evaluation for the object. The results showed that the drawn object’s position in not only aconcrete but also an abstract action sentence is changed before and after the action. Further, the results indicated that the heightand distance from the agent to the object in the sentence is related to the emotional evaluation of the agent for the object in thesentence.

A time-series eye-fixation analysis of the similarity-compromise effect inmulti-alternative choice

In decision-making tasks with two attributes and three alternatives, the similarity effect implies that, if the totalexpected utility is equal between two opposite alternatives (i.e., the target and competitor), the probability of the target beingchosen decreases with the addition of the decoy similar to the target. This study demonstrated the similarity-compromiseeffect, wherein the probability of the target being chosen increased with the addition of the decoy, under the same conditionsas the similarity effect, by setting all attribute values of three alternatives to broken numbers rather than rounded numbers.To determine the mechanism underlying this effect, we examined information acquisition patterns using eye-movement datacollected from 37 undergraduates who made 10 hypothetical purchase tasks with two attributes and three alternatives. Time-series analysis of fixation time for the three alternatives revealed dynamic temporal features distinct from those of attractionand compromise effects observed in our previous research.

Relationship between four measures reflecting representations of fractionmagnitude in adults: number line estimation, comparison, calculation of fractions,and immediate serial recall of fractions

Our previous studies (presented at the London meeting of EPS in 2017 and submitted for ICPS 2017) suggested thatimmediate serial recall tasks access magnitude representation of fraction. A subsequent research task is to explore the inter-correlations among four tasks stimulating representations of fraction magnitude: an immediate serial recall task with fractionstimuli and three typical tasks, number line estimation, comparison, and calculation of fractions. The purpose of this study is toexamine whether our new measure, the size of the magnitude similarity effect on immediate serial recall of fractions, relates toother typical measures for adults. The results from 36 university students showed a significant correlation between the size ofthe magnitude similarity effect and the RT of fraction calculation tasks but no correlations among any other tasks. This resultsuggests that it is necessary to reexamine what tasks could access the magnitudinal representation of fraction in adults.

Slow Change: The Visual Context for Real World Learning

The visual world can be a noisy and dynamic place. This poses problems for novice word learners who must mapheard names to objects in scenes with their many potential referents. In this study, we consider how the visual stability andselectivity of scenes from the first-person perspective may simplify the learning problem. 12- and 30-month-old children worehead cameras and played with a large set of toys. Through analyzing head-camera video frame by frame, we measured the rateof change of scene information in the natural world of children in this context, and found that the visual world from the child’sperspective changes continuously. However, this change is slow and incremental – tiny steps – even across increasingly largertimescales. We discuss the importance of understanding the dynamics of real world environments for understanding visualprocessing, sustained attention, and early object name learning.

Effects of attention to emergent phenomena on rule discovery

In this study, we focused on effects of finding of emergent phenomena in rule discovery. In the experiment, weused Conway’s Game of Life, which generates high-order phenomena from fundamental rules. Our research question is torealize the effects of attention to emergent phenomena on finding the fundamental rules. The two experimental conditions(chaotic and static) differed only in initial states. In the chaotic condition, the initial state consisted some Methuselahs, whichtake long period until they become stable. On the other hand, in the static condition, the initial condition consisted manyemergent patterns: still lifes and oscillators, which repeat same pattern in short period. We classified the hypotheses reportedby the participants to either mentioning about emergent phenomena or not. This result revealed that people might see emergentphenomena not only in the static condition but also in the chaotic condition, which do not include the emergent patters.

Scheduling system delays for optimal user performance: Don’t predict time; lettime predict!

System delays affect user performance and experience when interacting with computers. We investigated the effectsof different prediction relations between delay duration and response requirements on user performance. In one experiment,delay duration predicted, to different degrees (50 % vs. 75 % vs. 100 %), the following system response. Predictabilitysubstantially increased users’ response speed, while adaptation was highly flexible, between different prediction regimes. Ina second experiment, users’ responses predicted system delay duration. Compared to the first experiment, users’ responsespeed was moderately increased, while the adaptation was rather inflexible across different prediction regimes. In a thirdexperiment, we directly compared both types of predictability. The results confirmed a stronger and more flexible adaptationeffect when time predicted the system response, compared to when users’ responses predicted time. These findings haveimportant implications for scheduling data transmission rates across different users in internet-based parallel computing.

