Open Access Policy Deposits
Parent: Department of Linguistics
eScholarship stats: History by Item for January through April, 2025
Item | Title | Total requests | 2025-04 | 2025-03 | 2025-02 | 2025-01 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
05b2s4wg | The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology | 186 | 47 | 53 | 48 | 38 |
7f26m713 | Challenges for a theory of islands: A broader perspective on Ambridge, Pine, and Lieven | 108 | 11 | 15 | 48 | 34 |
04r5q6qn | WPP, No. 105: Linguistic Voice Quality | 80 | 15 | 17 | 27 | 21 |
0n76t3hn | WPP, No.111: Glottal articulations of phonation contrasts and their acoustic and perceptual consequences | 78 | 22 | 19 | 20 | 17 |
3fq7x9mb | Vocal fold vibratory patterns in tense versus lax phonation contrasts | 68 | 31 | 9 | 18 | 10 |
9pn2b9qm | Measures of the glottal source spectrum. | 63 | 11 | 15 | 23 | 14 |
9xx930j1 | WPP, No. 108: Phonation Contrasts Across Languages | 59 | 12 | 10 | 16 | 21 |
1xn120qk | When is a Verb not a Verb? | 55 | 12 | 12 | 21 | 10 |
88x529wr | A comparison of informal and formal acceptability judgments using a random sample from Linguistic Inquiry 2001–2010 | 55 | 18 | 12 | 11 | 14 |
4mt0s60w | Measuring vocal quality with speech synthesis. | 54 | 14 | 13 | 18 | 9 |
60v8v517 | Dependent-case assignment could be AGREE | 53 | 17 | 13 | 15 | 8 |
2mw9c40q | Non-literal language processing is jointly supported by the language and theory of mind networks: Evidence from a novel meta-analytic fMRI approach. | 51 | 16 | 19 | 12 | 4 |
4qs31528 | WPP, No. 108: Perception of pitch location within a speaker’s own range: fundamental frequency, voice quality and speaker sex | 50 | 19 | 9 | 12 | 10 |
8dr06327 | Perception of aperiodicity in pathological voice | 50 | 12 | 13 | 13 | 12 |
92d624qq | “Narten formations” versus “Narten roots” | 50 | 10 | 9 | 20 | 11 |
29s5h9w3 | Information conveyed by voice qualitya) | 45 | 7 | 11 | 17 | 10 |
52g5f2dq | More on (the Lack of) Reconstruction in English Tough-Constructions | 43 | 9 | 12 | 16 | 6 |
6mt3d01x | Semantic projection recovers rich human knowledge of multiple object features from word embeddings. | 43 | 14 | 12 | 12 | 5 |
0xd5n4gk | High-toned [il] in Korean: Phonetics, intonational phonology, and sound change | 42 | 13 | 4 | 11 | 14 |
5dp6b6s4 | Not all reconstruction effects are syntactic | 42 | 7 | 15 | 13 | 7 |
9n26b70j | Information Structure Preferences in Focus-Sensitive Ellipsis: How Defaults Persist | 42 | 11 | 9 | 12 | 10 |
7x39t5hg | Three levels of the symbolosphere | 41 | 13 | 13 | 12 | 3 |
90d6532f | WPP, No. 104: Intonational Phonology of Seoul Korean Revisited | 41 | 9 | 12 | 10 | 10 |
0n100842 | Grammar and the use of data | 40 | 5 | 11 | 14 | 10 |
3987s5jj | Differential Tracking of Linguistic vs. Mental State Content in Naturalistic Stimuli by Language and Theory of Mind (ToM) Brain Networks. | 40 | 9 | 15 | 11 | 5 |
8m31s5n6 | Functional characterization of the language network of polyglots and hyperpolyglots with precision fMRI. | 40 | 19 | 21 | ||
0hc673ms | Synchronic and diachronic microvariation in English do | 36 | 11 | 9 | 9 | 7 |
46v4j8dt | Probabilistic atlas for the language network based on precision fMRI data from >800 individuals | 36 | 2 | 21 | 4 | 9 |
8rv9t589 | WPP, No. 106: Effects of Initial Position versus Prominence in English | 36 | 14 | 7 | 7 | 8 |
9w85x4mc | Validity of rating scale measures of voice quality. | 36 | 8 | 7 | 14 | 7 |
0r69t4tr | WPP, No. 103: Linguistic Phonetics in the UCLA Phonetics Lab | 35 | 9 | 13 | 9 | 4 |
1gs6h5k7 | WPP, No. 108: The acoustic consequences of phonation and tone interactions in Jalapa Mazatec | 34 | 4 | 12 | 12 | 6 |
5bc7k7xd | WPP, No.110: Perception of spectral slopes and tone identification in White Hmong | 34 | 12 | 8 | 6 | 8 |
5sv727q6 | WPP, No. 103: Phonetic Encoding of Prosodic Structure | 33 | 16 | 3 | 10 | 4 |
5tj8f292 | Against some approaches to long-distance agreement without AGREE | 33 | 7 | 6 | 10 | 10 |
4kz9d7v0 | No evidence for differences among language regions in their temporal receptive windows | 32 | 5 | 8 | 12 | 7 |
50d070bw | Degree modification in natural language | 32 | 15 | 6 | 6 | 5 |
61f416jp | fMRI reveals language-specific predictive coding during naturalistic sentence comprehension | 32 | 2 | 13 | 11 | 6 |
69r4g5xd | Parameterizing passive participle movement | 32 | 7 | 4 | 14 | 7 |
6tg5z1jk | The polysemy of measurement | 32 | 12 | 4 | 14 | 2 |
8k4092h0 | Sources of listener disagreement in voice quality assessment. | 32 | 6 | 5 | 14 | 7 |
3xk61586 | Cue-shifting between acoustic cues: Evidence for directional asymmetry | 31 | 11 | 6 | 9 | 5 |
9mt1x9z7 | A meta-analytic review of morphological priming in Semitic languages | 31 | 5 | 10 | 7 | 9 |
1640t82j | Vocal Fundamental Frequency and Sound Pressure Level in Charismatic Speech: A Cross-Gender and -Language Study. | 30 | 7 | 7 | 10 | 6 |
1gh6x943 | WPP, No. 108: Comparison of speaking fundamental frequency in English and Mandarin | 30 | 12 | 2 | 10 | 6 |
0s9963dr | Incremental Language Comprehension Difficulty Predicts Activity in the Language Network but Not the Multiple Demand Network | 29 | 7 | 6 | 6 | 10 |
4gk6008p | WPP, No. 105: Optical Cues to the Visual Perception of Lexical and Phrasal Stress in English | 29 | 3 | 12 | 8 | 6 |
7qw6m187 | Watching videos of a drawing hand improves students understanding of the normal probability distribution. | 29 | 6 | 14 | 9 | |
98j5s1r7 | Keep it local (and final): Remnant preferences in “let alone” ellipsis | 29 | 7 | 5 | 11 | 6 |
8hb954bq | Serbo-Croatian clitic placement: An argument for prosodic movement | 28 | 8 | 6 | 6 | 8 |
Note: Due to the evolving nature of web traffic, the data presented here should be considered approximate and subject to revision. Learn more.