Computational Foundations of Cultural Evolution: Modeling the Emergence ofSystems from Higher-order Probabilistic Inference

Cumulative cultural evolution in humans is the process through which behaviours gain structure and complexityas they are transmitted from one generation of learners to the next. A central challenge in the cultural evolution literatureis to understand how the unique computational principles of human cognition scaffold the emergence of complex behaviouralsystems. I explore how the human ability to make inferences at higher order levels of abstraction can lead to cultural complexity,in two ways: by allowing initially independent behaviours to gradually acquire group-like structure as new learners repeatedlyimpose an expectation for statistical dependence; and by allowing inferences in one domain to be rapidly transferred to newdomains which share features at higher-order levels of abstraction. I model these processes in populations using a probabilisticcognitive model for the acquisition of vowel systems in human language.

Counterfactual thoughts and judgments about morally good actions

Evaluating the morality of an action is affected by thoughts about whether the outcome might have turned outdifferently. We report experimental results that show a moral action effect occurs for judgments about morally good actions.Participants read stories about a morally elevating situation, e.g., an agent is found to be a match as a bone-marrow donor forsomeone else. The agent decided to act or not to act, and the outcome turned out well or it did not turn out well. Participantscreated counterfactual thoughts and they also made judgments about whether the agent should have acted, and whether theagent was morally responsible for the outcome. The results show a moral action effect: participants judged that the actionshould have been taken, and that the agent was morally responsible for the outcome, when the agent acted compared to whenthey did not act, regardless of the outcome.

Gradually ascending sound with accelerating automatic driving vehicle mightchange passengers’ tension or anxiety: analysis of biometrical index.

When people ride an autonomous car, they might feel anxiety because they cannot know how it may move. Addingartificially augmented signals, which represent coming changes of the vehicle, it may be useful to reduce anxiety by changeexpectation. Thus we executed an experiment examining whether ascending sound could decrease passenger’s anxiety, whileriding on virtual autonomous car. In the experiment, participants saw 360-degree computer-graphics world through a head-mounted-display. The stimuli were views from a moving car with 2 speed (19 and 320 km/h), half of which was addedascending and descending sound at first / last 6 secs. Results of the heart-wave analysis as biometric index, i.e., index ofsympathetic nervous (LF/HL), showed a marginal interaction between existence of sounds and the vehicle speed; while soundsreduced participants’ anxiety with high-speed condition, they showed higher tension with sound at slow-speed conditions.

Does sonority influence the syllable segmentation in visual identification?Evidence in French skilled readers.

Many studies focused on the importance of statistical and distributional properties to account for the prelexicaland segmental role of syllable-sized units in silent reading in French. We explored how skilled readers segmented printed(pseudo)words when no reliable statistical cues were available around and within the syllable boundary. We were interestedin how sonority, a universal phonological element, might be a reliable source for syllable segmentation. We tested 160 nativeFrench-speaking adults with pseudowords in which orthographic and phonological statistical properties were (quasi)null for thefirst three letters including the syllable boundary in a revisited version of the paradigm used by Treiman and Chafetz (1987).Five sonority profiles within the syllable boundaries along a continuum from legal to illegal clusters were designed. Our resultsshowed that segmentation does not strictly depend on statistical cues; participants were also sensitive to the legality of thesonority profile to locate the syllable boundary.

Poverty of materials makes recursive combination operation evolvable

Humans can use recursive combination operation in various behaviors; other primates, however, rarely perform thisoperation. In our previous research, using an evolutionary simulation of combination behavior, we showed that recursive com-bination was more adaptive than repetitive combination in cases where the robustness of production or the diversity of productswas required. In this research, we examined the evolvability of recursive combination in combinatorial space parametrizedby kinds of elemental materials and the number of elements per product. Recursive combination evolved in the region of lowkinds of elemental materials and large number of elements per product. This region may be compared with the situation of themiddle stone age when invented diversified tools with limited kinds of materials such as stone, bone, and woods. The recursivecombinatorial operation could scaffold the evolution of general recursive combination abilities including language, technology,music, and mathematics.

Fitting a Stochastic Model to Eye Movement Time Series in a Categorization Task

Our goal is to develop an efficient framework for fitting stochastic continuous-time models to experimental data incognitive psychology. As a simple test problem, we consider data from an eye-tracking study of attention in learning. For eachsubject, the data for each trial consists of the sequence of stimulus features that the subject fixates on, together with the durationof each fixation. We fit a stochastic differential equation model to this data, using the Approximate Bayesian Computationframework. For each subject we infer posterior distributions for the unknown parameters in the model.

Gaze during utterances and silence in L1 and L2 Conversations

Gazing activities during utterances and silence were analyzed in a face-to-face three party conversation setting in anative language (L1) and in a second language (L2). The function of each utterance was categorized according to the GroundingActs defined by Traum (Traum, 1994) so that gazes during utterances could be analyzed from the viewpoint of grounding incommunication (Clark, 1996). Factor analyses of gaze activities showed similar factor structures in L1 and L2 conversations:the first factor was characterized by speakers’ gazes and gazes during silence, and another factor was characterized by listeners’gazes in each condition. Analyses of the participants based on the factor scores, however, showed different tendencies betweenthe two conditions, suggesting that language proficiency affects gaze activities during utterances.

Statistical Learning Contributions to Semantic Knowledge Development

The organization of semantic knowledge according to relations between concepts influences many facets of highercognition. Therefore, understanding the origins of relations knowledge is a key focus of cognitive development research. Thisstudy investigated the contributions of environmental statistical regularities to relations knowledge in preschool-age children.Using CHILDES to estimate co-occurrence between familiar items, we constructed triads consisting of a target, related distrac-tor, and unrelated distractor in which targets and related distractors consistently co-occurred (e.g., sock-foot), belonged to thesame taxonomic category (e.g., sock-coat), or both (e.g., sock-shoe). Using a Visual World paradigm, we then measured rela-tions knowledge as the degree to which children looked at related versus unrelated distractors when asked to look for targets.The results revealed that co-occurrence, regardless of taxonomic relatedness, influenced whether participants looked signifi-cantly more at related versus unrelated distractors. These findings demonstrate that co-occurrence regularities between entitiesin the environment shape knowledge organization.

Improving Number Foundations in Preschoolers: ANS versus SymbolicKnowledge

The current study examined whether preschoolers who are low achievers (LA) on mathematical tasks benefit morefrom a training programme that focuses on magnitude comparisons or ANS abilities (PLUS games) compared to games thattarget symbolic knowledge (DIGIT games).Twenty-four preschoolers played PLUS games and 21 children played DIGIT games 3 times per week for 5 weeks. Perfor-mance scores were compared to 25 typical control children who did not play any games. All children were assessed pre andpost-intervention on Test of Early Mathematics (TEMA), a computerized ANS task, the Give a Number task (Wynn, 1990) toassess cardinality and a counting and Digit Recognition task.The results showed that, although the DIGIT and PLUS groups performed lower than the Control group, both PLUS andDIGIT games improved mathematical abilities in LA children. These results suggest that there is a complex interaction betweenANS, symbolic, and formal mathematical abilities.

Biological and Artificial Perspectives on Metacognition

Metacognition may be broadly understood as awareness, monitoring, and regulation of an intelligent agent’s owninternal processing, a “thinking about thinking”. The cognitive complexity and self-maintenance value of this introspectiveskillset has considerable current interest in the study of both biological and artificial intelligence, with intriguing parallels.Study of metacognition in some nonhuman species and Biologically-Inspired Cognitive Architecture (BICA) systems reflectevidence of, at best, an attenuated form of the elaborated human manifestation, with ongoing difficulties in operationalizingmetacognitive components and traits. A linked exploration of these “inhuman” forms of metacognition may better clarify thelocus of divergence from the human form and illuminate the role of the skill in supporting potentially emergent cognitivetraits, from self-recognition to Theory of Mind understanding. The current review will take a comparative approach in as-sessing metacognitive systems in nonhuman biological and artificial agents in pursuit of clarity for future methodological andconceptual directions.

Predicting Future Performance in an ITS system via Gradient BoostingClassification

Gradient Boosting Classification (GBC) models are well known to machine learning and artificial intelligence.Having the ability to predict user performance is imperative to the outcomes and purpose of an intelligent tutoring system.The Center for the Study of Adult Literacy (CSAL) intelligent tutoring system aims to improve reading comprehension inlow-literacy adult learners. A GBC was applied to preliminary data gathered from high-literacy adult readers (N =1800 ob-servations). Our model was shown high accuracy in predicting users’ correct/incorrect responses to our multiple choice items.Specifically, users’ reaction times and order of question presentation are important features of the model to consider. Lessimportant features are difficulty of the item and the users reading ability. Our next steps are to apply GBC to high-literacycollege students, followed by low-literacy readers, as a test set. Our eventual goal is to predict correctness prior to scoring.

Interactivity, Stereotype threat, and Working memory

The purpose of the current study was to investigate the role of interactivity (the use of pen and paper) in defusingthe impact of stereotype threat on difficult mental arithmetic tasks, covering all four operations of mathematics. Eighty-four16-year-old girls from secondary schools in South East England (UK) participated in this study. Participants carried out (in aneducational setting) difficult, multi-digit mental arithmetic tasks in a stereotype threat or control condition, crossed with interac-tivity or no interactivity. The primary dependent variables were the overall performance of the participants in accuracy, latencyto solution, working memory, and mathematics anxiety. Increased interactivity enhanced mental arithmetic performance. Girlsin the stereotype condition performed worse in the working memory test than the participants in the control condition. However,there was no causal role of working memory in reduced mathematics performance under stereotype threat. Reasons for thisfinding and recommendations for future studies are discussed.

Judging Magnitude: Is there a Common Cognitive System for Different Types ofMagnitude Judgments?

It has been suggested that a common cognitive system is employed in magnitude judgments across multiple modal-ities (Walsh, 2003). To test this theory, we examined whether performance on magnitude judgments of number, surface area,duration, and loudness correlated with each other in both magnitude comparison (e.g., determine which is more), and mag-nitude estimation (e.g., if magnitude 1 value = 100, estimate the value of magnitude 2) tasks. For magnitude comparison,significant correlations were observed between number, surface area, and loudness (but not duration) tasks (percent correctmeasured). Similar results were observed for magnitude estimation (mean absolute percent deviation of value estimates fromcorrect measured). These results are indicative of a common cognitive system for at least some magnitude judgment modalities,and suggest that such a system may play a role not only in more-than/less-than magnitude judgments, but also in the process ofassigning numerical values to magnitudes.

Effects of Auditory-Feedback Delays and Musical Roles on Coordinated TimingAsymmetries in Piano Duet Performance

Recent research in human behavioral dynamics has demonstrated that co-actors often successfully achieve jointgoals by adopting functionally asymmetric patterns of behavior. To better understand the evolution of such patterns in anaturalistic musical context, the current study examined how auditory-feedback delays and individual musical roles affectcollective temporal stability and relative adaptability during duet performance. The delays between pianists were short (10-40 ms), bidirectional, and remained constant during a single trial, simulating those typical in internet-mediated performance.Preliminary results show increasingly reduced collective stability for longer delays along with a distinct pattern of asynchroniesacross the points where temporal synchrony would be expected, in which individuals exhibited consistent alternation betweenplaying before or after their co-performer. Furthermore, asynchronies became greater when the two musical parts were lesssimilar. Thus, emerging coordinative dynamics appear to be shaped both by asymmetries in co-performers’ assigned roles andexternal constraints on shared information.

What influences the impact of warning labels in decisions fromdescription-plus-experience

Warning labels can be considered as descriptions added to repeated decisions-from-experience. Limited researchso far has looked at the theoretical integration of decisions from descriptions and decisions from experience when the two areavailable concurrently. We explore how the presence and timing of such warning labels influence behaviour. We expected theprovision of warning labels to subsequently reduce risk taking, and that more prior experience before the appearance of suchlabels would lead to stronger habit formation and reduce their behavioural impact. Instead, we show how the appearance ofdescriptions warning against risks can have a perverse effect of increasing risk taking. And counter-intuitively, we also observethat the amount of previous experience prior to the appearance of descriptions does not impact behaviour. Briefly presentedwarning labels also have the same effect as constantly present ones. All of these findings have strong implications on the designof effective warning labels.

Later lexical development in bilinguals

We investigated object naming in Dutch-French bilingual children to determine the developmental trajectory of thecross-language convergence in naming patterns shown by bilingual adults. We collected name choices for nearly 200 commonhousehold containers from French-Dutch simultaneous bilinguals of 6 different age groups, along with monolingual controlgroups. Multidimensional scaling analysis on a group level suggests that convergence is present in bilinguals at all ages. Onthe individual level, pairwise between-subject correlations show that monolingual naming patterns in different languages showa remarkable correspondence at younger ages. Between age 5 and adulthood, the naming patterns of monolingual childrendemonstrate increasing divergence as they learn the language specificities of their L1. Bilingual children, however, maintain afully converged naming pattern up till age 10. They start learning some language-specific idiosyncrasies from age 12 onwards,but never to the extent of monolinguals. We propose a gradual divergence perspective for bilingual lexical development.

An immersive binaural horizon for sonic data analytics

Accessible data analytics—that can be experienced through vision, hearing, and touch—poses a challenge to in-teraction design. It is also a human rights requirement because many societies mandate that all individuals have the right toexperience products and services, yet not every individual accesses media visually. As more data is presented through visual-ization, accessibility for populations who do not access data through vision decreases.Guidelines that claim to make visual media accessible through text fail to translate the iconic properties of visual shapes, thussubtracting affordances for pattern recognition. Non-linguistic sonication can be a means for non-visual pattern recognition.Hearing is optimized for detecting locations on a horizontal plane, and our approach for presenting data analytics recruitsthis optimization by using an immersive binaural horizontal plane. We will demonstrate our approach via two case studies: Asonic translation of a map and a sonic translation of a computational fluid dynamics simulation.

A model of cultural co-evolution of language and mindreading

Language requires mindreading for entertaining communicative intentions, and mindreading in turn profits fromlanguage as a means for sharing mental states. Hence it has been hypothesised that the two skills have co-evolved.We present a Bayesian agent-based model to formalise this hypothesis. This model combines referential signalling withmental states, such that a speaker’s topic choice is probabilistically dependent on their perspective on the world. In order tolearn the language, a learner has to simultaneously infer the speaker’s lexicon and perspective. Learners can solve this task bybootstrapping one with the other, but only if the speaker uses an informative language.We will present results of an iterated learning version of this model, showing that selection on communication results inthe emergence of a fully informative lexicon from scratch. However, selection on perspective-taking alone also results in theemergence of partially-informative lexicons, which is sufficient for inferring others’ perspectives.

Language as a process: An exploration among pre-adolescent Chinese EFLs

This paper reports a reading intervention programme, the LMVS (Linguistically Mediated Visual Search) amongpre-adolescent Chinese EFLs. It sets out to test whether managing the process of silent reading might modify text complexityas perceived. The paper is a combination of two studies. The first study was the development and assessment of a reading com-prehension test. The second study piloted an intervention for pre-adolescents. Item-by-item analysis of students’ performancein the post-test show changes in the perception of item difficulty after the intervention. Chinese EFL struggling readers werefound to be weaker in lexical analysis. They also faced difficulties in decoding main ideas in compound/complex sentences. Inresponse to the analysis, strategies were developed for automatic syntactic processing. The paper proposes seeing language asa process, rather than a product so that learner management skills might be prepared for reading intervention.

Influences of the Matching Effects of Cognitive and Emotional Factors on AttitudeChange

This research is aimed to study whether we will have the same attitude change when we have the same intensityof cognitive or emotional level for attitude. People who have higher involvement will be persuaded by central route and lowinvolvement will be persuaded by peripheral route: the matching effect in attitude change. The present study controlled theintensity of both cognitive and emotional factor and instructed participants to express their initial attitude as well as attitudechange under four experimental manipulations. Results showed that only matching effect of emotional factor was found butnot cognitive factor. A connectionist model was therefore built to simulate the processes and found that there would havedifferent thresholds for cognitive and emotional routes and the threshold of cognitive route should be higher than emotionalroute. Implications are proposed based on the behavioral and simulation investigations.

Is infants’ mutual exclusivity response based on preference to novelty or non-nameof an object?

Although “mutual exclusivity (ME)” is the term to refer to the behavior that infants map a novel label onto a novelobject rather than a familiar object, two studies, using preferential looking paradigm, aimed to investigate whether infants’ MEis based on preference to novelty or non-name of an object. In Study 1, 18-month-olds were tested on 2 conditions: familiar-object/novel-object trials with known label and familiar-object/novel-object trials with unknown label. The infants preferredto novel objects before naming but no naming effect found for both conditions. In Study 2, 18-month-olds in the same twoconditions as Study 1 were pre-familiarized to both of novel and familiar objects. The results showed that the naming effectswere found for both conditions, indicating that ME occurred. The findings of the present studies suggest that pre-familiarizationcould be used to validate if 18-month-olds’ ME response is based on non-name preference of an object.

Children’s Attention to Semantic Content versus Emotional Tone: Differencesbetween Two Cultural Groups

People from varied cultural backgrounds differ in their attention to particular aspects of emotional cues. Whereassemantic content explicitly expresses feelings, vocal tone conveys implicit information regarding emotions. This study exam-ined the attention to different emotional cues in European-American and Chinese children. Participants were 121 European-American and 120 Chinese children (4-9 years old). They played two games in which they listened to spoken words and judgedthe pleasantness of the word meaning while ignoring the vocal tone (meaning game) or judged the pleasantness of the vocaltone while ignoring the word meaning (tone game). Preliminary results showed that European-American children paid moreattention to word meaning than did Chinese children. Additionally, older (8-9 years old) Chinese children attended more tovocal tone than did their European-American counterparts. The results suggest that children acquire culturally specific attentionbias by 8-9 years old.

Knowledge partitioning in forecasting

In this study, we would like to examine whether the learned forecasting function can be separated for use by context.The participants were asked to learn to forecast the position of a target, defined as a sine function of trial number. A context cuewas paired with the moving of the target systematically and randomly in two conditions. The learning performance was quitegood in both conditions. In the transfer phases, in the systematic-context condition, some participants learned to rely on contextto direct their prediction (i.e., knowledge partitioning), whereas some others and those in the randomized-context conditionlearned to rely on the concept about the function for forecasting. However, contrary to the precedent knowledge partitioningstudies, the variety of using context or not was found within participants across transfer phases. The modeling results favoredthe associative account over the rule account on accommodating the training and transfer response patterns.

Working Memory and lexical ambiguity resolution in Chinese

Two cross-modal priming experiments were conducted to examine the underlying mechanism of lexical disam-biguation process was in activational nature or in inhibitory approach. In experiment one, forty native Cantonese listeners wererecruited to participate in two tasks (1) a Chinese version reading span task (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980) to measure theirWM capacity and (2) a cross-modal priming task (Yip, 2015). In experiment two, another group of native Mandarin listenerswere recruited to participate in the same two tasks in Mandarin. The results revealed that sentence context had an early effecton the disambiguation processes for both high- and low-WM span groups and the underlying mechanism of the disambiguationprocess for the high-WM span group seemed to be in an inhibitory nature.

Impact of Polarity, Rationality, and Math Ability on Numerical MagnitudeKnowledge

Previous research has shown that numerical magnitude knowledge is related to current mathematic abilities andpredictive of future mathematics performance. Much of this early research examined magnitude knowledge of positive wholenumbers, more recently this has been extended to positive rational numbers. However, research about negative number mag-nitude knowledge is less abundant. The present study aims to understand how different types of magnitude knowledge relateto one another and whether performance differs according to the type of number line scale. Thirteen number line scales wereused to assess 7th grade students’ (N=180) magnitude knowledge of positive and negative, whole and rational numbers. Cor-relational analyses illustrate that performance on most scales are significantly related. Further analyses reveal that students’performance differed depending on the scale’s polarity and the number type of the scale. Moreover, performance differenceswere found to vary according to students’ mathematics classroom ability level. Educational implications are discussed.

Global consequences of local complexity: evidence from recall of visuallypresented nonwords

There is extensive evidence that structural regularities affect the processing of visually-presented words. However,it is not known whether the processing consequences of an orthographic violation are limited to the offending subpart (e.g.,an unattested onset cluster) or apply more globally (e.g., to the entire word). We provide evidence of global disruption fromthe recall of briefly-presented nonwords that were manipulated for degree of orthographic markedness and length. Error rateswere higher for both the initial and final portions of nonwords beginning with more marked onsets; symmetrically, report ofmarked onsets was degraded in words with longer endings. These effects suggest that, as in other visual tasks, the fidelity withwhich one element can be represented depends on the overall stimulus complexity. We present a modified version of rationalmodels of visual word perception in which global effects result from the distribution of a limited processing resource over letterpositions.

Children’s Reasoning about Geometric Footprints

We explored preschool’s children’s understanding of the correspondence of 3-D objects and 2-D faces in a noveltask. In the “footprints” task children were shown a geometric solid, such as a pyramid or a prism, and asked to select whichshape the solid would make if it were dipped in ink and stamped on a piece of paper. Through a latent class analysis of children’serrors we found children differed significantly in their misconceptions about object structure. Three distinct classes of childrenemerged: children who could only match visible faces, children who believed solids have an ‘essential’ face irrespective ofrotation, and children who differentiated faces based on a solid’s rotation. We examined the characteristics of children in eachof these classes using a battery of spatial tasks and numeric tasks. Our results suggest errors found in older children’s andadults’ reasoning about geometric concepts develop prior to formal schooling.

Relevance Theory, Pragmatic Inference and Cognitive Architecture

Relevance Theory (RT: Sperber&Wilson, 1986) argues that human interpretative processes maximize relevance andpostulates that there is a relevance-based procedure that a listener follows when trying to understand utterances. However, Maz-zone (2013) points out that RT fails to explain how speaker-related information, such as the speaker’s abilities or preferences, isincorporated into pragmatic processes. He proposes that situational or goal schemata, with the speaker represented as a compo-nent, are sufficient to activate the hearer’s speaker-related knowledge and further asserts that human communication is drivenby goal management and action rather than relevance maximization. Yet Mazzone cannot fully explain how linguistic meaningand speaker-related knowledge are integrated within a modular system. Based on RT’s cognitive requirements and contempo-rary cognitive theory, we argue that this integration is realized within working memory via production-like conversational ruleswith which the constructed utterance interpretation should be consistent, and present a simple model of this process.

Do you forgive past mistakes of virtual assistant? A study on changing impressionsof virtual character when using its assistance multiple times

We investigated the gain-loss effect of virtual/personal assistant character, which provides intelligent assistance tohumans, and focused on not only the first impression of using the assistance but also changing impression about the characterwhen used multiple times for assistance. The experiment used a fictive retrieval system (searching onomatopoeia), and thevirtual assistant character looked up for suitable words for the user (success), or failed to find the words (mistake). There werethree sessions, differing by the task of character’s mistake; two tasks were successes and one was a failure in each session. Theresults showed that the group of people who had low expectation from its first appearance, formed negative impressions afterthe final mistake, significantly. Consequently, final mistake influenced the formation of negative impression more than othermistakes, thus showing that the final mistake in multiple times of assistance was associated with loss effect.

Knowledge acquiring on event chronology in Russian-language texts

Chaining the sequence of events through awareness of their temporal relations is an important aspect of text un-derstanding. As a rule, text provides only partial knowledge of event unfolding however various types of additional sources(documents, personal diaries, etc.) provide an added knowledge to make chronology more precise.The paper argues the novel approach to automated retrieval of information on temporal relations between events marked inthe text. The data retrieved will provide additions to computer ontology which formally represents the actual events chronology.A system of linguistic algorithms for analyzing the contexts with specific verbal (or linguistic situations) inputs is suggestedwithin the present approach. We use syntactic graphs of the sentences and some grammatical characteristics of the wordsproduced by the system for the automatic syntactic analysis of Russian texts.

The Impact of Presentation Order on Category Learning Strategies: BehavioralData and Self-Reports

The presentation order in supervised categorization learning can influence the category representation. For example,the order can bias a rule-based approach focusing the identification of relevant features or an exemplar-based approach focusingthe similarity of category members. In a blocked design stimuli can either be presented in a way that relevant features overstimuli become obvious or that two succeeding stimuli share as many common features as possible (cf. Mathy & Feldman,2016). In an empirical study with 21 participants we investigated both orders for the 5-4 category structure (Medin & Schaffer,1978) and assessed categorization behavior and self-reports in the first trials. Results suggest that the answer behavior and self-reports during the first trials can be influenced by the presentation order. However, in both groups feature-based and similarity-based explanations were reported. Additionally, the similarity-based group named more feature-based rules including irrelevantfeatures.

Walking dynamics of intertemporal choice

The notion that cognitive processes ”leak” into motor output of decisions inspired much recent process-tracingresearch. In mouse-tracking, an increasingly popular decision-making paradigm, difficult choices lead to increased curvatureof the mouse trajectories towards the unchosen option. Here we explore whether traces of a decision process can be found inits motor output in a more naturalistic setting. Our subjects performed a series of choices between a smaller reward now and alarger reward at some delay. Using Kinect camera, we recorded subjects’ walking trajectories when they moved towards theirpreferred option displayed in one of the corners across the room. We found that deviation of subjects’ trajectories from theideal trajectory increased with delay when they preferred the ”later” option, and decreased with delay in trials where the ”now”option was chosen. Our results suggest that walking trajectory of a person can provide information about their ongoing thoughtprocesses.

A Spatial-Temporal Analysis of a Visual Working Memory Task with EEG andECoG

In this study, we investigated the neural correlates of a visual working memory task. Two experiments were carriedout using scalp electroencephalography (EEG) and Electrocorticography (ECoG), respectively. In each trial, participants judgedwhether a test face had been among a small set of recently studied faces. We used a combination of hidden semi-Markov models(HSMMs) and multi-variate pattern analysis (MVPA) to decompose the neural signal into a sequence of latent stages. Analyzedseparately, EEG and ECoG data yielded converging results on the durations of recovered stages. Combining these stages withthe high spatial resolution of ECoG suggested that activity in the temporal cortex reflected item familiarity in the retrievalstage; and that once retrieval is complete, there is active maintenance of the studied face set in the medial temporal lobe (MTL).During this same period, the frontal lobe guides the decision by means of theta coupling with the MTL.

To organize or not to organize? Examining biases in search strategies using Legobuilding blocks

A widely-accepted notion is that organization can improve task performance and generally allow us to better functionwithin a given task environment (Kirsh, 1995; 1996). However, it remains unclear the extent to which individuals believe thatorganization will help to improve task performance when they are asked to carry out mundane tasks in the real world. Toexamine this, individuals were asked to search through a pile of Lego building blocks for specific pieces. Prior to the searchtask, they were asked their preferred strategy for this task (e.g., organizing vs. not organizing the Lego pile prior to search) andto estimate how much time and effort each strategy would take for task completion. While both strategies were comparable interms of objective task completion time and subjective time and effort estimations, participants were overwhelmingly biasedagainst choosing the organization strategy. Implications for the current study will be discussed.

Emotion in Deceptive Language

Deception involves emotions of fear and guilt. These negative emotions are expressed in language in terms ofpsychological distance from the deception object. The psychological distance and emotional experience reflect an attemptto control the negative mental representation. More especifically emotional distance is represented in deceptive language bymeans of cues of reference, verb tense and detail avoidance. Then, hints of emotions of fear and guilt should be displayedin language.The present work analyses emotional language cues for deception detection by means of Machine Learning(ML)techniques and Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC). Results show that Support Vector Machines (SVM) best representsthe discrimination between true and false information (up to 74.15 % of accuracy rates) based only on the effect of emotion indeceptive speech.